Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Opposite Cannabis-Cognition Associations in Psychotic Patients Depending on Family History

  • Ana González-Pinto ,

    anamaria.gonzalez-pintoarrillaga@osakidetza.net

    Affiliations Centre for Biomedical Research in the Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain, Department of Psychiatry, Araba University Hospital, University of the Basque Country, Vitoria, Spain

  • Itxaso González-Ortega,

    Affiliations Centre for Biomedical Research in the Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain, Department of Psychiatry, Araba University Hospital, University of the Basque Country, Vitoria, Spain

  • Susana Alberich,

    Affiliations Centre for Biomedical Research in the Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain, Department of Psychiatry, Araba University Hospital, University of the Basque Country, Vitoria, Spain

  • Sonia Ruiz de Azúa,

    Affiliations Centre for Biomedical Research in the Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain, Department of Psychiatry, Araba University Hospital, University of the Basque Country, Vitoria, Spain

  • Miguel Bernardo,

    Affiliations Centre for Biomedical Research in the Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain, Barcelona Clinic Schizophrenia Unit, Neuroscience Institute, Hospital Clinic of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain, Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, University of Barcelona. Institut d'Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Barcelona, Spain

  • Miquel Bioque,

    Affiliations Centre for Biomedical Research in the Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain, Barcelona Clinic Schizophrenia Unit, Neuroscience Institute, Hospital Clinic of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain

  • Bibiana Cabrera,

    Affiliations Centre for Biomedical Research in the Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain, Barcelona Clinic Schizophrenia Unit, Neuroscience Institute, Hospital Clinic of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain

  • Iluminada Corripio,

    Affiliations Centre for Biomedical Research in the Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain, Department of Psychiatry, Institut d'Investigació Biomèdica-Sant Pau (IIB-SANT PAU), Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona (UAB), Barcelona, Spain

  • Celso Arango,

    Affiliations Centre for Biomedical Research in the Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Department. Gregorio Marañón General University Hospital. School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, IiSGM, Madrid, Spain

  • Antonio Lobo,

    Affiliations Centre for Biomedical Research in the Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain, Aragon Institute for Health Sciences (IIS Aragón), Zaragoza, Spain

  • Ana M. Sánchez-Torres,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Navarre Hospital Complex, Pamplona, Spain. IdiSNA, Navarre Institute for Health Research, Pamplona, Spain

  • Manuel J. Cuesta,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Navarre Hospital Complex, Pamplona, Spain. IdiSNA, Navarre Institute for Health Research, Pamplona, Spain

  • the PEPs Group

    Membership of the PEPs Group is listed in the Acknowledgments.

Abstract

The objective of this study is to investigate cognitive performance in a first-episode psychosis sample, when stratifying the interaction by cannabis use and familial or non-familial psychosis. Hierarchical-regression models were used to analyse this association in a sample of 268 first-episode psychosis patients and 237 controls. We found that cannabis use was associated with worse working memory, regardless of family history. However, cannabis use was clearly associated with worse cognitive performance in patients with no family history of psychosis, in cognitive domains including verbal memory, executive function and global cognitive index, whereas cannabis users with a family history of psychosis performed better in these domains. The main finding of the study is that there is an interaction between cannabis use and a family history of psychosis in the areas of verbal memory, executive function and global cognition: that is, cannabis use is associated with a better performance in patients with a family history of psychosis and a worse performance in those with no family history of psychosis. In order to confirm this hypothesis, future research should explore the actual expression of the endocannabinoid system in patients with and without a family history of psychosis.

Introduction

Cannabis is the most commonly used illicit drug in psychosis [1]. Moreover, it is a controversial drug, with detractors and supporters, which has a high social impact. There is strong evidence that long-term cannabis use is associated with decreased functionality in psychosis [2]. However, recent meta-analyses have also found associations between a history of cannabis use and improved cognition in psychosis [3,4]. Furthermore, the study by Yücel et al. [4] found that patients with first-episode psychosis (FEP) and earlier cannabis use had better cognitive performance than patients with later onset of use. There are also numerous contrasting findings in relation to neurobiological research on cannabis and cognition in psychosis. Whereas some studies have revealed cognitive impairment [510] and deficits in grey matter, lateral ventricle enlargement, and brain abnormalities [1113] in FEP cannabis users, other authors have suggested that FEP related to cannabis use is associated with better cognitive functioning and fewer brain abnormalities [4,1416]. In addition to these controversial results, a deleterious effect of cannabis on cognition has been demonstrated in both animal models [17] and healthy subjects [18]. However, an increased vulnerability to the deleterious effect of cannabis on cognition has been found in patients with schizophrenia as compared to healthy controls, specifically in the memory and learning domains [9]. In contrast, other authors have reported that FEP patients who used cannabis have lower cognitive impairment relative to healthy controls in relation to visual memory, working memory, planning and reasoning domains [6]. Moreover, shared effects of genetics and family environment have been suggested as influential in the development of brain structure and cannabis use. Pagliaccio et al. [6] have reported that cannabis exposure is associated with small subcortical structures in a large sample of normal twins/siblings discordant for cannabis abuse. However, not only were these variations within the normal range, they were also primarily attributed to familial factors, rather than to a direct neurotoxic effect [6]. In respect of cognitive impairment, psychotic patients with a family history (FH) of psychosis had previously shown worse cognition than patients with no FH of psychosis [1924].

