Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Relationship between Parental Feeding Practices and Neural Responses to Food Cues in Adolescents

Abstract

Social context, specifically within the family, influences adolescent eating behaviours and thus their health. Little is known about the specific mechanisms underlying the effects of parental feeding practices on eating. We explored relationships between parental feeding practices and adolescent eating habits and brain activity in response to viewing food images. Fifty- seven adolescents (15 with type 2 diabetes mellitus, 21 obese and 21 healthy weight controls) underwent fMRI scanning whilst viewing images of food or matched control images. Participants completed the Kids Child Feeding Questionnaire, the Childrens’ Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ) and took part in an observed meal. Parents completed the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionniare and the DEBQ. We were particularly interested in brain activity in response to food cues that was modulated by different feeding and eating styles. Healthy-weight participants increased activation (compared to the other groups) to food in proportion to the level of parental restriction in visual areas of the brain such as right lateral occipital cortex (LOC), right temporal occipital cortex, left occipital fusiform gyrus, left lateral and superior LOC. Adolescents with type 2 diabetes mellitus had higher activation (compared to the other groups) with increased parental restrictive feeding in areas relating to emotional control, attention and decision-making, such as posterior cingulate, precuneus, frontal operculum and right middle frontal gyrus. Participants with type 2 diabetes mellitus also showed higher activation (compared to the other groups) in the left anterior intraparietal sulcus and angular gyrus when they also reported higher self restraint. Parental restriction did not modulate food responses in obese participants, but there was increased activity in visual (visual cortex, left LOC, left occipital fusiform gyrus) and reward related brain areas (thalamus and parietal operculum) in response to parental teaching and modelling of behaviour. Parental restrictive feeding and parental teaching and modelling affected neural responses to food cues in different ways, depending on motivations and diagnoses, illustrating a social influence on neural responses to food cues.

Introduction

Social context is an important influence on eating behaviour. How much people eat and what they choose to eat depends on who they dine with [1]. For instance, adults tend to eat more when eating with friends and family than when alone but, on the other hand, tend to eat less in the company of strangers or other people they wish to impress [1]. In addition, people tend to follow social norms when making food-related decisions even if other people are not present at the time of decision making [2]. For example, exposing a person to information that others are consuming the recommended amount of vegetables in their diet results in greater selection of vegetables at a lunch buffet than when they are simply exposed to the health benefits of eating vegetables [3]. Children and adolescents are similarly susceptible to the influence of social context on eating [4], in particular, the influence of their parents. Parental feeding practices affect the eating environment, food availability and learning about foods, which have all been related to variability in food responsiveness and tendency towards overeating [5].

Several types of parental feeding practice have been previously identified including controlling practices, emotional and instrumental feeding and modelling [6, 7]. Some parents are reported to attempt to use foods to either manage children’s mood state (emotional feeding) or to reward desirable behaviour (instrumental feeding)[8]. These practices have been associated with increased responsiveness to food cues. Controlling feeding practices, involving for example restricting access to “unhealthy” foods, are often imposed by parents of obese children due to concerns about their children’s weight [9] [10] but are not effective in improving the quality of the diet or reducing adiposity in the long term. On the contrary, these feeding practices lead to increased adiposity through their negative impact on children’s eating behaviour. Perceptions of parental controlling feeding practices are associated with greater dietary self restraint, emotional and external eating in preadolescents [11], extreme weight control behaviours [12] and unhealthy eating attitudes [13] in adolescent samples. Controlling feeding practices have also been associated with higher food responsiveness and eating in the absence of hunger [14] and greater emotional eating [15]. Restrictive control appears to be particularly deleterious in its effects on subsequent weight gain in samples of children at risk for being overweight on the basis of parental adiposity [16]. Teaching about eating, on the other hand, especially in the context of modelling intake of foods by parents, has been associated with lower food fussiness and food responsiveness [17]. For example, children are much more likely to try a new food if they see a parent also eating that same food [18].

The use of parental feeding practices in young people vary depending on individual child characteristics, such as their weight. For example, parents of overweight or obese adolescents report greater use of restriction and monitoring than the parents of lean adolescents [19, 20]. Parents who are anxious about their child's eating and weight are more likely to use restrictive feeding practices [10]. A factor that has received little attention is how feeding practices and their consequences relate to the motivations underlying these practices. Restriction of eating may arise from a concern about weight gain but may be required for medical reasons, for example to ensure that a child is following a prescribed diet for control of type 2 diabetes [21]. However, little is known about how parental feeding practices interact with such motivations to influence adolescent eating responses. The specific mechanisms underlying the effects of parental feeding practices on their children’s eating are also unknown. It has been reported that social influence more generally affects activity in brain areas associated with the neural computation of the subjective value of rewards [22, 23]. One possibility is, therefore, that parental feeding practices affect reward-related responses to food cues and this explains at least in part subsequent behaviour around food. Based on this evidence, it is possible that parental feeding practices of restriction, emotive feeding and teaching will be associated with eating behaviour via an effect on neural processes related to food reward.

Recent neuroimaging studies suggest a strong link between obesity and activation of reward related circuits [24]. It has been proposed that obesity, and by extension type 2 DM, results from distortions in reward saliency, motivation (towards food), learning and inhibitory control (e.g. [25]). Consistent with this, several studies have found increased activation to food cues in reward related areas such as caudate, insula, or orbitofrontal cortex in adults with obesity [26]. Furthermore, the activation level of these areas seems to be modulated by context such as stress [27] in obese, but not lean, adults. Although no studies have investigated the social or familial context of eating behaviour in adolescents, it has been found that emotional eating modulates food cue responses in the insula for obese adults (40–70 years old) and in the insula, orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala for adults with type 2 DM [28]. In younger adults, self-restraint in eating has been found to be related to modulation of the functional connectivity in temporal visual association networks (occipital and fusiform regions) [29].

Here we examine how variations in parents’ feeding practices link to adolescents’ responsiveness to food cues using fMRI and a measure of eating in the absence of hunger. We predicted that the use of restriction, emotive feeding and teaching practices will be associated with enhanced reactivity to food cues in reward-related brain areas. In addition, we examine whether similar relationships hold for the parental practices of children for whom there may be specific motivations to control the diet due to weight control (obesity) or medical reasons (type 2 diabetes mellitus; type 2 DM); specifically, perhaps the effect of parental control on reactivity to food cues and reward might be less detrimental when control is motivated by medical reasons rather than weight control goals alone.

