Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Environmental Light Exposure Is Associated with Increased Body Mass in Children

  • Cassandra L. Pattinson ,

    cassandra.pattinson@qut.edu.au (CLP); simon.smith@qut.edu.au (SSS)

    Affiliation Centre for Children’s Health Research, Institute for Health and Biomedical Innovation, Queensland University of Technology, Kelvin Grove, Queensland, Australia

  • Alicia C. Allan,

    Affiliation Centre for Accident Research and Road Safety – Queensland (CARRS-Q), Queensland University of Technology, Kelvin Grove, Queensland, Australia

  • Sally L. Staton,

    Affiliation Centre for Children’s Health Research, Institute for Health and Biomedical Innovation, Queensland University of Technology, Kelvin Grove, Queensland, Australia

  • Karen J. Thorpe,

    Affiliation Centre for Children’s Health Research, Institute for Health and Biomedical Innovation, Queensland University of Technology, Kelvin Grove, Queensland, Australia

  • Simon S. Smith

    cassandra.pattinson@qut.edu.au (CLP); simon.smith@qut.edu.au (SSS)

    Affiliations Centre for Children’s Health Research, Institute for Health and Biomedical Innovation, Queensland University of Technology, Kelvin Grove, Queensland, Australia, Centre for Accident Research and Road Safety – Queensland (CARRS-Q), Queensland University of Technology, Kelvin Grove, Queensland, Australia

Abstract

The timing, intensity, and duration of exposure to both artificial and natural light have acute metabolic and physiological effects in mammals. Recent research in human adults suggests exposure to moderate intensity light later in the day is concurrently associated with increased body mass; however, no studies have investigated the effect of light exposure on body mass in young children. We examined objectively measured light exposure and body mass of 48 preschool-aged children at baseline, and measured their body mass again 12 months later. At baseline, moderate intensity light exposure earlier in the day was associated with increased body mass index (BMI). Increased duration of light exposure at baseline predicted increased BMI 12-months later, even after controlling for baseline sleep duration, sleep timing, BMI, and activity. The findings identify that light exposure may be a contributor to the obesogenic environment during early childhood.

Introduction

The contemporary child is exposed to greater daily duration and increased variation in intensity, temporal distribution, and spectra of environmental light, than children of any previous generation [1]. This is attributable to the use of artificial lighting, and has paralleled global increases in the incidence of obesity [1, 2]. Coupled with the known physiological impacts of light on human physiology, this raises a question; is light a factor in pediatric obesity?

It is estimated that 42 million children under the age of 5-years are classified as overweight or obese globally [3], including 23% of children in developed countries [4]. This is a significant clinical, public, and population health concern, as pediatric obesity is associated with a multitude of negative psychosocial and health sequela. Potential mechanisms that might be driving the global increase in obesity include increased calorie intake, decreased physical activity, and more recently, short sleep duration [5, 6], variable sleep timing [7], and gut flora [8]. However, attempts to address these factors have not, as yet, led to effective and sustained change in the prevalence and incidence of obesity [4]. Therefore, significant efforts are being made to identify modifiable factors that contribute to weight gain and that constitute the obesogenic environment. Recent evidence suggests that environmental light exposure may be one such factor.

Light is the principal cue for circadian entrainment in all species [9]. Circadian processes drive physiological and behavioral mechanisms including sleep-wake cycles [10], regulation of metabolism [1113], emotion [14], and body mass [1517]. Through the adoption and use of artificial lighting, humans have constructed a photoperiod that is malleable, creating an environment of relatively dim days and bright nights [1, 18]. Manipulation of the timing, intensity, and duration of light exposure to suit contemporary lifestyles has occurred with limited consideration of its effects on health, behavioral, and environmental outcomes. An understanding of these effects is only now beginning to emerge [1, 14, 18, 19].