As the data regarding the relation between cannabis use and cognition are highly controversial, and cognitive deficits are core symptoms of schizophrenia, it was considered possible that not all cases of psychosis had the same cognitive associations with cannabis use. Therefore, we focused on FH of psychosis, and split the sample into two different groups according to this variable: people with FEP and with or without FH of psychosis.

We conducted a study to investigate the associations between cannabis use and cognition in patients with psychosis, distinguishing the interactions by having a familial vs. no familial history of psychosis. The specific aims of the study were: 1) to compare cognition between patients with a family history of psychosis (FH+) and patients with no family history of psychosis (FH-) as regards cannabis use; 2) to determine the independent effect of cannabis use and the FH of psychosis on cognition and the interaction between these two factors; and 3) to analyse the associations between cannabis use and cognition in healthy controls and to compare them with those in FEP patients.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

This work was part of the “Phenotype-genotype and environmental interaction: application of a predictive model in first psychotic episodes” study (PEPs study, from its acronym in Spanish) [25]. Patients were matched with healthy controls by age (±10%), sex and parental socio-economic level, and assessed using the Hollingshead-Redlich index of social position [26] (±1 level).

Patients and controls were required to be fluent in Spanish and to provide informed consent. The clinicians who contributed to recruitment of the sample assessed the capacity of patients to provide informed consent via a clinical interview. In the cases of patients not able to provide consent, or of patients who were minors, a legally authorized representative gave consent on behalf of the participants. Moreover, patients were required to have had psychotic symptoms of less than 12 months’ duration.

Exclusion criteria for patients and controls were: mental retardation according to the DSM-IV [27] (including both an IQ below 70, as determined using the test described in Table 1, and poor functionality), a history of head trauma with loss of consciousness, and organic illness with mental repercussions. Controls were also excluded if they had suffered, or were suffering, from psychotic disease, were experiencing major depression according to the DSM-IV [28,29], or had a first-degree family member with a history of psychotic disease.

The study was approved by the following clinical research ethics committees: Araba University Hospital, Hospital Clinic of Barcelona, Gregorio Marañon General University Hospital, Navarre Hospital Complex, and the centres included in the PEPs Group.

Initially, 335 FEP patients and 253 healthy controls were included in the study. Of these subjects, 69 (53 patients and 16 controls) were excluded for not having completed seven or more neuropsychological tests. Moreover, 14 patients were excluded because of a lack of data on FH of psychosis (first- or second-degree). As a result, the final study sample consisted of 268 patients and 237 controls. Patients were distributed into two groups, depending on whether or not they had an FH of psychosis. A total of 88 patients were FH+, whereas 180 were FH-. For cognitive comparison, each group was split into two subgroups: one group of cannabis users (FH- = 73; FH+ = 34; controls = 37) and one group of non-users (FH- = 107; FH+ = 54; controls = 200).

Data collection

Socio-demographic and clinical data were collected at baseline and cognitive assessment was performed at two months follow-up, in order to ensure the psychopathologic stability of patients. The assessment protocol is fully discussed in the paper by Bernardo et al. [25].

The socio-demographic variables included were sex, age, civil (marital) status, educational background, parental socio-economic status, and occupation.

Adult patients were diagnosed using the SCID-I and II [28], whereas the K-SADS-PL [29] was used for patients aged below 18 years, in accordance with the DSM-IV criteria. Patients were classified into three groups according to diagnosis: 1) those with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders, including schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorders and schizophreniform disorders; 2) those with affective-spectrum disorders, such as bipolar disorder, manic and depressive episodes; and 3) those with other psychoses, such as substance-induced psychosis, brief psychotic disorders, and psychoses not otherwise specified.

Pharmacological treatment was also recollected at baseline. Antipsychotic daily doses were converted to chlorpromazine equivalents [30].

Use of cannabis and other substances (including alcohol and tobacco) was assessed in patients and healthy controls using the European Adaptation of a Multidimensional Assessment Instrument for Drug and Alcohol Dependence (EuropASI), and was categorized into four groups (no use, use, abuse and dependence) according to EuropASI scores [31]. Age of first use was also recorded.

The Family History of Psychosis-Related Disorder (FHPRD) was assessed using a protocol that evaluated first- and second-degree family history of mental illnesses, according to DSM-IV criteria. In the case of patients under 18 years of age, at least one adult relative was interviewed for each participant. The FHPRD was positive when one or several members of the family (first- or second-degree) presented a diagnosis of psychotic disorder with delusions or hallucinations requiring psychiatric treatment.

Cognitive performance was evaluated using the following cognitive domains: processing speed, attention, verbal memory, working memory, executive function and estimated premorbid IQ. A Cognitive Global Index (CGI) was calculated by averaging the scores of the cognitive domains, except the estimated premorbid IQ, which was used as an adjustment variable in the statistical analyses. Development of these domains is reported in the papers by Bernardo et al. [25] and Cuesta et al. [32] (Table 1).

Statistical Analyses

Differences in socio-demographic variables between FH+ and FH- patients and healthy controls were analysed using one-way ANOVA tests for continuous variables and the Chi-Squared test for categorical variables. For those significant differences between groups, a post-hoc analysis was performed.