While traditionally confined to the middle-aged and elderly population, there has been a marked world-wide increase in type 2 diabetes among younger people [30, 31]. Although its causes are likely to be multi-factorial, childhood obesity is believed to be an important underlying factor [31]. Progression from prediabetes to full diagnosis can be much quicker in the young than in the older population [32]. Both obesity and type 2 DM have been associated with increased activity in brain areas linked to reward when cued to, or reminded of, food [8, 33, 34]. We hypothesise that after diagnosis, adolescents with type 2 diabetes and their carers, who will have received specific dietary counselling and medical care, will show differential feeding practices. This, then, will alter the social context of food. We were, thus, able to examine whether enhanced reward related responding in obesity and type 2 DM are related to specific parental feeding practices.

Methods

Participants

There were 57 adolescent participants, including fifteen with type 2 DM, 21 obese and 21 healthy weight controls (see Table 1). Adolescents with type 2 DM were referred to us by paediatric endocrinologists in the UK Midlands and North-West within the duration of the project. Selection criteria included: (1) between 12–18 years, (2) being able to understand and read English and (3) diagnosis of type 2 DM > 6 months. Obese adolescents were referred by dieticians or responded to advertisements and were included if their BMI exceeded defined International Obesity Task Force age specific cut offs [35]. Healthy weight control participants were recruited from local schools. Exclusion criteria were; major medical conditions (except diabetes, polycystic ovarian syndrome, hirsutism) or learning disabilities, claustrophobia, contraindications to MRI and major changes in diabetes related medication within 6 months. To confirm group allocation, within a 6-month period, before or after scanning, all control and obese participants had their fasting glucose, c-peptide and HbA1c measured and underwent an oral glucose tolerance test. For participants with diabetes clinical data were obtained from their medical files. Clinical and demographic data were obtained from the participants’ clinical files or collected during this study. The groups were matched for (self reported) ethnicity and recruited from similar geographic areas. Fully informed written consent was taken from all participants and their respective parent/guardian prior to participation. The study protocols were approved by the National Research Ethics Committee and the Birmingham University Imaging Centre. Two participants were excluded from analysis, not reflected in the numbers above, due to signal loss and a brain abnormality.

thumbnail
Table 1. Demographics and relevant histories of participants, by group. Age differences were controlled for in the analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157037.t001

Stimuli

A total of 120 food and 120 non-food visually matched (in shape, complexity, brightness and colour) control pictures and 1 target picture were used (Fig 1). The non-food pictures were unrelated to food e.g. buttons, furniture. The food pictures included equal amounts of high fat, high sugar foods (e.g. cake, ice-cream); high fat, low sugar foods (e.g. fried chicken, nuts); low fat, high sugar (e.g. sweets, apples); low fat, low sugar foods (e.g. carrots, peas).

thumbnail
Fig 1.

a) illustration of extract of stimulus sequence. b) examples of stimulus and control pairs from each condition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157037.g001

Procedure

Participants were asked to consume their usual breakfast at home, at approximately 8am, or arrived at 8am and were given breakfast (cereal, juice, toast). All adolescents were then asked to complete the following questionnaires: the Kid’s Child Feeding Questionnaire (KCFQ), to examine perceived parental feeding practices [36], the Perceived Deprivation Scale [37], the Children’s Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire (DEBQ-C) to examine their external, emotional and restrained eating styles [38], and dietary self-efficacy [39] and the adapted motivation for dietary self-care [40, 41]. A parent/guardian was asked to complete the adult version of DEBQ, and the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire (CFPQ) [42]. Adolescents with type 2 DM were also asked to complete the Summary of Diabetes Self-Care Activities scale [43] and the Diabetes Responsibility Scale [21]. Participant weight and height was measured while wearing light clothes without footwear to allow calculation of BMI. Height was measured with a Seca 213 portable stadiometer. Weight was measured with Tanita 384 digital scales. BMI was calculated and converted to z scores adjusted for age and gender according to British 1990 growth reference (UK90) curves [44].

Participants' current disposition to eat (hunger, thirst and fullness) was measured using a10cm Visual Analogue Scale, from 0 (not at all) to 10 (very much). There were no differences between the groups on pre-scan hunger, thirst and fullness scores (F(2, 55) = .205 p = .82; F(2,55) = 1.409 p = .23; F(2,55) = .166 p = .89).

Immediately, prior to the scanning session, at 10.30am, blood glucose was measured by the finger prick method, using a FreeStyle Optium Blood Glucose Monitor. Where necessary, participants were encouraged to take their normal medication or consume a small snack.

fMRI.

Participants viewed food and non-food pictures within the MRI scanner. Pictures were presented using an event-related design in a pseudo random order. Each picture was presented for 1.5s and followed by a fixation cross (3.5s to 9s). Each picture was presented once and the experiment was split into 3 separate runs. Participants were instructed to look carefully at each picture and to imagine eating the food item. Participants were asked to press a button when they saw a yellow smiley face, presented five times per run at random intervals.

After exiting the scanner, participants then rated the food pictures that were shown to them during the scan on how appetising they thought the food was on scale from 1 (not appetising) to 7 (very appetising) and also their current desire to eat each food from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much).

Post-scanning.