Animal studies indicate that the timing and intensity of light exposure is critical for metabolic functioning and weight status. Rodents exposed to continuous white light, even at low levels, exhibited symptoms of metabolic syndrome, increased adiposity, glucose intolerance [20, 21], and reduced sympathetic activity in brown adipose tissue [22], independent of their caloric intake and locomotor activity. Many of these symptoms are abolished when regular light-dark cycles are reinstated [23]. Furthermore, studies of the natural environment indicate that increased artificial light at night, both through direct illumination (e.g. structural, security, street, and advertising lighting) and skyglow, affect the reproductive, migrative, and daily movement behaviors of multiple plant and animal populations [18, 2426]. The cost of these changes are not yet fully understood. In adult humans, morning bright light treatment has been shown to reduce body fat and appetite [27, 28], improve mood [27], and modulate concentrations of the appetite regulating hormones; leptin and ghrelin [29]. Commensurately, recent evidence shows that exposure to light of moderate intensity (~500 lux) earlier in the day is associated with lower body mass, independent of sleep timing, total sleep duration, and activity in adults [17]. Taken together, these data indicate that the timing, duration and intensity of light exposure has a potent role in metabolic and physiological functioning. Early childhood is a pivotal time in the establishment of lifelong growth and adiposity trajectories [30]. However, to date, no studies have examined the effect of habitual light exposure on body mass in children.

The present study investigated the relationship between timing, duration, and intensity of light exposure and weight status of healthy, free-living children aged 3 to 5 years, both concurrently and longitudinally. Standardized independent measurements of body mass index (BMI; kg/cm2) were taken for all participating children at both time points. BMI measurements were transformed into age- and sex-specific BMI z scores for each child [31]. It was hypothesized that, independent of sleep midpoint (used as a proxy for sleep timing and circadian phase; [32]), sleep duration and activity, timing and intensity of light exposure (earlier in the day) would be associated with lower concurrent BMI z score. Further, it was hypothesized that timing of light exposure at baseline, would be associated with BMI z score 12-months later.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

The study protocol was approved by Queensland University of Technology’s Human Research Ethics Committee. Written informed consent was provided by directors, teachers and the legal guardians of the children. Children gave their assent to participate.

Study Design

Initially, 62 healthy pre-school children (32 Males (51.6%); M = 56.51 months, SD = 5.94, and Age Range: 39.0–74.0 months) were recruited for the 12 month study, from six long-day child care services in Brisbane, a capital city in subtropical Australia. Participating child care services were recruited from a pool of 118 services participating in a pre-existing study [33]. All services were located in high socio-economic status (SES) areas according to postcode (SEIFA [34]) and were randomly selected to be approached to take part in the study. Lower SES is associated with a range of sociodemographic factors which may impact upon child health and development [3537], as such services in high SES areas were specifically targeted in the study design to control for some of these variations. Within each childcare service, one room catering for children within the preschool age range (3–5 years) was targeted for recruitment. All children attending the target rooms were invited to participate in the study. To avoid school holiday periods, the 14th of December, 2012 (final date of the school term in Queensland, Australia) was the predefined study endpoint.

At baseline participating families were sent a 14-day sleep diary, parent survey and Actiwatch 2 (MiniMitter Phillips) device. Actigraphs were worn by the study child on the non-dominant wrist for 14 days. The sleep diary was completed concurrently by parents who were asked to record their child’s sleep and wake times, napping behaviour and any instances which the actigraph was removed from the child’s wrist. The parent survey included demographic information and an 8-item Food Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ), which asked parents to indicate, “In the last 24 hours, how often has the study child had the following foods?” with the following trichotomous responses available “Not at all,” “Once,” or “More than once” [38]. Within the 14-day testing period, researchers visited each participating pre-school classrooms on the same designated testing day each week (i.e. Tuesday, Wednesday, or Thursday). Researchers observed the childcare routine and environment, including sleep practices and behaviours, and on one visit measured each participant’s height and weight. The baseline measurements were conducted in the Australian spring/summer between October and December, 2012. During this period, in Brisbane, Queensland (study location) the average sunrise occurred at 4:55am and sunset occurred at 6:17pm.

Participating families were then contacted to participate again 12 months later (follow-up). Accordingly, the follow-up period was between October and December, 2013. All participating families were sent a parent survey and a researcher visited the family to collect the child’s height and weight measurements.