With regard to cognitive measures, Z-scores were derived, based on the means and standard deviations of the control group to standardize the different measurement scales used in the neuropsychological tests. Composite scores for cognitive domains represented by more than one measure were calculated by taking the mean of all the Z-scores included in each cognitive domain. Cronbach’s alpha was calculated to assess the internal consistency of the composite scores, and gave a value of 0.84 for the processing speed domain, 0.82 for verbal memory, 0.73 for working memory and 0.76 for executive function. Since all coefficients indicate moderate reliability, we decided to use these domains to analyse the relationship between cannabis use and cognition.

One-way ANOVA tests were performed to assess differences in patient cognition between groups of cannabis users (no use, use, abuse and dependence) with/without FH of psychosis. Subsequently, Bonferroni post-hoc tests were used to determine the groups for which differences in ANOVA tests were found. Differences between controls with/without cannabis use at baseline were compared using Student’s t-test for independent samples.

Hierarchical-regression models were developed to assess the association between FH of psychosis, cannabis use and cognition. Cognitive domains were taken as dependent variables in these models. Cannabis use at baseline (presence/absence) and the subject variable (FH+ patient, FH- patient, controls) were introduced as independent variables. In addition, the interaction between cannabis use and subject variable was also considered in order to assess the combined effect of these variables on cognition. We identified the following variables as potential confounders: sex, age, occupation, educational level, civil status, alcohol and tobacco consumption, and IQ, and, after verifying the independence of significant potential confounders (occupation, educational level, alcohol, tobacco consumption and estimated premorbid IQ), the models were finally adjusted for estimated premorbid IQ and alcohol consumption.

To facilitate interpretation and comparison of groups in the hierarchical-regression models developed to analyse the cognitive domains, FH- patients were used as the reference group, while, for cannabis use at baseline, non-user patients was the reference category.

All statistical analyses were performed using the R statistical package, version 3.1.2 [42].

Results

Baseline characteristics

The baseline characteristics for controls and FH+ and FH- patients are summarized in Table 2. Patients (FH+ and FH-) were less likely to have a university education (P<0.001) and were more likely to be tobacco (P<0.001) and cannabis users than were controls (P<0.001). In contrast, controls were more commonly active workers (46% vs. 19.5% and 12.8%, respectively, P<0.001) and alcohol users (65% vs. 48.2% and 47.9%, respectively, P<0.001). Statistically significant differences were also found between controls and patients in respect of estimated premorbid IQ (P<0.001), with FH+ patients having the lowest scores and close to 16 points below that for controls. In the post-hoc analysis, all the significant differences were between patients (FH+ and FH-) and controls. Moreover, a comparison of estimated premorbid IQ between cannabis users and non-users showed that the latter group had a significantly higher IQ than patients who were cannabis users. On analysis, this relationship was only found to be significant in the FH- group. Finally, antipsychotic doses for both groups were compared, and there were no significant differences.

thumbnail
Table 2. Baseline characteristics of controls and FH+ and FH- patients.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160949.t002

Neuropsychological results

S1 Table shows the Z-scores for the cognitive domains for each group, depending on cannabis use and FH of psychosis.

Statistically significant associations were found in all cognitive domains, except for Attention in regression models. The results are summarized in Table 3 and Fig 1. Fig 2 shows the interactions between subjects and cannabis use in the domains in which a statistically significant interaction was found.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Graphical representation of the regression models.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160949.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Statistically significant interactions between cannabis use and subject type.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160949.g002

thumbnail
Table 3. Hierarchical-regression models between subject type, cannabis use and cognition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160949.t003

Controls had better scores than FH- patients for the processing-speed domain, while FH+ patients were the worst group, with the lowest scores. Cannabis use had no impact on any group.

A significant interaction between cannabis use and the subject-type variable was obtained for the verbal memory domain. As a consequence, whereas controls attained the best values in this domain, the group of FH+ cannabis users had the best estimated score amongst patients, while FH+ non cannabis users had the worst score (Fig 2).

Both cannabis use and FH were significantly related to inferior working memory, although their interaction was not significantly associated with this domain. As a consequence, cannabis users (regardless of whether they were patients or controls) had lower scores in this domain than did non-users. The same occurred with FH+ patients, who had worse scores than FH- ones (regardless of whether or not cannabis was used) (Fig 3).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Association between cannabis use and working memory.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160949.g003

With regard to executive function, a statistically significant interaction with cannabis use was obtained in FH+/FH- patients (Table 3). The group with the best estimated scores in this domain was that of FH- non-cannabis users, whereas FH+ non-user patients had the worst estimated executive function. The control group had the highest scores, irrespective of cannabis consumption (Fig 2).

Finally, both cannabis use and the subject-type variable interacted significantly in the Global Cognitive Index (GCI) domain (Table 3). Indeed, as was the case with the executive-function domain, FH- patients who did not use cannabis were the patient group with the best estimated score in the GCI, while FH+ non-cannabis users had the worst estimated score in this domain. Controls (using cannabis or not) gave the highest scores in the GCI (Fig 2).

