Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Does Subjective Rating Reflect Behavioural Coding? Personality in 2 Month-Old Dog Puppies: An Open-Field Test and Adjective-Based Questionnaire

  • Shanis Barnard ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Shanis Barnard, Sarah Marshall-Pescini

    Current address: School of Psychology, Queen's University Belfast, Belfast, United Kingdom

    Affiliation Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Università degli Studi di Parma, Parma, Italy

  • Sarah Marshall-Pescini ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Shanis Barnard, Sarah Marshall-Pescini

    sarah.marshall@vetmeduni.ac.at

    Affiliations Comparative Cognition, Messerli Research Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine, Vienna, Medical University of Vienna, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria, Wolf Science Centre, Ernstbrunn, Austria

  • Chiara Passalacqua,

    Affiliation Sez. di Neuroscienze, Dipartimento di Fisiopatologia medico-chirurgica e dei Trapianti, Università di Milano, Milan, Italy

  • Valentina Beghelli,

    Affiliation Dipartimento di Scienze Mediche Veterinarie, Università degli Studi di Bologna, Bologna, Italy

  • Alexa Capra,

    Affiliation Gentle Team, Associazione Sportiva Dilettantistica, Asti, Italy

  • Simona Normando,

    Affiliation Dipartimento di Biomedicina Comparata e Alimentazione, Università degli Studi di Padova, Padua, Italy

  • Annalisa Pelosi,

    Affiliation Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Università degli Studi di Parma, Parma, Italy

  • Paola Valsecchi

    Affiliation Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Università degli Studi di Parma, Parma, Italy

Abstract

A number of studies have recently investigated personality traits in non-human species, with the dog gaining popularity as a subject species for research in this area. Recent research has shown the consistency of personality traits across both context and time for adult dogs, both when using questionnaire based methods of investigation and behavioural analyses of the dogs’ behaviour. However, only a few studies have assessed the correspondence between these two methods, with results varying considerably across studies. Furthermore, most studies have focused on adult dogs, despite the fact that an understanding of personality traits in young puppies may be important for research focusing on the genetic basis of personality traits. In the current study, we sought to evaluate the correspondence between a questionnaire based method and the in depth analyses of the behaviour of 2-month old puppies in an open-field test in which a number of both social and non-social stimuli were presented to the subjects. We further evaluated consistency of traits over time by re-testing a subset of puppies. The correspondence between methods was high and test- retest consistency (for the main trait) was also good using both evaluation methods. Results showed clear factors referring to the two main personality traits ‘extroversion,’ (i.e. the enthusiastic, exuberant approach to the stimuli) and ‘neuroticism,’ (i.e. the more cautious and fearful approach to the stimuli), potentially similar to the shyness-boldness dimension found in previous studies. Furthermore, both methods identified an ‘amicability’ dimension, expressing the positive interactions the pups directed at the humans stranger, and a ‘reservedness’ dimension which identified pups who largely chose not to interact with the stimuli, and were defined as quiet and not nosey in the questionnaire.

Introduction

In recent years there has been an increasing interest in the evolutionary significance of the concept of personality, making it an appealing research topic for comparative psychologists, biologists, and evolutionary scientists alike. As a consequence, many species have become the object of interest for personality-researchers [1], and one species that has received increasing attention is the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) [24]. The reasons for this interest are varied, but the potential applicability, for example in helping to find appropriate homes for shelter dogs [57], selecting the most appropriate puppies for working dog training [814], and the early detection of behavioural problems that may hence obtain rapid treatment [15,16], has had a strong impact on the field.

Various methods have been used to assess the personality of dogs. Behavioural testing has been adopted in a number of studies using a variety of tests [1719]. This method involves presenting a selection of stimuli/situations to dogs in a standardized manner, to allow comparisons to be made. Behavioural coding can then be combined either with detailed analyses of behaviour (mostly done at a later stage from video) or using rater coding of behavioural categories (which can be done either in situ, from video, or both). Detailed analyses of behaviour normally involves measuring frequency, duration and latency of specific behaviours, for example coding the occurrence of behaviours such as bared teeth, raised hackles, and growling [20,21]. Rater-coding is normally done on a predetermined scale [22], which can measure either the presence/absence of a particular behaviour/behaviours (e.g. whether the dogs do or do not exhibit stress signals, which would then include, lip-licking, yawning, etc.) or the intensity of its exhibition (e.g. how much a dog rates on a scale of 1 to 5/7 on for example aggression, each score would then coincide with the presence/absence of a certain combination of behaviours relating to aggression such as growling, baring teeth, snapping etc.) [20].

Finally, numerous studies have used a questionnaire-based approach, where typically either the owner or a person well acquainted with the dog rates the dog’s behaviour in everyday situations [23]. In a number of cases, the questionnaire was validated using a behavioural test or other measure (vet visit in the case of behavioural problems for example) by comparing results obtained with the two different methods [21,2426]. However, only a few attempts have been made to evaluate the degree to which proposed canine personality dimensions predict observed behaviour of individuals in contexts different from those in which they were developed [2527].

Each method used independently has its pros and cons. The problem with behavioural testing is that, aside from the time-consuming aspect of carrying out the test (which can last anything between 2 to 30 minutes), whether using rater-coding, or an in depth analyses of specific behaviours, it requires evaluators to be trained and knowledgeable of dog behaviour, and in the latter case, it requires at least the same amount of time in coding than it did in testing. Questionnaire-based evaluations are much faster, however, so far they have mostly been used with dogs being exposed to a number of different situations (repeatedly), or by asking owners to assess the dogs’ behaviour across many contexts. Furthermore, questionnaire-based evaluations are considered to be more subjective, since the assessment of the dog’s behaviour is filtered through the person’s perception of the dog (including long-held, but perhaps no longer applicable beliefs, or breed prejudices etc.), although according to some authors, given they are normally based on a much wider perspective and information base, they may prove more accurate [28].