At baseline, complete data was obtained for 49 (79.03%) children. One participant was excluded due to insufficient actigraphy data (< 2 days), giving a final sample of 48 children at baseline. Of the 48 children who completed the baseline measurements, 9 children did not complete the follow-up BMI measurements, giving an attrition rate of 18.75%. No differences were found between those participants who did and did not complete the study at either time point, for gender, age or BMI.

Measurement of light, activity and sleep

The Actiwatch devices measured children’s motor activity (range: 0.5–2G) and white light luminance exposure (range: 5–100,000 lux) in 1 minute epochs for 14 days. The Actiwatch 2 has been calibrated to the international standard ISO-10526 (CIE-S-005) and has been shown to measure illuminance of multiple white light sources in agreement with a National Institute of Standards in Technology (NIST)-traceable photometer [39]. Rest/sleep intervals were assessed by parent reported sleep diaries and wrist actigraphy, using Actiware 5.2 software (Phillips Respironics, Bend, Oregon 97701 USA). Sleep onset, offset and duration were determined using the parent-reported sleep diary in conjunction with the actigraphy data. Sleep onset was determined by using the exact diary time indicated by the parent; unless the time indicated fell on an epoch determined as “sleep” (S) by actigraphy then, sleep onset was operationalized as the last “wake” (W) epoch before the first 3 consecutive S epochs. Sleep offset was determined by using the exact diary time indicated by the parent; unless the time indicated fell on an epoch determined as S then, the time was extended until the first W epoch, after the last 5 consecutive S epochs. Data were cleaned using the Actiware 5.2 software which involved excluding periods in which the parent indicated that the actigraph was removed from the child’s wrist. Total sleep duration was calculated based on the mean duration of all sleep periods (day and night) over the 14-day period. Sleep midpoint was calculated based on the average of sleep onset and offset for the 14-day period. Sleep midpoint was used as a proxy for circadian phase [32]. For a conservative estimate of children’s motor activity and light exposure, any extended inactive periods (>5mins), not recorded by the parents in the sleep diary, were excluded.

Light and activity analysis

Intensity, duration and timing of exposure to light were determined by using a similar methodology to that described by Reid and colleagues [17]. Light and activity data were collected and exported from Actiware 5.2 software in .csv format at a 1-minute (epoch) resolution. Data was imported into RProject 2.11.1. and smoothed using a 5-minute rolling average (to account for the finer measurement window used compared to Reid and colleagues) [17], and then aggregated over 24-hours for each participant. Any 24-hour periods with greater than 4 hours of excluded data were considered invalid and subsequently excluded from further analyses. These aggregate data allowed the calculation of time above threshold (TAT), and mean light timing above threshold (MLiT) [17]. TAT is the average daily number of minutes (epochs) spent above a given lux threshold. This captures both, intensity and duration of light exposure. TAT intensity thresholds ranged from 10 to 3000 lux. MLiT [17] describes the daily distribution of light exposure. Calculation of MLiT incorporates intensity (lux threshold), duration (number of minutes above a threshold), and timing of exposure (clock time of each minute above the threshold; 10 to 3000 lux). The formula used to calculate MLIT was produced by Reid and colleagues [17], however in this study j (minute of day) = 1, …, 1440, as light exposure (lux) was measured at a resolution of 1 minute epochs for 24 hours (24 x 60 = 1440mins). Also, k (day) = 1, …, 14, as the children in this study wore the Actiwatch for 14 days. To illustrate, throughout 24-hours across a 14 day period, a MLiT200 of 721minutes indicates that the child’s light exposure above 200 lux was, on average, centred around 12pm (or the 721st minute in the 1440 minute day from 12am). Representative examples of individual light profiles of participating children are illustrated in Fig 1. The measure of activity used in this analysis was the mean of each epoch of activity over 24 hours, across the 14 days of recording.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Smoothed 7-day light exposure plots from three individual participants.