Discussion

The two main findings of this study are that cannabis use is associated with inferior working memory in FEP patients, regardless of having a positive FH for a psychosis-related disorder, and that cannabis is clearly associated with inferior cognitive performance in FH- patients in relation to the verbal-memory, executive-function, and GCI domains, while FH+ cannabis users showed better cognition in these domains. These findings are relevant and have translational value, because treatment of cannabis use requires different approaches, depending on FH of psychosis. The association found between cannabis use and cognitive deficits in these patients varied depending on FH. These results clarify some of the controversies concerning the association between cannabis use and cognitive deficits, and should be taken into consideration for the prevention and treatment of these patients.

As shown in Table 3, cannabis users (patients and controls) had worse working memory and executive function to non-users, when FH was not considered. Acute administration of THC causes more deleterious cognitive effects in patients with psychosis than in healthy control subjects [9]. Some studies have found cognitive impairment in FEP cannabis-user patients compared with control subjects [9] and cognitive deficits in processing speed and verbal memory [43], along with brain abnormalities [1113]. In contrast, other authors have reported that psychosis patients who use cannabis have better cognitive functioning in some domains than do non-cannabis users [4,1416], with fewer brain abnormalities and less attention and executive impairment than patients who have never used cannabis [44].

These conflicting findings are probably mediated by different moderators. Yücel et al. [4] considered that there is probably a subgroup of cannabis users with psychosis who have better premorbid adjustment and cognitive performance. Meijer et al. [43] also found that current cannabis use is associated with poorer cognitive performance, but that lifetime cannabis users may have a higher cognitive potential. This does not mean that cannabis has a beneficial effect on cognition; on the contrary, it worsens cognitive performance, although some of these individuals had better premorbid capacities. Our study was performed on FEP, and all patients were included at the same time, consequently limiting the possible moderating effect of time.

According to Van Winkel et al. [10], the deleterious effect of cannabis use on cognition in patients with psychosis may be moderated by the AKT1 gene. These authors showed that cannabis-user patients who carried the CC variant in the AKT1 gene performed worse in attention tests than patients with this genotype who did not use cannabis, whereas cannabis users with the TT variant in AKT1 performed as well as, or better than, cannabis non-users [45]. We have not attempted to identify genetic differences amongst our patients in this study.

The different interaction of cannabis use depending on the FH of psychosis has not been demonstrated previously. Although we cannot state causal relationships, this interaction is sufficiently large as to be unlikely to be due to chance. This suggests that the FH-cannabis interaction affects more complex cognitive functions (for example, executive function), which depend on multiple cognitive processes, more than it does basic functions (for example, attention). These data support previous studies on cognition and cannabis use in patients with schizophrenia [7,8]. However, FH+ cannabis users show better functioning in complex cognitive domains, as reported in recent meta-analyses [4]. This finding must be interpreted with caution because, considering the data as a whole, cannabis use is associated with worse working memory regardless of having FH of psychosis or not. Our findings do not support the idea of better cognitive performance in patients with cannabis use; rather, they suggest an FH of psychosis as a possible factor contributing to the controversial results found in the literature.

A possible explanation for the differential effects of cannabis depending on FH in patients could be a distinct effect of cannabis compounds on the central nervous system of FH+ patients. Although the endocannabinoid system is involved in the development of cognitive deficits associated with cannabis use and schizophrenia [46], it is well known that cannabidiol (CBD) and tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) have opposing effects in various brain structures involved in psychiatric disorders, notably the striatum, cingulate, prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and amygdala. CBD might protect neurons against the possible neurotoxic effects of THC, meaning that it could inhibit the cognitive and psychotomimetic effects of THC. Moreover, CBD has been found to protect users against both psychotic symptoms and memory impairment [47]. One possible explanation for our findings is that the protective role of CBD is expressed better in FH+ patients than in FH- ones. Future research could explore the real expression of the endocannabinoid system in FH+ and FH- patients to confirm this hypothesis. Our findings suggest that FH is an important factor to consider when studying the effects of cannabis on cognition. Our results support some differences in cognitive deficits when taking into account cannabis use and FH. Cannabis and FH are separately associated with poorer cognitive functioning, while their interaction has no significant effects on cognition and impacts differently depending on whether FH is present or not.

When comparing cognition without considering cannabis use, FH+ patients had worse CGI, processing speed, verbal and working memory, and executive function than FH- patients. Previous studies have shown that FH+ patients have greater cognitive deficits [1924]. Moreover, some research has shown that FH+ patients and patients with a high clinical risk of psychosis have cognitive deficits, and that co-occurring attenuated symptoms and genetic risk are related to more severe cognitive deficits [20,48]. Bora et al. [20] suggested that cognitive impairment might predate the onset of FEP and could be genetically transmitted in some patients. In fact, there is evidence of cognitive impairment in relatives of patients with psychosis [4955]. In a study performed by Gur et al. [53], unaffected offspring, monozygotic and dizygotic co-twins, and other relatives, had deficits in episodic memory, working memory, and attention. Cognitive deficits have also been found in siblings when compared to healthy control subjects [56,57] and in the offspring of patients with schizophrenia [23,53]. Other studies that have compared IQ in patients with and without FH have also found that both patients and their relatives had lower global IQ than healthy controls [19,2123], while patients with no affected relatives had higher estimated premorbid IQ [58]. A recent study conducted by Derks et al. [59] concluded that FH+ patients showed more positive symptoms than FH- patients, but no differences were found as regards IQ, cannabis abuse, or age at onset. In our sample of FEP patients, patients with and without FH also had a poorer estimated premorbid IQ than healthy controls; there were no differences between patient groups.