A recent meta-analysis of data from 31 studies determined that there is moderately high temporal consistency (R = 0.43) in dog personality scores, with no differences in consistency between personality scores based on behavioural ratings versus behavioural coding [3]. Given the different methods are in theory measuring the same underlying trait, it is surprising how few studies have simultaneously used different evaluation tools. Perhaps, even more worryingly, where questionnaires and behavioural tests have been used in the same study, correlation coefficients among similar personality traits are usually low, falling between 0.2–0.4 [26,29,30]. When questionnaire and behaviour coding targeted very specific traits, then higher correlations emerged. For example, Kubinyi et al.[31] found that dogs assessed by owners as more active-impulsive and inattentive showed also more activity in four behaviour test situations (r = 0.53 and r = 0.25, respectively).

Overall, there is a fair amount of agreement in regards to the personality factors emerging from the behavioural and questionnaire data. However, most factors have been extrapolated from studies assessing the behaviour of adult pet dogs. Hence, it is not clear whether an evaluation of puppy personality (behavioural or questionnaire-based) would result in the same factors emerging in adult dogs. Furthermore, whereas a few adult-based studies have sought to establish the correspondence between behavioural testing and questionnaire-based evaluations [26,29,32,33], this has not been the case for studies with young puppies, where, to our knowledge, only behavioural testing with coder rating has been used to assess personality [34].

The aim of the current study was therefore to use an open-field test accompanied by a simple adjective-based questionnaire to test personality traits of 2-month-old dog puppies and assess the correspondence between these two methods of evaluation. To achieve our aim, a sample of 2-month old puppies was tested at their breeders. Independent observers carried out behavioural analyses from videos of the pups in an open-field test, and a different set of independent observers (unaware of the behavioural coding done by the others) scored each pup on a previously selected adjective-based questionnaire. The correspondence between the two methods was assessed by comparing the personality factors emerging from the results of the behavioural analyses with those emerging from the questionnaires-based evaluation. Consensus (i.e. inter-observer reliability) for the behavioural analyses in the open field test and the adjective-based questionnaire was assessed as a pre-requisite prior to all analysis. To further investigate the validity of the personality assessment using these different tools we assessed the internal consistency of the personality dimensions in the adjective-based questionnaire (i.e. the degree to which judgments about an individual’s personality are consistent across items (i.e. adjectives) thought to reflect the same behavioural dimension [35,36]. Finally, trait consistency (test-retest reliability) was assessed by re-testing a sample of puppies two months later with the same test and comparing results both in terms of the behavioural analyses and questionnaire-based evaluation.

At a theoretical level, identifying personality factors at an early age may increase the likelihood that these dimensions have a genetic component (i.e. endophenotyping), hence potentially providing a tool for gene-behaviour studies and adding to the growing interest on the evolutionary perspectives of personality [37]. Furthermore, a more general tool to assess personality in young puppies may have important outcomes also in a more applied setting, since personality has been shown to be an important variable in determining owner satisfaction [38].

Methods

Ethics statement

No special permission for use of animals (dogs) in such behaviour studies is required in Italy, however when first visiting the breeders, an in depth description of the test was presented by the researcher and consent, to video-record and use data in an anonymous form, was sought verbally prior to testing. Following agreement they compiled a form with details regarding the dog breeding activity (e.g. type of breed, number of females, number of litters per year etc.). Since breeders did not have an active role in the study and were never subjected to evaluation themselves, IRB or equivalent was not required. All procedures were performed in full accordance with Italian legal regulations and the guidelines for the treatments of animals in behavioural research and teaching of the Association for the Study of Animal Behavior (ASAB).

Subjects

A total of 79 litters, representing 21 breeds (range: 1 to 10 litters per breed, median: 3 litters per breed; mean: 3.8 litters; Table 1), were included in the study. Litters came from a total of 55 registered dog breeders. Wherever possible, we chose more than one breeder for each breed to avoid the risk of testing specific bloodlines. All puppies were tested at the breeders before adoption.

thumbnail
Table 1. Summary of the analysis carried out and details on sample size, breed, sex (M, F), number of litters from which the sample was taken associated to each analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149831.t001

An initial sample of 15 video-recorded tests, chosen to represent as much as possible the variability of the whole subject pool, was used only for the adjective-based questionnaire selection process (S1 Information). In this sample pups were balanced for sex (8 M; 7 F) and were representative of 15 different breeds (Table 1).

A sample of 154 puppies (79 males and 75 females) tested at 2 months (range 58–62 days) was selected from the above mentioned 79 litters taking a maximum of 2 puppies (1 male and 1 female wherever possible) from each litter (Table 1). The video coding of the tests carried out with this sample were analysed using both the behavioural and questionnaire-based method to allow an analysis of the personality traits emerging from the two different methods and potential correspondence between them.

Finally, in order to assess the trait consistency (test-retest reliability), a sample of eighteen puppies was tested at both 2 and 4 months of age. This was possible because these pups were not sold but kept by the breeders. Therefore, the test, the environment and the owner were exactly the same as when they were first tested. The questionnaire evaluation was also carried out on these subjects tested at different ages (see Table 1, for subject sex and breed).

Procedures

Open field test.

The open field test was carried out at the breeder’s place in a quiet, 5 x 5 m area, temporarily fenced off, using a portable ‘puppy pen’ (1m high) covered by a dimming net (to avoid distraction from the outside). Testing was normally carried out in the morning (9-11h), but could vary according to the breeder availability. Using powdered chalk, the area inside the pen was divided into 9 identical squares (Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Stimuli and setup in the modified open-field test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149831.g001

Each square contained a stimulus: 1) a realistic looking plastic dog (approx. 50 cm tall), displaying a rather assertive, erect posture, ears forward and docked tail, 2) a bowl of water, 3) a street cone, 4) a mirror propped up to be at puppy height, 5) a child-looking doll standing up (approx. 86 cm high) and positioned with her arms reaching in front of her, and her upper body slightly bent forward; 6) a squeaky dog toy; 7) a small nylon tunnel (53 cm long and 43 cm diameter), similar to those used in agility, with a small piece of food placed inside it. One square was left empty. Finally, two people were also present in the pen: the breeder, seated on a chair in the centre square, and a female researcher, seated on the ground in a corner square. The breeder was asked not to interact with the puppy and remain passive during the test. The experimenter seated on the ground, behaved in a natural way with the pup, in that she did not call or invite interaction but if the pup engaged with her she would briefly respond by petting it then would stop interacting and adopt a relaxed posture. The position of the stimuli was the same for all pups tested.