After the scanning session both adolescent and parent were taken to a behaviour-recording suite. Participants were invited to have lunch, sat at a table and were each given a tray containing a set prepared meal consisting of: cheese sandwich (150g, 370kcal), individual Chicago Town cheese pizza (150g, 453kcal), bowl of cherry tomatoes (50g, 18kcal), Activia strawberry yoghurt (125g, 123kcal), Granny Smith apple (188kJ, 45kcal), satsuma (approx 75kJ, 18kcal), Walkers ready salted crisps (25g, 130kcal), two Maryland double chocolate cookies (22g, 106kcal) and jug of water and glasses. They were asked not to share food from each other's trays and told that they were not expected to eat all the food, but to eat until they were full. After the participants indicated they had finished, the parent was asked to leave the room to complete further questionnaires and the adolescent was asked to wait. A tray with 4 bowls filled with Maltesers (50g, 253kcal), cucumber (80g, 253kcal), grapes (100g, 60kcal) and Hula-Hoops (25g, 127kcal) were placed in front of the adolescent together with books, magazines and food rating scales. The adolescent was told they could eat the snacks, whilst they waited, and was left for 10 minutes. Food was weighed before and after eating. The researcher left the room during the meal and snack. A remotely adjustable Sony camera (EVI-D70) recorded the eating behaviour.

fMRI acquisition

Participants were scanned on a Phillips 3T Achieva MRI scanner with 8-channel phased array SENSE head coil. Functional scans were acquired using a blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) contrast weighted echo planar sequence (TR = 2500ms, TE = 35ms, 82° flip angle, 4 slices, 2.5x2.5x3mm voxels, 96x96 pixel matrix). A T1-weighted image was acquired for co-registration and display of functional data (TR 8.4ms, TE 3.8ms, flip angle 8°, matrix resolution 288x288, 175 slices, 1x1x1mm voxels).

fMRI Analysis

FMRI data processing used FMRI Expert Analysis Tool Version 5.98, part of FSL (FEAT, www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). The following pre-statistics processing was applied; motion correction using MCFLIRT [45]; slice-timing correction using Fourier-space time-series phase-shifting; non-brain removal using BET [46]; spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of FWHM 5mm; grand-mean intensity normalisation of the entire 4D dataset by a single multiplicative factor and highpass temporal filtering. Melodic ICA was used to identify and remove artefacts from the data. FILM was used for time series statistical analysis with prewhitening [47]. Registration of functional to high resolution and standard images was done using FLIRT [45].

Responses to food cues were evaluated in a whole brain analysis. The first level (within scan) design matrix included regressors for the food, non-food and target trials and motion related regressors. Scans were averaged first within participants and then between groups. Age and gender were entered as covariates of no interest, in addition, to check whether gender affected our results, we re-ran the analysis with only females and found qualitatively the same results. For the reported analyses, Z (Gaussianised T/F) statistic images were thresholded using clusters determined by Z>2.3 and a (corrected) cluster significance threshold of P = 0.05 [48]. To test whether responses to food cues were modulated by behavioural measures (i.e. restriction, emotional feeding etc, see below) these were added as regressors to the general linear model at the group stage. Specific group comparisons are described in the results section.

Factor Analysis

To generate parsimonious variables for describing feeding and eating behaviour, factor analyses of the feeding and eating behaviour variables were conducted for data reduction purposes before examining relationships between the psychological and neuro-imaging variables. Responses from the CFPQ, DEBQ and KCFQ were combined to create factors for parental behaviour. Responses from the DEBQ-C, dietary self-efficacy and the adapted motivation for dietary self-care were analysed to create factors describing adolescent behaviour.

Results

Behavioural Measures

A series of ANOVAs were used to compare measures between groups (see Tables 1 and 2). Each adolescent’s BMI was converted to a Z score (SD-BMI) based on the British 1990 growth reference for height, weight, and body mass index (Cole, Freeman and Preece, 1995). Obese adolescents were defined as having a SD-BMI exceeding 1.96 standard deviations from the mean (>95th percentile). Results are reported for analysis without controlling for education but pattern of results is the same if this is controlled. Participant groups differed in terms of age as well as BMI and diabetes related measures, as expected (Table 1). They did not differ on any other characteristics. There were small group differences in dietary self-efficacy, and adapted motivation for healthy diet measures with the control group scoring highest on the self-efficacy measure and those with obesity scoring higher for amotivation (Table 2). Both the adolescents with type 2 DM and their parents ate the least sugar at lunch, other than this there were no overall significant differences between the groups for the amount eaten (Table A and Table B in S1 File), or in overall hunger or thirst ratings. The ratings of desirability and appeal of the food images did not differ.

thumbnail
Table 2. Questionnaire measures for eating style and reported desirability of the food images.

The table shows the results of comparisons between the group. Significance values reflect results of one way ANOVAs between groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157037.t002

The factor analyses revealed 3 factors each for the parent and adolescent behaviours (Table C and Table D in S1 File). Parental behaviours loaded on to factors that we labelled Teaching and Modelling (discussion of foods, promoting variety and modelling behaviours), Emotive Feeding (using food to modulate emotion, or as reward) and Restrictive Feeding (monitoring and guiding food intake and the child's perception of this). Adolescent questionnaire responses loaded on to three similar factors labelled Self Efficacy (perception of their own ability to control their diet), Emotion/External regulation (the desire to eat when emotional, in the presence of palatable food or other people) and Self Restraint (controlling food to avoid weight gain).

For healthy weight controls and obese participants, none of our factors correlated significantly with eating behaviour or intake (Table E in S1 File). Teaching and Modelling was negatively associated with snack intake for participants with type 2 DM (Table E in S1 File).

fMRI Results

The factors above were used as regressors in the analysis of fMRI data to find areas that were responsive to food images and modulated by feeding or eating behaviour (Fig 2, Tables 3 and 4). To facilitate comparison with existing literature we also report results of the contrast of food images vs. non food images without modulation by these factors (Fig 3, Table F in S1 File). We report the results from analysis of all participants but the pattern of results was not different when we analysed only girls (self-defined by participants), or only those without polycystic ovary syndrome (n = 5).

thumbnail
Fig 2.

a) Areas where activation for Restrictive Feeding (blue) and Self Restraint (red) was significantly higher for the Type 2 diabetes mellitus group compared to the other groups. b) Areas where there was significantly greater activation related to Restrictive Feeding in the control group compared to the other groups. c) Areas where there was significantly greater activation related to Teaching of Healthy Eating in the obese group, compared to other groups. Activations are shown on the average brain for the study. Insert: Relationship between HbA1c test value and MFG modulation to restriction. MFG = Middle Frontal Gyrus, SMG = Supramaginal Gyrus, LOC = Lateral Occipital, OFS = Occipital Fusiform Gyrus, MOG = Medial Occipital Gyrus/occipital cortex.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157037.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Areas of the brain where there is higher (cluster corrected p<0.05, Z>2.3) activation for food images compared to visually matched non-food images for the three groups.