This study has several strengths. First, it is a multi-centre study that includes a large sample of patients and controls recruited in multiple Spanish psychiatric admission centres for acute psychosis, meaning that generalizability of the results is ensured. Second, the study comprised child and adolescent patients, in addition to adults. This wide age window allows the collection of a more representative sample, with an average age (23.63±5.9) lower than that for other studies with large FEP cohorts (OPUS trial: 26.6±6.4; EUFEST trial: 26±5.6), which did not include child and adolescent patients [60,61]. Third, unlike other FEP studies, such as EUFEST [62,63], the neuropsychological battery used was extensive and covered the areas proposed by the NIMH-MATRICS consensus (except visual memory) [64,65].

Nevertheless, this study has some limitations that must be taken into account when interpreting the findings. These limitations include the naturalistic setting of the study, from which causal inferences cannot be drawn. A further limitation is that it would be necessary to assess patients during follow-up to ensure longitudinal stability. Although one of the inclusion criteria was that the control group did not have FH, it would be interesting to consider an FH+ control group. It has also been assumed that various combinations of FH carry equivalent genetic loadings. However, patients could have different genetic loadings (one vs. multiple family members affected). Another limitation is that type of cannabis use was not measured. Finally, future studies should analyse cognition in relatives of patients, in addition to the relationship between cognition, FH of psychosis, and extent of cannabis use.

Although further studies are required to improve the understanding of the influence of FH on the association between cannabis use and cognition in FEP patients, our findings show that cannabis use is associated with better performance in patients with an FH of psychosis and a worse performance in those with no FH of psychosis in the verbal-memory, executive-function and global-cognition domains.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. Cognitive domain scores depending on having family history and on cannabis use at baseline.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160949.s001

(DOC)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the following institutions: the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (Ref. ISCIII 2009–2011:PI 080208), the Instituto de Salud Carlos III-European Fund for Regional Development (PI10/01430, PI10/01746, PI11/01977, PI11/02708, 2011/1064, PI12/02077, PI13/02252, PI13/00451 and PI14/01900, PI11/02831), the Centre for Biomedical Research in the Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), the Health Department of the Government of Navarre (11/101 and 87/2014), the Government of Catalonia, Secretaria d’Universitats i Recerca del Departament d’Economia i Coneixement (2014SGR441), the Contract FI-DGR-2013 from the Agència de Gestió d’Ajuts Universitaris i de Recerca, AGAUR (2015 FI_B2 00100 to GM), the Esther Koplowitz Centre (Barcelona), local grants from the Department of Education, Linguistic Policy and Culture of the Basque Country Government, the Basque Foundation for Health Innovation and Research (BIOEF), Madrid Regional Government (S2010/BMD-2422 AGES), European Union Structural Funds and European Union Seventh Framework Program under grant agreements FP7-HEALTH-2009-2.2.1-2-241909 (Project EU-GEI), FP7-HEALTH-2009-2.2.1-3-242114 (Project OPTiMISE), FP7- HEALTH-2013-2.2.1-2-603196 (Project PSYSCAN) and FP7-HEALTH-2013-2.2.1-2-602478 (Project METSY); Alicia Koplowitz Foundation, Mutua Madrileña Foundation and the University of the Basque Country (GIC10/80, GIC12/84). The psychiatric research department at Araba University Hospital is supported by the Stanley Research Foundation (03-RC-003).

We also would like to thank the authors of the PEPs group who participated in the development of this manuscript, namely, Amaia Ugarte1,2, Miryam Fernández1,2, Gisela Mezquida3, Ana Meseguer3, Eduard Vieta1,12, Carla Torrent1,12, Fernando Contreras1,11, Auria Albacete1,11, Anna Alonso-Solís1,5, Mireia Rabella5, Lucía Moreno-Izco9, Judith Usall1,10, Anna Butjosa1,10, Ángela Ibáñez1,13, Julio Bobes1,14, Susana Al-Halabí1,14,15, Mª Teresa Zapata-Usábel7,8,16, Isabel Laporta-Herrero7,16, Vicent Balanzá-Martínez1,17, Daniel Bergé1,18, Anna Mané1,18, Roberto Rodriguez-Jimenez1,19, Isabel Morales-Muñoz1,19, Miguel Gutierrez1,20, Arantzazu Zabala1,20, Salvador Sarró1,21, Ramón Landin-Romero1,21, Elena de la Serna, Inmaculada Baeza1,22, Mara Parellada1,6, Ana Espliego6, Julio San Juan1,23 and Eduardo Aguilar.1,23

PEPs Group Affiliations

10 Research Unit, Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu, Universitat de Barcelona (UB), Sant Boi de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain.

11 Psychiatry Department, Bellvitge University Hospital-IDIBELL, Barcelona, Spain; Department of Clinical Sciences, School of Medicine, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain.

12 Bipolar Disorders Unit, Hospital Clinic, University of Barcelona, IDIBAPS, Barcelona, Spain.

13 Psychiatry Department, Ramón y Cajal Health Research Institute (IRyCIS), Ramón y Cajal Hospital, University of Alcalá, Madrid, Spain.14 Psychiatry Department, University of Oviedo, Oviedo, Spain.