The breeder was asked to carry the pup into the pen, and once seated, place the pup on the ground in front of his/her feet. The pup was then free to move around in the pen for 5 minutes. A video camera was set up on a tripod outside the pen, and manoeuvred by an assistant so as to insure the pup’s behaviour was recorded during the whole test.

Selection of an Adjective-based Questionnaire.

All questionnaires concerning the personality of dogs available in the literature, which have been specifically designed for use by owners, and mostly refer to the everyday situations dogs may be observed in, were excluded since they are difficult to apply to our current setting. A sub-sample of owner-directed questionnaires however, which were concerned more with scoring the dog’s personality on a number of characteristics best described by adjectives, were used. Of course the owners still refer to their knowledge of the dog in a daily context; however, adjective-based questionnaires, being less specific of the context, may be more easily applied to our research. Initially, four potential adjective-based questionnaires were taken into consideration: the canine Big Five Inventory (BFI) [26]; the Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire (MCPQ/MCPQ-Refined) [3941]; the demographic personality questionnaire [2] and the Free-choice profiling method [42]. However after a systematic selection process (see S1 Information), a modified version of the Ley et al. [40] was used (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. List of adjectives in the five personality subscales derived from the Ley et al. [37] and those used in the current study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149831.t002

Coding and Analyses

The authors of this paper carried out all video analyses, for both behavioural and questionnaire coding.

Behavioural Coding and Analyses.

After viewing approximately 30% of randomly chosen tests, an ethogram of the puppies’ behaviour during the open field test was determined (Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. Behavioural variables recorded during the study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149831.t003

Because the aim of the current study was to assess whether there are broad personality traits already visible in 2 month old puppies, we chose a midlevel analysis, i.e. rather than focus on single units of behaviours such as, the frequency of tail wags or the number of startle responses etc., we opted for a more global assessment of the dogs’ behaviour, especially those behaviours directed towards the stimuli presented. However, although the approach to the single stimulus was recorded as cautious, relaxed or exuberant, the assessment of which category to include the pup in was based on an objective observation of the pups’ behaviour in terms of body posture, speed of approach and tail movement (see Table 3 for a detailed description of these). Deflection from the stimuli (including avoidance behaviours, moving back/away, or a startle response followed by a change of direction) was also coded, as well as social and play behaviours directed either at the people or objects. These midlevel interpretations of the dogs’ behaviour were defined on the basis of specific behavioural patterns emerging from the literature (see Table 3) [4345]. In total 11 mutually-exclusive behavioural categories were recorded continuously in terms of duration of their occurrence. The occurrence of these behaviours was scored as directed towards each stimulus in the test enclosure. Video analyses of behaviours were carried out using behavioural event recording software (Observer XT 8.0, Noldus Information Technology, The Netherlands).

Two observers (SB and VB) scored 154 videos and 40% of these were coded by both. Consensus (inter-observer agreement) was evaluated using Cronbach’s alpha. A preliminary Principal Component Analysis was carried out to identify main clusters of behaviours but the KMO (Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy) was too low (0.539) to validate the analysis hence we decided to collect behaviours by means of a hierarchical cluster analysis (method: average—linkage between groups; similarity measure: Euclidean squared distance; (see e.g. [46,47]) using only behaviours shown by at least 30% of subjects (all behaviours reported in Table 3 except carry toy, deflection and other). The hierarchical cluster analysis creates subsets (or clusters) of objects (i.e., observations, individuals, items of variables) such that those within each cluster have a higher degree of similarity than objects assigned to different clusters. Similarities (or dissimilarities are defined by an appropriate metric (a measure of distance between pairs of observations), and a linkage criterion.

To evaluate trait consistency (test-retest reliability), the videos of the 18 pups tested both at 2 and 4 months of age were all coded by the same observer (SB) and a Spearman’s correlation on the main behavioural clusters was computed.

Questionnaire Coding and Analyses.

All 154 puppies’ tests were coded by SMP (unaware of the ethogram used in the behavioural analyses) using the adapted version of the Ley et al. [40] questionnaire.

A second coder (SN; also unaware of the ethogram used in the behavioural analyses) used the questionnaire to score a random selection of puppy tests (39% of the total). These data were used for consensus (inter-observer reliability) analyses using Cronbach’s alpha. For inter-observer coding, each adjective was analysed independently to evaluate which adjectives received a greater or lesser consensus when evaluating puppy behaviour.

Maintaining, the same behavioural dimensions identified by Ley et al. [40], the internal consistency of the test was calculated using Cronbach’s alpha (the average value of the reliability coefficients one would obtained for all possible combination of items when split into two half-tests) and the mean inter-item correlations (providing an assessment of item redundancy and representativeness of the content domain). It is reported that the combination of both this scores is the most accurate measure of internal consistency [48].

Cronbach’s alpha values expressing the internal consistency on the personality dimensions (see results section) were comparable to those reported for adult dogs by Ley et al. (which varied from 0.74 to 0.87) [40]. However, inter-item correlations were in some cases substantially low. Hence, since no study has been carried out using a questionnaire based methodology on puppies, we ran a Confirmative Factor Analyses (CFA using the Maximum Likelihood extraction method as rotation method, and setting the factor loading at 0.40 [49,50]) to assess whether the adjectives identified by Ley et al. [40] for adult dogs would group into similar factors also in puppies. Finally, Pearson’s correlation coefficients among the personality factors were evaluated.

Finally, SMP also scored all the puppies tested at 2 and 4 months and consistency (test-retest reliability) in scoring at these two time intervals was assessed using Spearman’s correlation coefficient.

Correspondence between behavioural and questionnaire-based method.

Correspondence refers to the extent to which judgments predict an external criterion for “reality” [51]; previous studies identified independent observations of behaviours as the most valuable external criterion [52,53]. Bivariate linear correlations were carried out using Pearson’s r between each personality trait emerging from the questionnaire-based factorial analyses and behavioural clusters emerging from the cluster analysis.

Results

Behavioural Analyses

Consensus.