Light exposure data (measured in lux) were smoothed and shown on a logarithmic scale for three representative children across 7 measurement days (plotted in hours). The horizontal yellow shaded area represents a threshold of ≥200 lux. Points where lux is registered as zero are not shown, as the log of zero is not defined.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143578.g001

BMI analysis

At both baseline and follow-up, height and weight were measured by trained researchers using calibrated stadiometers (SECA Leicester Portable Height Measure) and floor scales (HD-316, Wedderburn Scales; Tanita Corporation, Tokyo, Japan) with subjects dressed in light clothing, and without shoes. Children were measured twice and if measurements differed (weight >0.1kg; height >0.5cm) a third measurement was taken by the researcher. The mean of the measurements were used. Due to the high proportion of this sample being breast-fed at some point during infancy (88.4%) and no significant developmental delays identified, the WHO growth charts were utilised to calculate BMI and growth trajectories of the children [31]. BMI measurements were transformed into sex- and age-specific z scores using the WHO Anthro version 3.2.2 and AnthroPlus version 1.0.4.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted using SPSS v. 22.0.0.0. Sensitivity analyses were conducted to examine if any light variables were associated with BMI z score, both at baseline and follow-up. Consistent with the procedures used by Reid et al. [17], the light exposure variables with the highest correlation with BMI z score were then used in all subsequent analyses. Bivariate correlations (Tables A and B in S1 File) were run to examine the association between BMI z score at baseline and at follow-up with sleep midpoint, sleep duration, activity, diet variables, and the 24hr light variables identified in sensitivity analysis: TAT and MLiT. Sleep midpoint was not normally distributed so a log-linear transformation was conducted. Subsequently, all analyses shown include the log transformed sleep midpoint variable. Nutrition variables were available for a subset of children (n = 42). There was limited variability in parent reporting of nutritional intake. Furthermore, none of the nutritional intake items correlated with either BMI z score (baseline/follow-up) or the light variables. As such the nutritional items were not included within the final regression analyses. Multivariable linear regression models were then used to assess the relationships between BMI z score at baseline with activity, sleep midpoint, and sleep duration and the TAT and MLiT thresholds identified through bivariate correlations. To assess the relationship between light at baseline and BMI z score at follow-up another multivariable linear regression was conducted, adjusting for baseline measures of BMI z score, activity, sleep midpoint and sleep duration. Significance levels are indicated with asterisks: *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

Results

Participant demographics

Participant demographic, BMI, BMI z score, sleep, activity and light characteristics for both baseline and follow-up are described in Table 1. At baseline, the average age of participating children was 4.76 years (SD = 4.94 months), with 52.1% of the sample being female. According to WHO percentiles 97.9% of participants were classified as within the healthy weight range, with one participant being classified as overweight/obese [31]. None of the participating children were classified as underweight, at either time point, in this study. Parents of the participating children predominantly identified themselves as Australian (67%). Please refer to Table C in S1 File for further information regarding identified ethnic group. Valid actigraphy recording ranged from 3–13 days (M = 10.7 days). The average sleep onset time was 20:37 (SD = 00:39), sleep offset was 06:03 (SD = 00:36), sleep midpoint was 01:19 (SD = 00:34), and total sleep duration was 9.64 hours (SD = 31.19 minutes). MLiT200 centered on 12:37 (SD = 00:36) with a range from 11:10 to 14:28. The average duration of time that participating children spent in light above the 200 lux threshold was 3.43 hours (SD = 7.06 minutes). In comparison, children spent 64 minutes (SD = 25.22 mins) above the 2500 lux threshold. At follow-up, the mean age of participants was 5.74 years (SD = 5.12 months) and 51.3% of the sample were female. The majority of children (92.3%) were classified as normal weight, two children (5.1%) classified as overweight and one child (2.6%) classified as obese [31].

thumbnail
Table 1. Participant demographic, sleep, activity, and light characteristics at baseline and follow-up.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143578.t001

Light exposure profiles and baseline BMI z score

To determine which 24-hour light variables (MLiT/TAT), across the thresholds of 10–3000 lux had the strongest association with baseline BMI z score, sensitivity analyses were conducted (Fig 2A and 2B). TAT2500 was identified as having the strongest association with baseline BMI z score. This association was positive, indicating that longer daily duration of light exposure above a threshold of 2500 lux was associated with higher BMI. This illuminance level would be equivalent to outdoor lighting on an overcast day [40, 41]. MLiT was also significantly associated with baseline BMI z score, with the strongest association occurring at MLiT200. Earlier light exposure above a threshold of 200 lux (MLiT200) was associated with higher BMI. Representations of early and later MLiT200 are illustrated in Fig 3. Illumination of 200 lux is approximate to a typical home living room or kitchen [42].

thumbnail
Fig 2. Sensitivity Analyses showing Pearson correlations between BMI z score and a range of MLiT and TAT Light Thresholds (lux) at baseline and follow-up.