15 Institute of Neurosciences of the Principality of Asturias, INEUROPA, Oviedo, Spain.

16 Psychiatry Department, University Clinical Hospital, Zaragoza, Spain.

17 Teaching Unit of Psychiatry, Department of Medicine, School of Medicine, La Fe University and Polytechnic Hospital, University of Valencia, ISNPR, Valencia, Spain.

18 Hospital del Mar Medical Research Institute (IMIM), Barcelona, Spain.

19 12 de Octubre Hospital Research Institute (i+12), Madrid, Spain.

20 University of the Basque Country, Bilbao, Spain.

21 FIDMAG Hermanas Hospitalarias Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain.

22 Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Service, Hospital Clinic of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain.

23 Clinic Hospital (INCLIVA), Valencia, Spain.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: AGP M. Bernardo MJC.
  2. Formal analysis: SA.
  3. Investigation: AGP IGO SA SRA M. Bernardo M. Bioque BC IC CA AL AMS MJC PEPG.
  4. Methodology: AGP SA MJC M. Bernardo.
  5. Supervision: AGP CA MJC M. Bernardo.
  6. Writing - original draft: AGP IGO SA.
  7. Writing - review & editing: AGP IGO SA SRA M. Bernardo M. Bioque BC IC CA AL AMS MJC.