Inter-observer reliability between the two observers showed an average Cronbach’s alpha of 0.87 in behavioural scoring. In particular, the Cronbach’s alpha carried out on duration of main behavioural categories was as follows: cautious approach/interaction = 0.58, playful interaction = 0.97, walk = 0.97, deflection = 0.87, fast gait = 0.95, carry toy = 0.97, social interaction = 0.77, look at stimulus = 0.96, exuberant approach/interaction = 0.93, positive approach/interaction = 0.98, non-stimuli related behaviour = 0.97.

Hierarchical cluster analysis.

The dendrogram visual inspections along with the agglomeration matrix of the cluster analysis (maximum increment between stadiums criterion) suggested a 5 clusters solution (Fig 2): at the first stadium, we found a cluster composed by two subsets of symmetrical behaviours (exuberant approach/interaction and fast gait labelled “exuberant attitude” vs. look at stimuli and cautious approach/interaction labelled “cautious attitude”). The first subset outlines puppies behaving in one of two extreme manners: either hurtling towards all stimuli with boundless enthusiasm, the second outlines puppies looking at the stimuli from afar and when choosing to interact with them, doing so with a measure of anxiety and caution.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Behaviours hierarchical cluster analysis: agglomeration dendrogram.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149831.g002

At the third stadium a second cluster containing walk and positive approach/interaction, named “relaxed attitude” emerged. This cluster described those puppies that were relaxed in their interaction, investigating the objects in a positive way, yet without showing the extreme exuberance of those pups included in the ‘exuberant’ attitude group.

Social interaction converged with the first cluster at the fourth stadium; last, playful interaction and non-stimuli related behaviour did not converge and remained as single items until the end of the agglomeration program.

Trait consistency (test-retest reliability).

Spearman Rho correlation tests were carried out on the behavioural clusters of puppies tested at 2 months of age and retested at 4 months of age. Significant correlations emerged for two clusters: exuberant attitude (r = 0.69, p = 0.002) and cautious attitude (r = 0.50, p = 0.04) [relaxed attitude (r = 0.26, p = 0.29), social interaction (r = -0.23, p = 0.36), playful interaction (r = 0.42, p = 0.08), non-stimuli related behaviour (r = -0.20, p = 0.94).

Questionnaire Analyses

Consensus.

Inter-observer reliability between the two observers based on 40% of the tests showed a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.87. Cronbach’s alpha expressing the consensus for single adjectives varied from 0.52 to 0.90 (Table 4).

thumbnail
Table 4. Inter-observer reliability measures for each adjective used to assess puppies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149831.t004

Internal consistency and Factor reduction.

Mean internal consistency on the four personality dimensions identified by Ley et al. [40] was calculated on the data from the whole subject pool (154 puppies), coded by a single observer (SMP). Cronbach’s alpha (and inter-item correlation scores) was: 0.87 (0.45–0.94) for extraversion; 0.87 (27–82) for neuroticism, 0.80 (0.004–0.92) for amicability, and 0.77 (0.09–0.70) for self-assuredness.

The Confirmative Factor Analysis (Maximun Likelihood, orthogonal Varimax rotation, based on adjectives in Table 5) did not confirm the goodness of fit of a four-factors model (χ2 = 540.3, df = 227, p < .01; RMSEA = 0.091; CFI = 0.92; 21% not redundant residuals >0.05, [54]), but highlighted that the better, even optimal, model was a 5 factors solution accounting for 76.95% of the variance with substantial, but not complete, overlap with factors identified by the Ley study (χ 2 = 414.23, df = 226, p < .01; RMSEA = 0.07; CFI = 0.95; 12% not redundant residuals >0.05 [54]). The five factors were labelled: Extraversion, Neuroticism, Persistence Amicability, and Reservedness (Table 5).

thumbnail
Table 5. Factor loadings from the questionnaire-based confirmative factorial analysis.

Factor loadings > 0.40 are in bold.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149831.t005

Pearson’s correlation matrix showed that the first factor Extraversion was strongly and positively related to Persistence (r = 0.606) and to Amicability (r = 0.739) and was negatively associated to Neuroticism (r = -0.622) and to Reservedness (r = -.639). A higher score on Neuroticism was related to a weaker value in Persistence (r = -0.512) and Amicability (r = -0.463) and to a higher Reservedness (r = 0.650). Persistence was also related to Reservedness (r = -0.412) and Amicability (r = 0.391); the latter was negatively correlated to Reservedness (r = -0.554).

Hence, the general picture emerging suggests that subjective rating using an adjective based questionnaire is capable of picking out five specific types of puppies. The two major traits (Extraversion and Neuroticism) described respectively either an energetic puppy, who bounded towards the stimuli and explored them whilst displaying a relaxed posture and much tail-wagging, or conversely, a puppy that tended to either avoid the stimuli looking at it from afar, spending time sniffing around and interacting with other elements of the environment or approached them but with a slow gait, in a tentative manner, holding the tail and potentially the hind quarters low, and hence exhibiting signals of mild fear or apprehension.

The Reservedness trait identified those pups whom did not show particular fear or unease in relation to the stimuli but largely chose not to interact with them. The questionnaire also successfully identified the puppies that interacted in a positive manner with the stranger (amicability). The persistence trait was somewhat more problematic, correlating with too many behavioural factors, to allow a clear characterization of exactly what type of puppy it described.

Trait consistency (Test-retest reliability).

Spearman Rho correlation tests were carried out on questionnaire evaluation of puppies at 2 months of age retested at 4 months of age. A significant correlation, between scores at 2 and 4 months emerged only for three factors i.e. Extraversion (r = 0.80, p<0.001), Neuroticism (negative loadings: nervous, fearful, cautious, timid, sensitive; r = 0.67, p = 0.003) and Reservedness (negative loading: quiet; r = 0.56, p = 0.02); [non-significant results: Neuroticism (positive loadings: relaxed, independent; r = 0.35, p = 0.17); Persistence (negative loadings: gentle r = 0.14, p = 0.61); Persistence (positive loadings: persevering, tenacious, restless, assertive, determined r = 0.33, p = 0.20); Amicability (positive loadings: sociable, friendly r = 0.24, p = 0.36); Reservedness (positive loadings: nosey r = 0.42, p = 0.09)].