* indicates statistically significant at p < .05. (A) TAT thresholds of 2000–3000 lux were significantly associated with BMI z score at baseline, with TAT2500 having the strongest association (N = 48). (B) MLiT thresholds of 100–400 lux were significantly associated with BMI z score at baseline, with the strongest association at MLiT200 (N = 48). (C) TAT thresholds of 10–25 lux were significantly associated with follow-up BMI z score, with the strongest association at TAT10 (N = 39).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143578.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Representative light exposure profiles (log linear lux) for two individual participants with “Early” and “Late” light exposure.

The black dashed line depicts the mean light exposure of all participants across the recording period (N = 48); the blue line depicts the light exposure of a participant classified as having an “Early MLiT200”; the red line depicts the light exposure of a participant classified as having a “Late MLiT200”. The horizontal line represents the 200 lux threshold.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143578.g003

To determine the effect of duration and timing of light exposure on BMI z score, a hierarchical multivariable linear regression analysis was performed. Specifically, we examined the effect of TAT2500 and MLiT200 on BMI z score, after adjusting for activity, total sleep duration, and sleep midpoint (Table 2). The full model accounted for 27.3% of the variance in baseline BMI z-score (F5,42 = 3.153, p = .017, r2 = 0.273). In this model, later sleep midpoint was associated with increased BMI z score (β = .363, p = .020). Although TAT2500 was correlated with BMI z score, it was not a significant independent predictor when entered into the model. However, MLiT200 did have a significant, independent effect on BMI z score (β = -.419, p = .01). This result indicates that earlier exposure to moderate intensity light is associated with increased concurrent BMI z score in preschool children, independent of activity, total sleep duration, and sleep midpoint.

thumbnail
Table 2. Linear regression models predicting BMI z score at baseline and follow-up.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143578.t002

Light exposure at baseline predicts 12-month follow-up BMI z score

After examining the association between light exposure and BMI z score at baseline, we wanted to determine if baseline light exposure would predict BMI z score 12-months later. We conducted a sensitivity analysis to establish if any of the light variables at baseline had an association with BMI z score at follow-up, across each threshold (10–3000 lux). No baseline MLiT variables were associated with BMI z scores at follow-up. However, both TAT10 and TAT25 had a positive association with BMI z score at follow-up (Fig 2C), with the strongest association found for TAT10. Illumination of 10 lux is approximate to a candlelit room.

To test the hypothesis that baseline light exposure predicts BMI z score at follow-up (N = 39 children), a hierarchical multivariable regression analysis was conducted. This model was adjusted for baseline measurements of BMI z score, activity, sleep midpoint and total sleep duration (Table 2). The model significantly predicted a striking 59% of the variance in BMI z score at follow-up (F5,34 = 9.501, p < .001). Even after adjusting for baseline BMI z-score (β = .621, p < .001), TAT10 remained a significant and independent predictor (β = .400, p = .002) of BMI z score 12-months later. This result indicates that longer daily duration of light exposure greater than 10 lux at baseline is associated with increased BMI 12-months later, independent of baseline BMI, activity, sleep midpoint and total sleep duration.