References

  1. 1. González-Pinto A, Alberich S, Barbeito S, Alonso M, Vieta E, Martínez-Arán A, et al. Different profile of substance abuse in relation to predominant polarity in bipolar disorder: the Vitoria long-term follow-up study. J Affective Disorders. 2010; 124: 250–255.
  2. 2. González-Pinto A, Alberich S, Barbeito S, Gutierrez M, Vega P, Ibáñez B, et al. Cannabis and first-episode psychosis: different long-term outcomes depending on continued or discontinued use. Schizophr Bull. 2011; 37: 631–639. pmid:19915168
  3. 3. Rabin RA, Zakzanis KK, George TP. The effects of cannabis use on neurocognition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2011; 128: 111–116. pmid:21420282
  4. 4. Yücel M, Bora E, Lubman DI, Solowij N, Brewer WJ, Cotton SM, et al. The impact of cannabis use on cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of existing findings and new data in a first-episode sample. Schizophr Bull. 2012; 38: 316–330. pmid:20660494
  5. 5. Ringen PA, Vaskinn A, Sundet K, Engh JA, Jonsdottir H, Simonsen C, et al. Opposite relationships between cannabis use and neurocognitive functioning in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2010; 40: 1337–1347. pmid:19891810
  6. 6. Pagliaccio D, Barch DM, Bogdan R, Wood PK, Lynskey MT, Heath AC, et al. Shared predisposition in the association between cannabis use and subcortical brain structure. AMA Psychiatry. 2015; 72: 994–1001.
  7. 7. Smith MJ, Cobia DJ, Wang L, Alpert KI, Cronenwett WJ, Goldman MB, et al. Cannabis-related working memory deficits and associated subcortical morphological differences in healthy individuals and schizophrenia subjects. Schizophr Bull. 2014; 40: 287–299. pmid:24342821
  8. 8. Solowij N, Walterfang M, Lubman DI, Whittle S, Lorenzetti V, Styner M, et al. Alteration to hippocampal shape in cannabis users with and without schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2013; 143: 179–184. pmid:23201308
  9. 9. D'Souza DC, Abi-Saab WM, Madonick S, Forselius-Bielen K, Doersch A, Braley G, et al. Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol effects in schizophrenia: implications for cognition, psychosis, and addiction. Biol Psychiatry. 2005; 57: 594–608. pmid:15780846
  10. 10. Van Winkel R, van Beveren NJM, Simons C. The genetic risk and outcome of psychosis (GROUP) investigators: AKT1 moderation of cannabis-induced cognitive alterations in psychotic disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011; 36: 2529–2537. pmid:21775978
  11. 11. Rapp C, Bugra H, Riecher-Rössler A, Tamagni C, Borgwardt S. Effects of cannabis use on human brain structure in psychosis: a systematic review combining in vivo structural neuroimaging and post mortem studies. Curr Pharm Des. 2012; 18: 5070–5080. pmid:22716152
  12. 12. Rais M, Cahn W, Van Haren N, Schnack H, Caspers E, Hulshoff Pol H, et al. Excessive brain volume loss over time in cannabis-using first-episode schizophrenia patients. Am J Psychiatry. 2008; 165: 490–496. pmid:18281413
  13. 13. Rais M, van Haren NE, Cahn W, Schnack HG, Lepage C, Collins L, et al. Cannabis use and progressive cortical thickness loss in areas rich in CB1 receptors during the first five years of schizophrenia. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol.2010; 20: 855–65. pmid:20863671
  14. 14. Rodríguez-Sánchez JM, Ayesa-Arriola R, Mata I, Moreno-Call T, Perez-Iglesias R, González-Blanch C, et al. Cannabis use and cognitive functioning in first-episode schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Res. 2010; 124: 142–151. pmid:20826079
  15. 15. Arnold C, Allott K, Farhall J, Killackey E, Cotton S. Neurocognitive and social cognitive predictors of cannabis use in first-episode psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2015; 168: 231–237. pmid:26260081
  16. 16. Ferraro L, Russo M, O'Connor J, Wiffen BD, Falcone MA, Sideli L, et al. Cannabis users have higher premorbid IQ than other patients with first onset psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2013; 150: 129–135. pmid:23958486
  17. 17. Zanettini C, Panlilio VL, Aliczki M, Goldberg SR, Haller J, Yasar S. Effects of endocannabinoid system modulation on cognitive and emotional behavior. Front Behav Neurosci. 2011; 5: 57. pmid:21949506
  18. 18. Meier MH, Caspi A, Ambler A, Harrington H, Houts R, Keefe RS, et al. Persistent cannabis users show neuropsychological decline from childhood to midlife. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012; 109: 2657–2664.
  19. 19. Agnew-Blais J, Seidman LJ. Neurocognition in youth and young adults under age 30 at familial risk for schizophrenia: a quantitative and qualitative review. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2013; 18: 44–82. pmid:22998599
  20. 20. Bora E, Lin A, Wood SJ, Yung AR, McGorry PD, Pantelis C. Cognitive deficits in youth with familial and clinical high risk to psychosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2014; 130: 1–15. pmid:24611632
  21. 21. Eack SM, Mermon DE, Montrose DM, Miewald J, Gur RE, Gur RC, et al. Social cognition deficits among individuals at familial high risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2010; 36: 1081–1088. pmid:19366983
  22. 22. Fis NP, Cetin FC, Erturk M, Erdogan E, Dedeoglu C, Yazgan Y. Executive dysfunction in Turkish children at high risk for schizophrenia. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008; 17: 424–431. pmid:18427864
  23. 23. Maziade M, Rouleau N, Mérette C, Cellard C, Battaglia M, Marino C, et al. Verbal and visual memory impairments among young offspring and healthy adult relatives of patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: selective generational patterns indicate different developmental trajectories. Schizophr Bull. 2011; 37: 1218–1228. pmid:20410238
  24. 24. Zilles D, Burke S, Schneider-Axmann T, Falkai P, Gruber O. Diagnosis-specific effect of familial loading on verbal working memory in schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2009; 259: 309–315. pmid:19274424
  25. 25. Bernardo M, Bioque M, Parellada M, Saiz-Ruiz J, Cuesta MJ, Llerena A, et al. Assessing clinical and functional outcomes in a gene-environment interaction study in first episode of psychosis (PEPs). Rev Psiquiatr Salud Ment. 2013; 6: 4–16. pmid:23206389
  26. 26. Hollingshead AB, Redlich FC. Class and mental illness. New York: Wiley; 1958.
  27. 27. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV), fourth ed. Washington DC: APA; 1994.
  28. 28. First MB, Spitzer R, Gibbon M. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press Inc; 1997.
  29. 29. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, et al. Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children ‒ present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997; 36: 980–988. pmid:9204677
  30. 30. Gardner DM Murphy AL, O'Donnell H, Centorrino F, Baldessarini RJ. 2010. International consensus study of antipsychotic dosing. Am J Psychiatry. 2010; 167: 686–693. pmid:20360319
  31. 31. Kokkevi A, Hartgers C. EuropASI: European adaptation of a multidimensional assessment instrument for drug and alcohol dependence. Eur Addict Res. 1995; 1: 208–210.
  32. 32. Cuesta MJ, Sánchez-Torres AM, Cabrera B, Bioque M, Merchán-Naranjo J, Corripio I, et al. Premorbid adjustment and clinical correlates of cognitive impairment in first-episode psychosis. The PEPsCog Study. Schizophr Res.2015; 164: 65–73. pmid:25819935