Note that there was no significant difference between the groups when these were directly contrasted so this figure should be interpreted with caution. Colour scale is from red (z score = 2.3) to yellow (z score = 6.6). All images are shown at 2.3–6.6, x = -4, y = -54, z = 30 (MNI). Activations are shown on the average brain for the study. Activations for the three groups overlapped at the right frontal pole, left and right anterior and posterior cingulate, left supramarginal Gyrus, and in the precuneus (mostly left).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157037.g003

thumbnail
Table 3. Food related areas that were more active in participants with reported higher parental Restrictive feeding and Teaching and Modelling.

The table shows areas that were significantly active in the groups alone as well as areas that were found to be significantly more active in one group contrasted with the other groups, indicated in the left column. IPS: Intra Parietal Sulcus. WM: White matter.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157037.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. Food related areas that were more active in participants with reported higher self emotional/external regulation, self restraint and self-efficacy.

The table shows areas that were significantly active in the groups alone as well as areas that were found to be significantly more active in one group contrasted with the other groups, indicated in the left column.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157037.t004

Modulation of food cue activations

There was greater activity for healthy weight controls, compared to those with obesity and type 2 DM, to food images relating to Restrictive Feeding in posterior (predominantly visual) regions of the brain (Fig 2B, Table 3). Teaching and Modelling was associated with increased activity for obese participants (compared to the other groups) to food images in visual brain areas, the thalamus and the parietal operculum (Fig 2, Table 3).

There was greater activity compared to the other groups to food images relating to Restrictive Feeding in the adolescents with type 2 DM in the posterior cingulate, precuneus and middle frontal gyrus (MFG) compared to the other participants (Fig 2, Table 3). Restrictive Feeding related activation in the MFG was negatively correlated with HbA1c in this group, suggesting that when there is Restrictive Feeding, increased frontal activation to food is associated with better diabetes control. There was also unique activation for those with type 2 DM in relation to adolescents' own reports of Self Restraint in the intraparietal sulcus and the angular gyrus (Table 4).

Responses to food images vs. non food images without modulation by behaviour

For comparison with the literature we also report areas where there was higher activation for food images, compared to non-food images, for each group separately. There were no significant differences between the groups for activation to food (compared to non-food) images. Healthy weight participants showed extensive activation to food compared to non-food images (Fig 3, Table F in S1 File). There were peaks in gustation-related areas and activations in areas typically associated with food and reward, such as insula and operculum [49, 50]. In obese participants, food images (compared to non-food images) activated regions in the left precuneus, thalamus, left supramaginal and angular gyrus and regions around the frontal pole and superior frontal gyrus (Fig 3, Table F in S1 File). As expected, participants with type 2 DM had higher activation to food, compared to non-food images, without accounting for behaviour, in an extensive network of brain areas (Fig 3, Table F in S1 File). The regions found were similar to those previously reported to be responsive to food cues and associated with gustation and reward [50].

Activation for food images was correlated with BMI in the type 2 DM and obese groups in a range of regions including the left superior frontal gyrus, left and right inferior frontal gyri, left precuneus, left cingulate and right medio-temporal gyri (Table G in S1 File). For the healthy weight and obese participants, BMI modulated responses to food cues in a similar set of frontal regions, but also the operculum and cingulate.

Activation for images of greater fat food content was higher for people with type 2 DM than for those with obesity in a range of predominantly visual or visual assocation or attention areas (Table H in S1 File) including the calacrine sulcus, right termporal gyrus, right angular gyrus and inferior parietal regions. Activation for high fat food was greater in healthy weight controls than in people with obesity in regions including the left and right insula, left and right operculum and around the right supramarginal gyrus and left inferior frontal gyrus.

Discussion

We investigated the differences in eating styles and behaviour between control (healthy weight) adolescents and adolescents who were obese or had type 2 diabetes. We show that parental feeding practices had different associations with brain responses to food among different groups of adolescents. We found that the three groups demonstrated different responses to food that could not be captured by observational, behavioural or brain imaging results alone. Healthy weight participants showed higher visual activation to food cues when there was higher Restrictive Feeding. For adolescents with obesity the Teaching and Modelling factor was related to higher brain activity in visual and reward related areas. Participants with type 2 DM showed unique food related brain activations modulated by parental Restrictive Feeding practices. This is despite only small differences in food judgements or food consumed (although it should be noted that this was a single recorded eating episode in a laboratory setting). We show, therefore, that parental behaviours are associated with food responses in their children and that this depends on individual differences in motivations to control diet.

We hypothesised that parental restriction would be associated with enhanced reactivity to food cues in reward related areas. Consistent with this, for those with type 2 DM at least, Restrictive Feeding increased activity in the precuneus and posterior cingulate. The areas partially overlap with areas implicated in hedonic hunger [51] this might be consistent with increased reward response to food. These areas, however, are not consistent with more conventional reward or food motivation driven areas. For instance, they do not overlap with areas implicated in the anticipation of chocolate milk reward or that are moderated by treatments known to moderate food reward such as glucagon-like peptide 1[52].

Brain responses to different eating and feeding practices showed different relationships in groups of adolescents believed to have specific motivations to control the diet. Those with a diagnosis of type 2 DM might be considered to have the highest motivation to improve their diet and will have received advice from medical professionals. Consistent with this, the type 2 DM group scored highest on the motivation for dietary change questionnaire and were higher on the Monitoring and Restriction questionnaire measures. For this group there were clear differences in brain activity modulations in response to Restrictive Feeding (see above). In contrast, Restrictive Feeding did not modulate brain activity in those with obesity. For the healthy weight controls, Restrictive Feeding increased activation in brain regions specialised for vision consistent with increased attentional weight (or ‘saliency’) for restricted food rather than reward per se. This suggests that the pattern of activity for those with type 2 DM was linked specifically to the increased motivation for dietary change. This perhaps suggests a mechanism by which behavioural effects of parental restriction have different effects depending on children's tendency to overeat [53].