Correspondence between behavioural analyses and questionnaire-based traits

To assess if the observed behaviour of the puppy could be associated with factor emerging from the personality questionnaire, a set of bivariate correlations was carried out (Table 6, Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons).

thumbnail
Table 6. Pearson’ correlation coefficient and significance levels used to assess the correspondence between the behaviour factors identified by the hierarchical cluster analysis (rows) and personality factors emerging from the questionnaire-based analyses (columns).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149831.t006

The exuberant attitude cluster, i.e. running around the arena with exuberant approach and interactions to objects and people, was positively correlated to Extraversion (r = 0.565, p<0.01), Persistence (r = 0.433, p<0.01) and Amicability (r = 0.492, p<0.01), while negatively correlated to Neuroticism (r = -0.380, p<0.01) and Reservedness (r = -0.400, p<0.01). The cautious attitude cluster, i.e. looking at people/object from a distance and approaching/interacting with the stimuli with a cautious posture, was positively correlated to Neuroticism (r = 0.423, p<0.01) and negatively to Extraversion (r = -0.389, p<0.01) and Persistence (r = -0.412, p<0.01). The relaxed attitude cluster, i.e. walking around the arena with a positive/neutral approach/interaction to the stimuli, showed a negative relation to Neuroticism (r = -0.318, p<0.01) and Reservedness (r = -0.290, p<0.05). Social interaction cluster correlated positively with Amicability (r = 0.445, p<0.01) and playful interaction was positively correlated to Persistence (r = 0.445, p<0.01). Non-stimuli related behaviour was strongly positively correlated to Neuroticism (r = 0.534, p<0.01) and Reservedness (r = 0.607, p<0.01), whilst negatively associated to Extraversion (r = -0.472, p<0.01), Persistence (r = -0.468, p<0.01) and Amicability (r = -0.450, p<0.01).

To summarise, the Extroversion trait from the questionnaire associated positively with the behavioural cluster subgroup expressing an energetic approach to the social and non-social stimuli in the test environment (e.g. fast gait, exuberant approach/interaction, playful interaction) and negatively with those expressing a more cautious approach (e.g. look stimulus, cautious approach/interaction) or a lack of interest in the stimuli (i.e. non-stimuli related behaviours). Interestingly, the analysis identified a very similar pattern of behaviours for the trait Persistence (Table 6), which supports the strong positive correlation between these two traits.

Conversely, the Neuroticism trait of our questionnaire was positively associated with cautious attitude (i.e. look at stimulus, cautious approach/interaction) and the lack of engagement with the stimuli, hence it describes puppies who either explored the stimuli but in a cautious manner, or largely did not interact with these but rather spent time sitting/lying or sniffing around and/or interacting with other elements of the environment (e.g. grass, leaves etc.) Furthermore, this trait correlated negatively with both behavioural dimensions describing positive interactions with the stimuli (exuberant and relaxed attitude) and playful interactions.

The Amicability trait was correlated positively to the social interaction dimension, confirming its description, but it also correlated positively to exuberant attitude, and negatively to cautious attitude and non-stimuli related behaviour. Indeed both exuberant and cautious attitude included respectively a positive and friendly or a more cautious interaction with the person, hence, these correlations are to be expected. The negative correlation with non-stimuli related behaviour is also to be expected since this largely described a puppy that mostly did not interact with the stimuli including the person present. Finally, Reservedness, reflected a puppy who largely chose not to interact with the stimuli (animate and inanimate), since it correlated significantly with non-stimuli related behaviours (e.g. sitting/lying, sniffing around), but negatively with both dimension describing positive interactions with the stimuli (exuberant and relaxed attitude).

Discussion

The aim of this study was two-fold. First, to assess whether individual personality traits could be detected in puppies as early as 2-months of age applying tools that are largely used to assess personality in dogs (i.e. an adjective based questionnaire and a behavioural coding method). Second, to investigate the correspondence between these two methods in defining behavioural patterns. Overall results from both the adjective based questionnaire and the behavioural analysis suggest that at 2-months of age, when exposed to both social and non-social stimuli in an open-field test, puppies already show specific behavioural patterns which can be identified as relating to personality traits. Both methods proved to be reliable tools for the assessment of personality traits in as much as the inter-observer reliability was high in both cases, confirming previous findings [32] and a good correspondence in personality traits emerged between the two independent measures used. A further test of the strength of specific personality traits is represented by potential consistency found over time in the subset of puppies re-tested 2 months later.

A number of personality traits emerged. Based on the behavioural analyses, a first clear cluster which expressed the puppies’ ‘style’ of interacting with both social and non-social stimuli emerged describing an Exuberant versus Cautious attitude. From the questionnaire the first two factors (Extraversion and Neuroticism), largely identified the same pups falling in the Exuberant vs. Cautious factor dimensions, and indeed the high correspondence between these traits confirmed that there is a consistent behavioural pattern in the subjects tested. Overall these traits describe how pups interact with the stimuli in the test, whether they keep a distance and look at them from afar or whether they choose to explore them in an exuberant manner. In many respects this can be equated to the shyness-boldness dimension identified by other studies on dogs [17,27]. The shyness-boldness trait is a dimension, which has been described for many animal species [55], and from the evolutionary perspective it appears to be adaptive since it allows animals to cope with fluctuating environmental conditions [56]. In dogs it is likely that this trait has been maintained from the wolf-ancestor, although it is an open question to what extent wolves and dogs may differ in their representation along this continuum. Regardless, it is considered to be one of the more stable personality traits in dogs [27] and this seems to be confirmed also in the current study, by the fact that it was the only trait showing also temporal consistency in the test-retest reliability analyses.