Discussion

This study is the first to investigate the relationship between the timing, duration, and intensity of light exposure and the body mass of young children. We found that daily environmental light exposure had a significant association with the children’s body mass, both concurrently and longitudinally. Our findings are consistent with those from studies conducted with animal models, and in adult humans, which indicate that variations in light exposure may influence body mass [17, 2022, 27]. In the current study, earlier exposure to moderate levels of light was associated with higher concurrent BMI. In clinical terms, for every hour earlier that MLiT200 occurred during the day, there was a .6 unit increase in BMI. While this degree of body mass gain may seem modest, it could indicate an early deviation in a lifelong body mass trajectory. The direction of this relationship contrasts with those reported for adults, where earlier light exposure was found to be associated with decreased body mass [17]. The difference in the direction of these findings may reflect variations in biological timing and threshold of exposure at which light exerts an influence on physiological processes in young children. Consistent with this interpretation, a recent study indicated that adolescents have a heightened sensitivity to light exposure when compared to older adults [43]. Whilst the timing of light exposure at baseline was not predictive of BMI 12-months later, the duration of light exposure was. Specifically, longer duration of total light exposure at baseline was predictive of higher BMI at follow-up. The increased use of electronic equipment such as night lights, tablets, mobile phones, and televisions has been well-documented for children 3–5 years [44, 45]. The current result may help us to better understand findings of an association between this increased duration of screen use and light in the bedroom and body mass in children.

Our findings provide evidence consistent with profound metabolic and physiological effects of light on the human body [18, 19, 4649]. These results are especially striking when we consider that BMI in the first five years of life is predictive of life-long body mass trajectories [30]. Unlike activity, dietary intake, and sleep duration, light exposure is easily and directly manipulated; literally through the flick of the switch. The current ubiquitous social, industrial, and culturally driven manipulation of our environmental light may impact on body mass through three very broad mechanisms that warrant exploration. Firstly, increased light duration may provide insufficient dark, and insufficient metabolic ‘down time’, for normal recuperative processes to occur. Indeed, depending on geographical location, skyglow and other artificial light at night sources are increasing at rates of up to 20% per year [50]. Children are increasingly exposed to broader spectral signatures and more diverse intensity profiles of light [51]. Secondly, chronically increased daily light duration may provide a biological signal analogous to endless summer days, with the potential to amplify any seasonally-driven metabolic processes, such as body mass acquisition [52, 53]. Alternatively, a child’s initial light state may promote some mediating phenomena such as problematic behavior, physiological or metabolic changes, which in turn, promote changes in BMI. One example of light states interacting with physiological behavior is in the case of sleep. Multiple studies document an association between short sleep duration and variability in sleep timing with increased body mass in pediatric populations [57]. Thus, a confounding relationship between sleep and light exposure is expected as sleep timing and duration likely to influence the timing and duration of light exposure. In this study sleep duration was not associated with either BMI or light exposure variables at either time point. Although surprising, this finding is consistent with some research conducted in the early childhood period [54, 55]. Furthermore, the null findings may also be explained by our use of ambulatory recording versus parent report methods used commonly in research reporting an association between sleep and body mass in children (see review [56]). However, it is noted that sleep midpoint was associated with timing of light exposure in this study. Further, in our model, later sleep midpoint was a significant independent predictor of increased BMI z-score. This indicates that timing of sleep and light exposure may be interacting to influence body mass of children.

There are limitations to our study. We have not measured the spectral signatures to which children are exposed, instead measuring light objectively in ambient lux (lumens/m2, weighted to human perception of brightness) [57]. Throughout the day, wavelength composition varies and studies have shown that spectral variations have very distinct impacts on different circadian, behavioral and physiological responses [58]. As such, it is recommended that future studies use devices that measure spectral power distribution, such as spectroradiometers [59]. Direct measurements of circadian phase and metabolic hormones were not determined in this study. Future work should include measurement of circadian phase and metabolic hormone variation of children to provide tests of the direct or indirect path of associations found between light and body mass. For example, timing of light exposure has been shown to affect expression of melatonin and shift circadian phase [59, 60], which in turn impacts on hormones such as insulin [12, 61]. Additionally, the light intensities shown to be significantly associated with body mass in this study need to be confirmed in a larger cohort of children. It is noted that in this study, body mass was treated as a continuum, with only a small number of children classified in the clinical range for overweight and obesity. As such future research could investigate this association using children in the clinical range for overweight and obesity. Although BMI has been shown to have good agreement with body composition in children [62], future studies could consider the use of other estimates of adiposity including; skin fold thickness, waist circumference, or dual energy x-ray. Further research is needed to address these issues as well as the mechanisms responsible for the association between light exposure and body mass in children.