We expected that the influence of parental feeding would depend on diagnosis (or lack of it). Consistent with this, for obese participants (without diagnosis with type 2 DM), higher scores on the Teaching and Modelling factor, but not Parental Restriction, were related to higher brain activity in visual and reward related areas. This occurred despite the obese group not being significantly higher than the other groups on teaching and modelling related questionnaire scores. This illustrates that within this group, activity is modulated by these parental behaviours, even if the overall level of these behaviours is not higher. Consistent with our results, activation to both food cues and learnt rewards has previously been found to be higher in the parietal operculum in obese adolescents [54] but we extend this to relate specifically to parental teaching. In the obese participants, even Teaching and Modelling may be linked to a heighted orienting of attention to food and linking of food with reward. Indeed, weight change after a weight loss intervention has been found to be positively correlated with food related activation in both reward and visual areas i.e. more activation in visual areas was correlated with weight gain, rather than loss [55].

We found this relationship between teaching and enhanced reward only existed in the obese group. Our data do not allow us to establish the direction of this relationship, nor examine the content or manner of teaching used by parents, so it is possible that the obese adolescents’ focus and attention to food elicits parental teaching about eating. It is worth noting, however that some of our obese group is likely to already have impaired glucose metabolism and that we lack data on weight changes prior to the study. This is almost inevitable in such a sample since the progression for obese adolescents to type 2 diabetes is known to be faster than in adult groups [32]. It is also possible that there is a qualitative difference in the type of teaching received by obese adolescents in comparison to adolescents who are healthy weight or who have type 2 DM that could not be captured by the measures used in this study. Even with these caveats, it is possible to say that parental feeding practices influence neural responses to food differentially, depending on diagnosis.

As well as differences in motivations towards food, the three groups received different medical treatments. Most participants with type 2 DM were receiving Metformin, Insulin, GLP-1 or some combination of the three. Four (of 21) obese participants were receiving Metformin. This study was not designed to investigate the differences between medications, so conclusions about the differential effects cannot be drawn here. Nevertheless it should be noted that insulin has been proposed to have a protective effect in the brain against cognitive decline, at least in older people [56, 57]. Furthermore, insulin administered to young people over 8 weeks has been shown to improve memory and performance on a set of cognitive tests [58]. Similarly there is some evidence that metformin treatment (in rats) improves performance on memory-based tasks [58]. It is possible that the medication received by those with a diagnosis of type 2 DM (or obesity) improves their ability to develop appropriate coping mechanisms or feeding behaviours, as well as acting directly on metabolism.

This study cannot fully elucidate the relationship between Restrictive Feeding and food responses in type 2 DM, but it is worth noting that activity in one of the areas activated, the MFG, was positively correlated with diabetes control (i.e. HbA1c). This area has been implicated in emotional control [59] suggesting that adolescents may be starting to automatically invoke control mechanisms. This is also consistent with our previous finding in older people with type 2 DM that inhibiting emotional responses to food supports dietary management [34]. Alternatively, those with better diabetes control may then have more cognitive resources to support dietary control [60]. Longitudinal studies are necessary to disambiguate this question.

Adolescents with type 2 DM own reports of Self Restraint were also linked to increases in activity in parietal regions commonly linked to spatial attention [61]. These areas are also within the postulated temporal visual association network, activation within which has been correlated with self restraint scores in young adults [29]. This is consistent with both parental Restrictive Feeding and Self-Restraint leading to more complex processing of food cues perhaps involving consideration of duties and obligations [62] and decision making [63] all of which might be consistent with participants’ responses to external restriction of food. Restriction in those with type 2 DM leads to intense processing of food cues, perhaps connected to emotional control, duties or decision-making, despite minimal differences in observed behaviour. This is in contrast to the healthy weight controls where Restrictive Feeding was related to increases in brain regions associated with visual processing, consistent with simple increases in saliency.

Unmodulated responses to food cues

Several studies have suggested that people with T2DM or obesity show alterations in striatal regions (e.g. Nucleus Accumbens, caudate, putamen) limbic (amygdala, hypothalamus, thalamus) and cortical areas such as the insula or inferior parietal cortex in response to food cues or images [64, 65]. It is proposed that obesity increases the anticipatory reward to food cues (but reduces the reward of food itself) [25]. Although we did find responses to food cues in a set of similar areas to those found previously, including parts of the striatal and limbic systems, in the three groups (Table F in S1 File), we did not find the expected between group differences. Furthermore, although activity in frontal and medial areas (i.e. precuneus and cingulate) correlated with BMI (Table G in S1 File), we did not find correlations between food cue responses and activation in striatal or limbic areas. This suggests that in our groups, at least, the proposed reward pathways to simple presentation of food images, are not yet clearly discriminable between groups.

Previous studies have suggested that people with obesity, and, where tested, type 2 DM also show higher activations to images of high calorie food, particularly in the regions described above [64]. This subject has been studied extensively; see [50] for a review. For instance, in a group of obese women, high- calorie food images (compared to low calorie images) activated the putamen [66]. On the other hand, an analysis restricted to reward related regions of interest found that obese women had higher activation in a range of areas (e.g. orbitofrontal regions, insula, anterior cingulate, caudate, hippocampus) but not the putamen [67]. Contrary to these previous findings, we found lower activation for obese, compared to healthy weight participants, in operculum, insula, inferior and orbitofrontal cortex for high fat foods (Table H in S1 File), suggesting, counterintuitively, lower anticipatory reward responses. Those with type 2 DM show higher activation (compared to obese) in predominantly visual or visual association areas, suggesting greater salience of food cues in the diabetic group. Thus, our results, whilst replicating typical gustation reward activations, do not seem to show the same pattern of effects of obesity or type 2 DM.

Chechlacz et al. (34) found that activations in the orbitofrontal cortex and left insula were not only positively associated with external eating and predicposition to eat, but also to dietary self-efficacy and dietary self-care in adults with T2DM. This suggests that, in adults, these areas might be involved restraint immediate desire in favour of long-term outcomes. The data presented here imply that this function may be impaired in the obese group.