A separate cluster, although closely linked to the exuberant/cautious attitude one, emerged for sociability (termed ‘Social interaction’) expressed by the time spent interacting with the people in the arena in a friendly manner. A very similar dimension emerged from the questionnaire (termed ‘Amicability’). Looking at the correspondence between the two methods it emerges that Amicability positively correlates with Social interaction and with the Exuberant attitude from the behavioural analysis, thereby confirming the link between these traits. The ‘sociability’ trait has been identified by a number of prior studies on puppies, suggesting it is one of the most easily identified [34]. However, in some studies the sociability trait emerged as embedded within the more general shy-boldness axis [27]. Results from our study are mixed in that it is closely associated with the exuberant attitude although it emerged as a separate factor. Surprisingly, consistency was not confirmed by our test-retest. Given that the puppies were tested twice in the same environment, with no major changes (e.g. adoption, change of home) affecting their social life experiences, we would have expected higher correlation estimates for this trait. Whether this was due to our experimental constraints (i.e. small sample size) or to the particular sensitivity of this trait during development is unclear and needs to be investigated further. Contrasting evidence emerges from the literature. The sociability trait has been reported to be moderately stable over time in adult dogs [57], however, studies on puppies are far fewer. Scott and Fuller [58] reported that social investigation and attraction toward humans remains fairly consistent after 7 weeks of age (p 137). However, a recent review found little consistency over time for this trait in puppies [3]. This discrepancy may depend on several factors: test-retest interval plays an important role in detecting consistency of personality traits (the larger the test interval the smaller the strength of consistency) [3] and age at testing is also known to affect consistency, with several studies showing that testing puppies at less than 12 weeks of age is not predictive of future behaviour [10,18,59].

Playfulness remained as a distinct cluster characterized by those pups spending their time in playful interactions whether with the inanimate stimuli or the experimenter. This factor correlated the most with the Persistence trait emerging from the questionnaire, probably because it described those pups who were persistent in their attempts to play with the experimenter (who was instructed to largely not respond to these attempts), and who persevered in playing with a specific stimuli (e.g. toy) somewhat to the exclusion of all else. Interestingly, the playful trait showed a positive correlation with Extraversion and a negative one with Neuroticism. In a seminal work, Svartberg [60] showed how the selective pressure on dog breeds is still in progress shaping, and significantly affecting the personality of modern breeds and breed lines. Importantly, he reported that popular modern breeds have higher sociability and playfulness scores than both less popular breeds and breeds used in shows, highlighting that both these aspects of a dogs’ behaviour are potentially very salient for pet owners. Indeed, Svartberg’s work suggest that playfulness is a stable trait and could indeed be defined a personality dimension in dogs, confirmed by its stability over time in adult dogs (correlation estimates 0.76–0.89) [25]. However, recent reviews on dog personality [3,34] have not identified playfulness as a trait and, in a recent study, no consistency over time from puppy- to adult-hood was found [59]. Never the less, studies on puppies are still few, hence given the relevance of this behaviour for pet owners, future studies should indeed aim at investigating this aspect of dog’s behaviour further.

The final behavioural cluster emerging, was labelled ‘non-stimuli related behaviour’. It identified puppies that spent most of their time not interacting with the stimuli presented, but rather displayed either passive behaviours (sitting and lying down), or they moved around sniffing the environment but without showing expressions of fear or anxiety. This cluster showed the highest correspondence with the Reservedness trait from our questionnaire, effectively describing a pup which was the opposite of nosey/curious and rather quiet. Results from both measures taken together then, describe those pups that mostly ‘do their own thing’, are not so interested in exploring the stimuli, but do not show great anxiety related to these.

Overall, the correspondence scores emerging from the current study are comparable to other studies with adult dogs in which correspondence between subjective and behavioural methods were found [29,30,32,61]. Indeed, the factors emerging from the personality questionnaire and the behaviours largely showed a coherent picture with a good correspondence between the two methods of analyses. The adjective-based questionnaire, despite not being specifically designed for the current study, showed remarkably similar dimensions to its previous use with adult pet dogs. It easily identified the major dimensions of Extraversion, and Neuroticism and although with a curtailed set of adjectives it also largely allowed the amicability dimension to emerge unchanged compared to the adult study. The larger differences emerged in the Persistence dimension which only partly reflected Ley et al.’s Self-assuredness/Motivation dimension, and the Reservedness dimension which was largely novel. The fact that the adult and puppy dimensions emerging are not identical may be due to the different use of the questionnaire i.e. in an open field test for puppies, or in a pet every day situation for adult dogs, and/or to the fact that the questionnaire used for this study was adapted, i.e. some terms were omitted because not suitable. This could have affected the reduction analysis and factor loadings. However, it may also be that dimensions at this young age are somewhat different. Interestingly, this phenomenon has been reported in developmental psychology where age-specific personality dimensions, independent of the Big Five in adults were reported in adolescents [62]. Future research will be needed to assess whether a similar pattern is occurring also in dogs.

A number of limitations in the current study need to be kept in mind. Jones and Gosling [63] suggested that an important aspect of personality assessment was the consistent emergence of similar traits in different context. Indeed, in a recent study on adult dogs [61], authors looked at the correspondence between dogs’ personality traits as assessed by owner questionnaire and the analyses carried out by researchers on the dogs’ behaviour during a temperament test. In our own study, although similarly to the study with adult dogs we sought to assess the correspondence between a questionnaire-based and behavioural-based analysis, the context remained the same: the open-field test. Future developments of this study would include testing the two different methods to assess puppies’ personality in various experimental contexts (e.g. a playful session with a stranger, and or potential conflict over a food source).

The assessment of the trait’s consistency over time, which is necessary for a factor to be considered a stable personality trait [3] is another limit of the present study. Considering that we were able to re-test only 18 puppies, the conclusion as to the stability over time of the different personality factors identified in the current study are potentially premature and would need confirmation with a larger sample size. The subsample of puppies we were able to test did not undergo any specific selection, in that some were pups that the breeder decided to keep as future breeding stock (which could have been chosen on both morphological or behavioural traits) whilst others were pups that were not yet sold/ given away. Even though there was no systematic bias in the choice of pups the sample size is very small. Nevertheless, consistency over time emerging for the two main behavioural aspects (exuberant and cautious attitude) and questionnaire based personality traits (extroversion and neuroticism) that were also highly correlated with each other, suggest that these factors may form the basis of a dogs’ developing personality. Thus, although further studies with puppies are needed to confirm results on the consistency of personality traits over time, current results on the factors showing correlation between the two time periods are comparable to those reported in previous studies for adult dogs [18,23,31].