We live in a society of relatively dim days and bright nights [1, 16]. The findings of this study suggest biologically inappropriately timed light exposure and ‘longer’ light periods, may be problematic for body mass of children. If light is in fact a meaningful and distinct contributor to body mass and weight gain, then quantification of light exposure could be included in clinical assessment protocols, and even used routinely in pediatric assessments concerned with incipient obesity. Furthermore, clinical prescription of ‘dark time', analogous to current light therapies, could restitute a state of shorter, brighter days and longer dark nights, with resultant increases in the amplitude of a child’s natural circadian rhythm. Indeed, inexpensive consumer-grade wearables already collect similar data to that provided by actigraphy, and individual tracking of habitual activity, sleep-wake patterns, and light exposure, is already possible. The rapid acceptance and uptake of these devices increases the potential for future effective and well-evaluated public health interventions around light exposure. Likewise, ‘smart house’ applications already allow control of artificial lighting in the home, school, and childcare environments, and provide another potential point for intervention with public health implications. By customizing our light environment, we have launched a global naturalistic experiment, the effects of which are only just beginning to emerge. Our data provides an impetus to investigate environmental light as a factor in the obesogenic environment during human development. This may reveal new targets for pediatric obesity intervention and prevention.

Supporting Information

S1 File.

Table A. Bi-variate correlations between Baseline BMI z-score (BMIz), TAT, MLiT, sleep, and activity (N = 48). Table B. Bi-variate correlations between Follow-up BMI z score (BMIz) and Baseline BMI z score, TAT, sleep, and activity variables (N = 39). Table C. Proportion of parents in each identified ethnic group (N = 42).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143578.s001

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the participating families, services, teachers and research staff who took part in this study. We also thank A. Zele for his comments.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: SLS KJT SSS. Performed the experiments: CLP SLS. Analyzed the data: CLP ACA SSS SLS KJT. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: SLS ACA KJT SSS CLP. Wrote the paper: CLP ACA SLS SSS KJT.