One important difference between our participants and those in previous studies is their age. There is some evidence that responses to food cues change with age, at least with healthy weight participants. It has previously been reported that whilst (healthy weight) adolescents show activation to food images in predominantly visual areas (e.g. fusiform gyrus), adults show activation in prefrontal cortex, and it has been suggested that there is an increased role of prefrontal regions in the processing of food with age [50, 68]. There have been few studies of brain activation to food cues in obese adolescents, but what evidence there is suggests that there is little difference in responses to food cues between obese adolescents with, and without insulin resistance, although there were differences in reward related brain activation between lean and obese participants [64]. In our groups, activations are consistent with increasing reward and saliency for only the participants with the highest BMI. It is possible that the documented reward pathways to food take some years to develop, but that the restraint, restriction and teaching behaviours above facilitate and shape these pathways.

Conclusions

We demonstrate that different motivations towards food lead to both different brain activation patterns and different modulations of brain responsivity to food cues. Those with high motivations to improve their diet show neural modulation in proportion to the degree of parental restriction. Parental Teaching and Modelling, on the other hand, was linked to increases in activation in reward related areas. We conclude that social and external factors can influence food based brain activity in the absence of intake and eating behaviour differences.

Acknowledgments

Jennifer Walsh for assistance with data coding. Paul Ashmore and Rebecca Morgan at the Wellcome Trust Research Facility, Birmingham Children’s Hospital who helped with blood testing and recruitment. Jane Fowlie at the University of Nottingham and Dr Adrian Heald, at Leighton Hospital, Crewe, who also helped with recruitment. Some of the study procedures were performed in the NIHR Wellcome Clinical Research Facility by NIHR research staff. Children with type 2 diabetes were recruited from the MRC Patient Cohorts (type 2 diabetes) study G0800661. Parts of this work were presented at the 49th Annual meeting of the European Association for the Study of Diabetes 23–27 September 2013.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: HA JB SH AN. Performed the experiments: HA AC MC. Analyzed the data: HA AC JB MC. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: TB. Wrote the paper: HA AC JB MC TB SH AN. Selected and advised on diagnosis and analysis: TB.