Finally, since the aim of the current study was an evaluation of two methods for assessing personality traits in puppies, we sought to maximize the variability of puppies represented in the sample, thereby including subjects from 21 different breeds differing in size and belonging to different breed groups. Nevertheless, some breeds were represented more than others, and breed (at least in adult dogs) has been shown to affect personality [60,64]. Although potential breed differences in the representation of personality traits does not alter current results in terms of the evaluation of the two methods adopted, it is possible that with a different sample of puppies from a different set of breeds, other personality traits would emerge, that were not observed in the current study. Considering breed difference in personality traits have both theoretical (e.g. the effect of selection) and applied (e.g. selection for specific ‘working’ purposes) future research on this aspect would be particularly welcome.

In summary, despite the use of two very different tools, a more easy-to-apply adjective based questionnaire (scored on a 5-point scale) and a more complex and demanding (in terms of time and experience) tool as the behavioural coding (recording frequency and duration of behaviour) results were rather consistent and showed good correlations. The consistent identification of two main ‘types’ of puppies was easily detectable while scoring the puppies in the open-field test. Following the descriptions provided in this paper of Extrovert (Exuberant) or Neurotic (Cautious) puppies, breeders could easily profile the puppies in their litters. As mentioned above, this does not ensure the stability of those traits in dogs’ adulthood never the less it gives a good indication of the present attitude of a pup and could be of help when selecting the most appropriate future family.

Supporting Information

S1 Information. Questionnaire selection method.

Description of the questionnaire selection procedure.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149831.s001

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank all the breeders who kindly participated in this study, Valentina Berto for help in data collection, Danilo Matteuzzi and Antonio Volgarino for technical support and Jenny Essler for language revision of this manuscript. Shanis Barnard, Chiara Passalacqua and Valentina Beghelli were supported by a doctoral grant from the Università di Parma, Università di Milano and Università di Bologna respectively. We thank Almo Natura, for providing dog food samples to give to breeders in thanks for their participation in the study.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: SMP SB AC CP PV. Performed the experiments: SB CP SN VB AC. Analyzed the data: SMP PV SB CP VB AC AP. Wrote the paper: SMP PV SB AP.