  33. 33. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, third ed. (Administration and Scoring Manual). San Antonio, TX, USA: The Psychological Corporation: 1997.
  34. 34. Wechsler D. Wechsler intelligence scale for children-IV (WISC-IV), 4th ed. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2003.
  35. 35. Reitan RM, Wolfson D. The Halstead–Reitan neuropsychological test Battery: therapy and clinical interpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychological Press; 1985.
  36. 36. Golden C. The Stroop color and word test: a manual for clinical and experimental uses. Chicago: Stoelting Co; 1978.
  37. 37. Conners CK. Continuous performance test II. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems Inc; 2004.
  38. 38. Benedet MJ, Alejandre MA. Test de aprendizaje verbal España—Complutense (TAVEC). Madrid, Spain: Tea Ediciones; 1998.
  39. 39. Loonstra AS, Tarlow AR, Sellers AH. COWAT metanorms across age, education, and gender. Appl Neuropsychol. 2001; 8: 161–166. pmid:11686651
  40. 40. Peña-Casanova J. Test Barcelona. Barcelona: Masson; 1990.
  41. 41. Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL, Kay GG, Curtiss G. Wisconsin card sorting test manual (revised and expanded). Odessa, FL: PAR Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 1993.
  42. 42. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. 2014. URL http://www.R-project.org/.
  43. 43. Meijer J, Simons J, Quee P, Verweij K, Genetic Risk and Outcome of Psychosis (GROUP) investigators. Cognitive alterations in patients with non-affective psychotic disorder and their unaffected siblings and parents. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2012; 125: 66–76. pmid:22013907
  44. 44. Cunha PJ, Rosa PG, Ayres AM, Duran FL. Santos LC, Scazufca M, et al. Cannabis use, cognition and brain structure in first-episode psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2013; 147: 209–215. pmid:23672820
  45. 45. Van Winkel R, Genetic Risk and Outcome of Psychosis (GROUP) Investigators. Family-based analysis of genetic variation underlying psychosis-inducing effects of cannabis: sibling analysis and proband follow-up. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011; 68: 148–157. pmid:21041608
  46. 46. Bioque M, García-Bueno B, McDowell K, Meseguer A, Saiz PA, Parellada M, et al. Peripheral endocannabinoid system dysregulation in first episodes of psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013; 38: 2568–2577. pmid:23822951
  47. 47. Robson PJ. Therapeutic potential of cannabinoid medicines. Drug Test Anal. 2014; 6: 24–30. pmid:24006213
  48. 48. Robles O, Blaxton T, Adami H, Arango C, Thaker G, Gold J. Nonverbal delayed recognition in the relatives of schizophrenia patients with or without schizophrenia spectrum. Biol Psychiatry. 2008; 63: 498–504. pmid:17916332
  49. 49. Becker TM, Kerns JG, Macdonald AW, Carter CS. Prefrontal dysfunction in first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients during a Stroop task. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008; 33: 2619–2625. pmid:18216774
  50. 50. Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Cognitive functioning in schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder and affective psychoses: meta-analytic study. Br J Psychiatry. 2009; 195: 475–482. pmid:19949193
  51. 51. Dickinson D, Goldberg TE, Gold JM, Elvevag B, Weinberger DR. Cognitive factor structure and invariance in people with schizophrenia, their unaffected siblings, and controls. Schizophr Bull. 2011; 37: 1157–1167. pmid:20351040
  52. 52. Greenwood TA, Braff DL, Light GA, Cadenhead KS, Calkins ME, Dobie DJ, et al. Initial heritability analyses of endophenotypic measures for schizophrenia: the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007; 64: 1242–1250. pmid:17984393
  53. 53. Gur RE, Nimgaonkar VL, Almasy L, Calkins ME, Ragland JD, Pogue-Geile MF, et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes in a multiplex multigenerational family study of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2007; 165: 813–819.
  54. 54. Hill SK, Reilly JL, Keefe RSE, Gold JM, Bishop JR, Gershon ES, et al. Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder: findings from the Bipolar-Schizophrenia Network on Intermediate Phenotypes (B-SNIP) Study. Am J Psychiatry. 2013; 170: 1275–1284. pmid:23771174
  55. 55. Zalla T, Joyce C, Szöke A, Schürhoff F, Pillon B, Komano O, et al. Executive dysfunctions as potential markers of familial vulnerability to bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2004; 121: 207–217. pmid:14675740
  56. 56. Groom MJ, Jackson GM, Calton TG, Andrews HK, Bates AT, Liddle PF, et al. Cognitive deficits in early-onset schizophrenia spectrum patients and their non-psychotic siblings: a comparison with ADHD. Schizophr Res. 2008; 99: 85–95. pmid:18083349
  57. 57. Hughes C, Kumari V, Das M, Zachariah E, Ettinger U, Sumich A, et al. Cognitive functioning in siblings discordant for schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2005; 111: 185–192. pmid:15701102
  58. 58. Norman RM, Manchanda R, Malla AK, Harricharan R, Northcott S. The significance of family history in first-episode schizophrenia spectrum disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2007; 195: 846–852. pmid:18043525
  59. 59. Derks EM, Boks MPM, Vermunt JK, and Genetic Risk and Outcome of Psychosis (GROUP) investigators. The identification of family subtype based on the assessment of subclinical levels of psychosis in relatives. BMC Psychiatry. 2012; 12: 71. pmid:22759464
  60. 60. Bertelsen M, Jeppesen P, Petersen L, Thorup A, Øhlenschlaeger J, le Quach P, et al. Five-year follow-up of a randomized multicenter trial of intensive early intervention vs. standard treatment for patients with a first episode of psychotic illness: the OPUS trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008; 65: 762–771. pmid:18606949
  61. 61. Kahn RS, Fleischhacker WW, Boter H, Davidson M, Davidson M, Vergouwe Y, et al. Effectiveness of antipsychotic drugs in first episode schizophrenia and schizophreniform disorder: An open randomized clinical trial. Lancet. 2008; 371: 1085–1097. pmid:18374841
  62. 62. Davidson M, Galderisi S, Weiser M, Werbeloff N, Fleischhacker WW, Keefe RS, et al. Cognitive effects of antipsychotic drugs in first-episode schizophrenia and schizophreniform disorder: a randomized, open-label clinical trial (EUFEST). Am J Psychiatry. 2009; 166: 675–682. pmid:19369319
  63. 63. Galderisi S, Davidson M, Kahn RS, Mucci A, Boter H, Gheorghe MD, et al. Correlates of cognitive impairment in first episode schizophrenia: the EUFEST study. Schizophr Res. 2009; 115: 104–114. pmid:19822407
  64. 64. Kern RS, Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Baade LE, Fenton WS, Gold JM, et al. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 2: co-norming and standardization. Am J Psychiatry. 2008; 165: 214–220. pmid:18172018
  65. 65. Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Kern RS, Baade LE, Barch DM, Cohen JD, et al. The MATRICS consensus Cognitive battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. Am J Psychiatry. 2008; 165: 203–213. pmid:18172019