References

  1. 1. Herman CP, Roth DA, Polivy J. Effects of the presence of others on food intake: A normative interpretation. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129(6):873–86. pmid:WOS:000186292400008.
  2. 2. Robinson E, Thomas J, Aveyard P, Higgs S. What Everyone Else Is Eating: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of the Effect of Informational Eating Norms on Eating Behavior. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. 2014;114(3):414–29. pmid:WOS:000332501100010.
  3. 3. Robinson E, Fleming A, Higgs S. Prompting Healthier Eating: Testing the Use of Health and Social Norm Based Messages. Health Psychology. 2014;33(9):1057–64. pmid:24295025
  4. 4. Bevelander KE, Anschutz DJ, Engels RCME. Social norms in food intake among normal weight and overweight children. Appetite. 2012;58(3):864–72. pmid:WOS:000304336800017.
  5. 5. Larsen JK, Hermans RCJ, Sleddens EFC, Engels RCME, Fisher JO, Kremers SPJ. How parental dietary behavior and food parenting practices affect children's dietary behavior. Interacting sources of influence? Appetite. 2015;89:246–57. pmid:WOS:000352328400032.
  6. 6. Shloim N, Edelson LR, Martin N, Hetherington MM. Parenting Styles, Feeding Styles, Feeding Practices, and Weight Status in 4–12 Year-Old Children: A Systematic Review of the Literature. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:1849. doi: https://doi.org/http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01849. pmid:26696920
  7. 7. Patrick H, Nicklas TA. A Review of Family and Social Determinants of Children’s Eating Patterns and Diet Quality. Journal of the American College of Nutrition. 2005;24(2):83–92. pmid:15798074
  8. 8. Carnell S, Gibson C, Benson L, Ochner CN, Geliebter A. Neuroimaging and obesity: current knowledge and future directions. Obesity Reviews. 2012;13(1):43–56. pmid:WOS:000298092500005.
  9. 9. Fisher JO, Birch LL. Eating in the absence of hunger and overweight in girls from 5 to 7 y of age. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2002;76(1):226–31. pmid:WOS:000176378500027.
  10. 10. Farrow C, Blissett J. Does maternal control during feeding moderate early infant weight gain? Pediatrics. 2006;118(2):E293–E8. pmid:WOS:000239440600084.
  11. 11. Houldcroft L, Farrow C, Haycraft E. Perceptions of parental pressure to eat and eating behaviours in preadolescents: The mediating role of anxiety. Appetite. 2014;80:61–9. pmid:WOS:000340307900009.
  12. 12. Loth KA, MacLehose RF, Fulkerson JA, Crow S, Neumark-Sztainer D. Are food restriction and pressure-to-eat parenting practices associated with adolescent disordered eating behaviors? International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2014;47(3):310–4. pmid:WOS:000332149400013.
  13. 13. Haycraft E, Goodwin H, Meyer C. Adolescents' Level of Eating Psychopathology Is Related to Perceptions of Their Parents' Current Feeding Practices. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2014;54(2):204–8. pmid:WOS:000329968800013.
  14. 14. Birch LL, Fisher JO, Davison KK. Learning to overeat: maternal use of restrictive feeding practices promotes girls' eating in the absence of hunger. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2003;78(2):215–20. pmid:WOS:000184366300005.
  15. 15. Kroeller K, Jahnke D, Warschburger P. Are maternal weight, eating and feeding practices associated with emotional eating in childhood? Appetite. 2013;65:25–30. pmid:WOS:000318132300005.
  16. 16. Faith MS, Berkowitz RI, Stallings VA, Kerns J, Storey M, Stunkard AJ. Parental feeding attitudes and styles and child body mass index: Prospective analysis of a gene-environment interaction. Pediatrics. 2004;114(4):E429–E36. pmid:WOS:000224242200006.
  17. 17. Gregory JE, Paxton SJ, Brozovic AM. Maternal feeding practices, child eating behaviour and body mass index in preschool-aged children: a prospective analysis. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2010;7. pmid:WOS:000280403900001.
  18. 18. Harper LV, Sanders KM. Effect of Adults Eating on Young Childrens Acceptance of Unfamiliar Foods. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1975;20(2):206–14. pmid:WOS:A1975AU80900002.
  19. 19. Loth KA, MacLehose RF, Fulkerson JA, Crow S, Neumark-Sztainer D. Food-Related Parenting Practices and Adolescent Weight Status: A Population-Based Study. Pediatrics. 2013;131(5):E1443–E50. pmid:WOS:000318270700008.
  20. 20. Towner EK, Reiter-Purtill J, Boles RE, Zeller MH. Predictors of caregiver feeding practices differentiating persistently obese from persistently non-overweight adolescents. Appetite. 2015;84:120–7. pmid:WOS:000347267000017.
  21. 21. Anderson BJ, Auslander WF, Jung KC, Miller JP, Santiago JV. Assessing Family Sharing of Diabetes Responsibilities. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 1990;15(4):477–92. pmid:WOS:A1990EF65500004.
  22. 22. Nook EC, Zaki J. Social norms shift behavioral and neural responses to foods. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2015;27(7):1412–26.
  23. 23. Zaki J, Schirmer J, Mitchell JP. Social influence modulates the neural computation of value. Psycholgical Science. 2011;22(7):894–900.
  24. 24. Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Fowler JS, Telang F. Overlapping neuronal circuits in addiction and obesity: evidence of systems pathology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences. 2008;363(1507):3191–200. pmid:WOS:000258969400009.
  25. 25. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Baler RD. Reward, dopamine and the control of food intake: implications for obesity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;15(1):37–46. pmid:WOS:000286865500006.
  26. 26. ten Kulve JS, Veltman DJ, van Bloemendaal L, Barkhof F, Deacon CF, Holst JJ, et al. Endogenous GLP-1 mediates postprandial reductions in activation in central reward and satiety areas in patients with type 2 diabetes. Diabetologia. 2015;58(12):2688–98. pmid:WOS:000364221200002.
  27. 27. Jastreboff AM, Sinha R, Lacadie C, Small DM, Sherwin RS, Potenza MN. Neural Correlates of Stress- and Food Cue-Induced Food Craving in Obesity Association with insulin levels. Diabetes Care. 2013;36(2):394–402. pmid:WOS:000314467100043.
  28. 28. van Bloemendaal L, Veltman DJ, ten Kulve JS, Drent ML, Barkhof F, Diamant M, et al. Emotional eating is associated with increased brain responses to food-cues and reduced sensitivity to GLP-1 receptor activation. Obesity. 2015;23(10):2075–82. pmid:WOS:000362201100022.
  29. 29. Kullmann S, Pape A-A, Heni M, Ketterer C, Schick F, Haering H-U, et al. Functional Network Connectivity Underlying Food Processing: Disturbed Salience and Visual Processing in Overweight and Obese Adults. Cerebral Cortex. 2013;23(5):1247–56. pmid:WOS:000318137500024.
  30. 30. Alberti G, Zimmet P, Shaw J, Bloomgarden Z, Kaufman F, Silink M. Type 2 diabetes in the young: The evolving epidemic—The International Diabetes Federation Consensus Workshop. Diabetes Care. 2004;27(7):1798–811. pmid:WOS:000222397100048.
  31. 31. Pinhas-Hamiel O, Zeitler P. The global spread of type 2 diabetes mellitus in children and adolescents. Journal of Pediatrics. 2005;146(5):693–700. pmid:WOS:000229109000023.
  32. 32. D'Adamo E, Caprio S. Type 2 Diabetes in Youth: Epidemiology and Pathophysiology. Diabetes Care. 2011;34:S161–S5. pmid:WOS:000290419300013.
  33. 33. Carnell S, Benson L, Driggin E, Kolbe L. Parent Feeding Behavior and Child Appetite: Associations Depend on Feeding Style. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2014;47(7):705–9. pmid:WOS:000344352600005.
  34. 34. Chechlacz M, Rotshtein P, Klamer S, Porubska K, Higgs S, Booth D, et al. Diabetes dietary management alters responses to food pictures in brain regions associated with motivation and emotion: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Diabetologia. 2009;52(3):524–33. pmid:WOS:000263072600018.