References

  1. 1. Gosling SD, John OP. Personality dimensions in nonhuman animals a cross-species review. 1999;8: 69–75.
  2. 2. Mirkó E, Kubinyi E, Gácsi M, Miklósi Á. Preliminary analysis of an adjective-based dog personality questionnaire developed to measure some aspects of personality in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2012;138: 88–98.
  3. 3. Fratkin JL, Sinn DL, Patall EA, Gosling SD. Personality consistency in dogs: a meta-analysis. 2013;8: e54907. pmid:23372787
  4. 4. Rayment DJ, De Groef B, Peters RA, Marston LC. Applied personality assessment in domestic dogs: Limitations and caveats. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2015;163: 1–18.
  5. 5. van der Borg J. A. M., Netto WJ, Planta DJU. Behavioral-testing of dogs in animal shelters to predict problem behavior. 1991;32: 237–251.
  6. 6. Valsecchi P, Barnard S, Stefanini C, Normando S. Temperament test for re-homed dogs validated through direct behavioral observation in shelter and home environment. 2011;6: 161–177.
  7. 7. Dowling-Guyer S, Marder A, D’arpino S. Behavioral traits detected in shelter dogs by a behavior evaluation. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2011;130: 107–114.
  8. 8. Wilsson E, Sundgren P. Behaviour test for eight-week old puppies—heritabilities of tested behaviour traits and its correspondence to later behaviour. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1998;58: 151–162.
  9. 9. Goddard M, Beilharz R. Genetic and environmental factors affecting the suitability of dogs as guide dogs for the blind. Theor Appl Genet. 1982;62: 97–102. pmid:24270555
  10. 10. Goddard M, Beilharz R. Early prediction of adult behaviour in potential guide dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1986;15: 247–260.
  11. 11. Slabbert J, Odendaal J. Early prediction of adult police dog efficiency—a longitudinal study. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1999;64: 269–288.
  12. 12. Serpell JA, Hsu Y. Development and validation of a novel method for evaluating behaviour and temperment in guide dogs. 2001;72: 347–364.
  13. 13. Duffy DL, Serpell JA. Predictive validity of a method for evaluating temperament in young guide and service dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2012;138: 99–109.
  14. 14. Svobodová I, Vápeník P, Pinc L, Bartoš L. Testing German shepherd puppies to assess their chances of certification. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008;113: 139–149.
  15. 15. Godbout M, Frank D. Excessive mouthing in puppies as a predictor of aggressiveness in adult dogs. 2011;6: 93.
  16. 16. Godbout M, Palestrini C, Beauchamp G, Frank D. Puppy behavior at the veterinary clinic: A pilot study. 2007;2: 126–135.
  17. 17. Svartberg K. ShynessΓÇôboldness predicts performance in working dogs. 2002;79: 157–174.
  18. 18. Wilsson E, Sundgren P. The use of a behaviour test for the selection of dogs for service and breeding, I: Method of testing and evaluating test results in the adult dogs, demands on different kinds of service dogs, sex, and breed differences. 1997;53: 279–295.
  19. 19. De Meester RH, De Bacquer D, Peremans K, Vermeire S, Planta DJU, Coopman F, et al. A preliminary study on the use of the Socially Acceptable Behavior test as a test for shyness/confidence in the temperament of dogs. 2008;3: 161–170.
  20. 20. Netto WJ, Planta DJU. Behavioural testing for aggression in the domestic dog. 1997;52: 243–263.
  21. 21. Van den Berg L, Schilder M, Knol B. Behavior genetics of canine aggression: behavioral phenotyping of golden retrievers by means of an aggression test. Behav Genet. 2003;33: 469–483. pmid:14574125
  22. 22. Diederich C, Giffroy J. Behavioural testing in dogs: A review of methodology in search for standardisation. 2006;97: 51–72.
  23. 23. Duffy DL, Serpell JA. Behavioral assessment of guide and service dogs. 2008;3: 186–188.
  24. 24. Hsu Y, Serpell JA. Development and validation of a questionnaire for measuring behavior and temperament traits in pet dogs. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 2003;223: 1293–1300. pmid:14621216
  25. 25. Svartberg K. A comparison of behaviour in test and in everyday life: evidence of three consistent boldness-related personality traits in dogs. 2005;91: 103–128.
  26. 26. Gosling SD, Kwan VS, John OP. A dog's got personality: a cross-species comparative approach to personality judgments in dogs and humans. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;85: 1161. pmid:14674821
  27. 27. Svartberg K, Forkman B. Personality traits in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2002;79: 133–155.
  28. 28. Vazire S, Gosling SD, Dickey AS, Schapiro SJ. Measuring personality in nonhuman animals. 2007: 190–206.
  29. 29. Carrier LO, Cyr A, Anderson RE, Walsh CJ. Exploring the dog park: Relationships between social behaviours, personality and cortisol in companion dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2013;146: 96–106.
  30. 30. Kubinyi E, Gosling S, Miklósi A. A comparison of rating and coding behavioural traits in dogs. Acta Biol Hung. 2015;66: 27–40. pmid:25740436
  31. 31. Kubinyi E, Vas J, Hejjas K, Ronai Z, Brúder I, Turcsán B, et al. Polymorphism in the tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) gene is associated with activity-impulsivity in German Shepherd dogs. 2012;7: e30271. pmid:22272320
  32. 32. Wilsson E, Sinn DL. Are there differences between behavioral measurement methods? A comparison of the predictive validity of two ratings methods in a working dog program. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2012;141: 158–172.
  33. 33. Ley JM, McGreevy P, Bennett PC. Inter-rater and test–retest reliability of the Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire-Revised (MCPQ-R). Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2009;119: 85–90.
  34. 34. McGarrity ME, Sinn DL, Gosling SD. Which personality dimensions do puppy tests measure? A systematic procedure for categorizing behavioral assays. Behav Processes. 2015;110: 117–124. pmid:25267465
  35. 35. Robins RW, John OP. The quest for self-insight: Theory and research on accuracy and bias in self-perception. 1997.
  36. 36. Wiggins JS. Personality and prediction: Principles of personality assessment: Addison-Wesley Pub. Co.; 1973.
  37. 37. Saetre P, Strandberg E, Sundgren P, Pettersson U, Jazin E, Bergstrom TF. The genetic contribution to canine personality. 2006;5: 240–248. pmid:16594977
  38. 38. Zeigler-Hill V, Highfill L. Applying the interpersonal circumplex to the behavioral styles of dogs and cats. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2010;124: 104–112.
  39. 39. Ley JM, Bennett PC, Coleman GJ. A refinement and validation of the Monash Canine Personality Questionnaire (MCPQ). 2009;116: 220–227.
  40. 40. Ley J, Bennett P, Coleman G. Personality dimensions that emerge in companion canines. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008;110: 305–317. doi: https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2007.04.016.
  41. 41. Kubinyi E, Turcsán B, Miklósi Á. Dog and owner demographic characteristics and dog personality trait associations. Behav Processes. 2009;81: 392–401. pmid:19520239
  42. 42. Walker J, Dale A, Waran N, Clarke N, Farnworth M, Wemelsfelder F. The assessment of emotional expression in dogs using a Free Choice Profiling methodology. 2010;19: 75–84.
  43. 43. Goodmann PA, Klinghammer E, Willard J, editors. Wolf Ethogram. Wolf Park Battle Ground IN: Eckhard H Hess Institute of Ethology; 2002.
  44. 44. Koler-Matznick J, Brisbin I, Feinstein M. An Ethogram for the New Guinea Singing (Wild) Dog (Canis hallstromi). 2005.
  45. 45. Handelman B. Canine behavior: A photo illustrated handbook: Dogwise Publishing; 2012.
  46. 46. Sokal RR, Sneath PH. Principles of numerical taxonomy. 1963.
  47. 47. Jolliffe IT. Discarding variables in a principal component analysis. I: Artificial data. 1972: 160–173.
  48. 48. John OP, Soto CJ, Robins R, Fraley R, Krueger R. The importance of being valid. 2007: 461–494.
  49. 49. Kline P. An easy guide to factor analysis: Routledge; 2014.
  50. 50. Comrey A, Lee H. A First Course in Factor Analysis, (New Jersey: Erlbuum, Hillsdale). 1992.
  51. 51. Kruglanski AW. The psychology of being" right": The problem of accuracy in social perception and cognition. Psychol Bull. 1989;106: 395.
  52. 52. Funder DC. On the accuracy of personality judgment: a realistic approach. Psychol Rev. 1995;102: 652. pmid:7480467
  53. 53. Kenny DA. Interpersonal perception: A social relations analysis: Guilford Press; 1994.
  54. 54. McDonald RP. Factor analysis and related methods: Psychology Press; 2014.
  55. 55. Wilson DS, Clark AB, Coleman K, Dearstyne T. Shyness and boldness in humans and other animals. 1994;9: 442–446. pmid:21236920
  56. 56. Réale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall PT, Dingemanse NJ. Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. 2007;82: 291–318.
  57. 57. Svartberg K, Tapper I, Temrin H, Radesater T, Thorman S. Consistency of personality traits in dogs. 2005;69: 283–291.
  58. 58. Scott JP, Fuller JL. Genetics and the Social Behavior of the Dog: University of Chicago Press; 2012.
  59. 59. Riemer S, Müller C, Virányi Z, Huber L, Range F. The predictive value of early behavioural assessments in pet dogs–A longitudinal study from neonates to adults. 2014;9: e101237. pmid:25003341
  60. 60. Svartberg K. Breed-typical behaviour in dogs:Historical remnants or recent constructs?. 2006;96: 293–313.
  61. 61. Mirkó E, Dóka A, Miklósi Á. Association between subjective rating and behaviour coding and the role of experience in making video assessments on the personality of the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2013;149: 45–54.
  62. 62. John OP, Caspi A, Robins RW, Moffitt TE, Stouthamer-Loeber M. The" little five": Exploring the nomological network of the five-factor model of personality in adolescent boys. Child Dev. 1994: 160–178. pmid:8131645
  63. 63. Jones AC, Gosling SD. Temperament and personality in dogs (Canis familiaris): A review and evaluation of past research. 2005;95: 1–53.
  64. 64. Mehrkam LR, Wynne CD. Behavioral differences among breeds of domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris): Current status of the science. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2014;155: 12–27.