  35. 35. Cole TJ, Flegal KM, Nicholls D, Jackson AA. Body mass index cut offs to define thinness in children and adolescents: international survey. British Medical Journal. 2007;335(7612):194–7. pmid:WOS:000248662200031.
  36. 36. Birch LL, Fisher JO, Grimm-Thomas K, Markey CN, Sawyer R, Johnson SL. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Child Feeding Questionnaire: a measure of parental attitudes, beliefs and practices about child feeding and obesity proneness. Appetite. 2001;36(3):201–10. pmid:WOS:000169238200002.
  37. 37. Timmerman GM, Gregg EK. Dieting, perceived deprivation, and preoccupation with food. Western Journal of Nursing Research. 2003;25(4):405–18. pmid:WOS:000182805900006.
  38. 38. Van Strien T, Van de Laar FA. Intake of energy is best predicted by overeating tendency and consumption of fat is best predicted by dietary restraint: A 4-year follow-up of patients with newly diagnosed Type 2 diabetes. Appetite. 2008;50(2–3):544–7. pmid:WOS:000254126600044.
  39. 39. Nouwen A, Law GU, Hussain S, McGovern S, Napier H. Comparison of the role of self-efficacy and illness representations in relation to dietary self-care and diabetes distress in adolescents with type 1 diabetes. Psychology & Health. 2009;24(9):1071–84. pmid:WOS:000273663200006.
  40. 40. Senecal C, Guay F, Auston S, Nouwen A. Construction and validation of the motivation scale for dietary self-care in adolescents with type 1 diabetes. Manuscript in Prep. 2006.
  41. 41. Austin S, Guay F, Senécal C, Fernet C, Nouwen A. Longitudinal testing of a dietary self-care motivational model in adolescents with diabetes. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2013;75:153–9. pmid:23915772
  42. 42. Musher-Eizenman D, Holub S. Comprehensive feeding practices questionnaire: Validation of a new measure of parental feeding practices. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2007;32(8):960–72. pmid:WOS:000249698600009.
  43. 43. Toobert DJ, Glasgow RE. Assessing diabetes self management: the summary of Diabetes Self Care Activities In: C B , editor. Handbook of Psychology and Diabetes: A guide to psychological measurements in Diabetes research and practice. London: Harwood Academic; 1994.
  44. 44. Cole TJ, Freeman JV, Preece MA. Body mass index reference curves for the UK. Archives of Disease in Childhood 1990;1995(73):25–9.
  45. 45. Jenkinson M, Banton T, Brady M, Smith SM. Improved optimisation for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage. 2002;17 (2):825–41. pmid:12377157
  46. 46. Smith S. Fast Robust Automated Brain Extraction. Human Brain Mapping. 2002;17 (3):143–55. pmid:12391568
  47. 47. Woolrich MW, Behrens TEJ, Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Smith SM. Multilevel linear modelling for FMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference. Neuroimage. 2004;21(4):1732–47. pmid:WOS:000220723900048.
  48. 48. Worsley KJ. Statistical analysis of activation images. In: Jezzard P, M P., Smith SM, editors. Functional MRI: And introduction to Methods. Oxford: OUP; 2001.
  49. 49. Schur EA, Kleinhans NM, Goldberg J, Buchwald DS, Polivy J, Del Parigi A, et al. Acquired differences in brain responses among monozygotic twins discordant for restrained eating. Physiology & Behavior. 2012;105(2):560–7. pmid:WOS:000300459800055.
  50. 50. Garcia-Garcia I, Narberhaus A, Marques-Iturria I, Garolera M, Radoi A, Segura B, et al. Neural Responses to Visual Food Cues: Insights from Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. European Eating Disorders Review. 2013;21(2):89–98. pmid:WOS:000314749000001.
  51. 51. Kroemer NB, Krebs L, Kobiella A, Grimm O, Vollstadt-Klein S, Wolfensteller U, et al. (Still) longing for food: Insulin reactivity modulates response to food pictures. Human Brain Mapping. 2013;34(10):2367–80. pmid:WOS:000324306900001.
  52. 52. van Bloemendaal L, Veltman DJ, ten Kulve JS, Groot PFC, Ruhe HG, Barkhof F, et al. Brain reward-system activation in response to anticipation and consumption of palatable food is altered by glucagon-like peptide-1 receptor activation in humans. Diabetes Obesity & Metabolism. 2015;17(9):878–86. pmid:WOS:000360215900008.
  53. 53. Gubbels JS, Kremers SPJ, Stafleu A, de Vries SI, Goldbohm RA, Dagnelie PC, et al. Association between parenting practices and children's dietary intake, activity behavior and development of body mass index: the KOALA Birth Cohort Study. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2011;8. pmid:WOS:000288861300001.
  54. 54. Stice E, Yokum S, Burger KS, Epstein LH, Small DM. Youth at Risk for Obesity Show Greater Activation of Striatal and Somatosensory Regions to Food. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31(12):4360–6. pmid:WOS:000288750700003.
  55. 55. Murdaugh DL, Cox JE, Cook EW, Weller RE. fMRI reactivity to high-calorie food pictures predicts short- and long-term outcome in a weight-loss program. Neuroimage. 2012;59(3):2709–21. pmid:WOS:000299494000070.
  56. 56. Gerozissis K. Brain insulin, energy and glucose homeostasis; genes, environment and metabolic pathologies. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2008;585(1):38–49. pmid:WOS:000256149300006.
  57. 57. Zhao WQ, Alkon DL. Role of insulin and insulin receptor in learning and memory. Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology. 2001;177(1–2):125–34. pmid:WOS:000169272600016.
  58. 58. Benedict C, Hallschmid M, Hatke A, Schultes B, Fehm HL, Born J, et al. Intranasal insulin improves memory in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2004;29(10):1326–34. pmid:WOS:000223563000012.
  59. 59. Urry HL, van Reekum CM, Johnstone T, Davidson RJ. Individual differences in some (but not all) medial prefrontal regions reflect cognitive demand while regulating unpleasant emotion. Neuroimage. 2009;47(3):852–63. pmid:WOS:000268926200009.
  60. 60. Perantie DC, Lim A, Wu J, Weaver P, Warren SL, Sadler M, et al. Effects of prior hypoglycemia and hyperglycemia on cognition in children with type 1 diabetes mellitus. Pediatric Diabetes. 2008;9(2):87–95. pmid:WOS:000255130400003.
  61. 61. Riddoch MJ, Chechlacz M, Mevorach C, Mavritsaki E, Allen H, Humphreys GW. The neural mechanisms of visual selection: the view from neuropsychology. Year in Cognitive Neuroscience 2010. 2010;1191:156–81.
  62. 62. Johnson MK, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Mitchell KJ, Levin Y. Medial cortex activity, self-reflection and depression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2009;4(4):313–27. pmid:WOS:000273231000001.
  63. 63. Krug A, Cabanis M, Pyka M, Pauly K, Walter H, Landsberg M, et al. Investigation of decision-making under uncertainty in healthy subjects: A multi-centric fMRI study. Behavioural Brain Research. 2014;261:89–96. pmid:WOS:000331497000013.
  64. 64. Jastreboff AM, Lacadie C, Seo D, Kubat J, Van Name MA, Giannini C, et al. Leptin Is Associated With Exaggerated Brain Reward and Emotion Responses to Food Images in Adolescent Obesity. Diabetes Care. 2014;37(11):3061–8. pmid:WOS:000343588500036.
  65. 65. van Bloemendaal L, Ijzerman RG, ten Kulve JS, Barkhof F, Konrad RJ, Drent ML, et al. GLP-1 Receptor Activation Modulates Appetite- and Reward-Related Brain Areas in Humans. Diabetes. 2014;63(12):4186–96. pmid:WOS:000345335500029.
  66. 66. Rothemund Y, Preuschhof C, Bohner G, Bauknecht H-C, Klingebiel R, Flor H, et al. Differential activation of the dorsal striatum by high-calorie visual food stimuli in obese individuals. Neuroimage. 2007;37(2):410–21. pmid:WOS:000248585400005.
  67. 67. Stoeckel LE, Weller RE, Cook EW III, Twieg DB, Knowlton RC, Cox JE. Widespread reward-system activation in obese women in response to pictures of high-calorie foods. Neuroimage. 2008;41(2):636–47. pmid:WOS:000256271100045.
  68. 68. Killgore WDS, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Developmental changes in the functional brain responses of adolescents to images of high and low-calorie foods. Developmental Psychobiology. 2005;47(4):377–97. pmid:WOS:000233152000007.