References

  1. 1. Wyse CA, Biello SM, Gill JMR. The bright-nights and dim-days of the urban photoperiod: Implications for circadian rhythmicity, metabolism and obesity. Ann Med. 2014 Jun 5;46(5):253–63. pmid:24901354
  2. 2. Coomans CP, van den Berg SAA, Houben T, van Klinken JB, van den Berg R, Pronk ACM, et al. Detrimental effects of constant light exposure and high-fat diet on circadian energy metabolism and insulin sensitivity. FASEB J. 2013;27(4):1721–32. pmid:23303208
  3. 3. WHO. Obesity: preventing and managing the global epidemic. World Health Organisation Technical Report Series No. 894. Geneva; 2000. Available: http://libdoc.who.int/trs/WHO_TRS_894.pdf
  4. 4. Ng M, Fleming T, Robinson M, Thomson B, Graetz N, Margono C, et al. Global, regional, and national prevalence of overweight and obesity in children and adults during 1980–2013: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2013. The Lancet. 2014;384(9945):766–81.
  5. 5. Bell J, Zimmerman F. Shortened nighttime sleep duration in early life and subsequent childhood obesity. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164(11):840–5.
  6. 6. Scharf R, DeBoer M. Sleep timing and longitudinal weight gain in 4- and 5-year-old children. Pediatric Obesity. 2015;10(2):141–8. pmid:24919802
  7. 7. Golley RK, Maher CA, Matricciani L, Olds TS. Sleep duration or bedtime? Exploring the association between sleep timing behaviour, diet and BMI in children and adolescents. Int J Obesity. 2013;37(4):546–51.
  8. 8. DiBaise JK, Zhang H, Crowell MD, Krajmalnik-Brown R, Decker GA, Rittmann BE. Gut microbiota and its possible relationship with obesity. Mayo Clin Proc. 2008;83(4):460–9. pmid:18380992
  9. 9. Cao R, Gkogkas CG, de Zavalia N, Blum ID, Yanagiya A, Tsukumo Y, et al. Light-regulated translational control of circadian behavior by eIF4E phosphorylatic. Nature Neurosci. 2015;18(6):855–62. pmid:25915475
  10. 10. Borbély AA. Processes Underlying Sleep Regulation. Horm. Res. 1998;49(3–4):114–7. pmid:9550110
  11. 11. Arble DM, Bass J, Laposky AD, Vitaterna MH, Turek FW. Circadian Timing of Food Intake Contributes to Weight Gain. Obesity. 2009;17(11):2100–2. pmid:19730426
  12. 12. Bass J, Takahashi JS. Circadian integration of metabolism and energetics. Science. 2010;330(6009):1349–54. pmid:21127246
  13. 13. Borniger JC, Maurya SK, Periasamy M, Nelson RJ. Acute dim light at night increases body mass, alters metabolism, and shifts core body temperature circadian rhythms. Chronobiol Int. 2014;31(8):917–25. pmid:24933325
  14. 14. Bedrosian T, Nelson R. Influence of the modern light environment on mood. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18(7):751–7. pmid:23711982
  15. 15. Bray MS, Young ME. Circadian rhythms in the development of obesity: potential role for the circadian clock within the adipocyte. Obes Rev. 2007;8(2):169–81. pmid:17300281
  16. 16. Wyse CA, Selman C, Page MM, Coogan AN, Hazlerigg DG. Circadian desynchrony and metabolic dysfunction; did light pollution make us fat? Med Hypotheses. 2011;77(6):1139–1144. pmid:21983352
  17. 17. Reid KJ, Santostasi G, Baron KG, Wilson J, Kang J, Zee PC. Timing and intensity of light correlate with body weight in adults. PLOS One. 2014;9(4):e92251. pmid:24694994
  18. 18. Gaston KJ, Visser ME, Hӧlker F. The biological impacts of artificial light at night: The research challenge. Phil Trans R Soc. 2015 May;370(1667):20140133.
  19. 19. Brooks E, Canal MM. Development of circadian rhythms: role of postnatal light environment. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37(4):551–60. pmid:23454636
  20. 20. Fonken LK, Workman JL, Walton JC, Weila ZM, Morris JS, Haim A, et al. Light at night increases body mass by shifting the time of food intake. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2010;107:18664–9. pmid:20937863
  21. 21. Fonken LK, Lieberman A, Weil ZM, Nelson RJ. Dim light at night exaggerates weight gain and inflammation associated with a high-fat diet in male mice. Endocrinology. 2013;154(10):3817–25. pmid:23861373
  22. 22. Kooijman S, van den Berg R, Ramkisoensing A, Boon MR, Kuipers EN, Loef M, et al. Prolonged daily light exposure increases body fat mass through attenuation of brown adipose tissue activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2015;112(21):6748–53. pmid:25964318
  23. 23. Fonken LK, Weil ZM, Nelson RJ. Dark nights reverse metabolic disruption caused by dim light at night. Obesity. 2013;21(6):1159–64. pmid:23666854
  24. 24. Evans WR, Akashi Y, Altman NS, Manville AM. Response of night-migrating songbirds in cloud to colored and flashing light. N Am Birds. 2012;60(4):476–88.
  25. 25. Kempenaers B, Borgstrӧm P, Lӧes P, Schlicht E, Valcu M. Artificial night lighting affects dawn song, extra-pair siring success, and lay date in songbirds. Curr Biol. 2010;20(19):1735–9. pmid:20850324
  26. 26. Stone EL, Jones G, Harris S. Street lighting disturbs commuting bats. Curr Biol. 2009;19(13):1123–7. pmid:19540116
  27. 27. Dunai A, Novak M, Chung SA, Kayumov L, Keszei A, Levitan R, et al. Moderate exercise and bright light treatment in overweight and obese individuals. Obesity. 2007;15(7):1749–57. pmid:17636093
  28. 28. Danilenko KV, Mustafina SV, Pechenkina EA. Bright light for weight loss: Results of a controlled crossover trial. Obes Facts. 2013;6(1):28–38. pmid:23429094
  29. 29. Figueiro MG, Plitnick B, Rea MS. Light modulates leptin and ghrelin in sleep-restricted adults. Int J Endocrinol. 2012;2012.
  30. 30. Campbell F, Conti G, Heckman JJ, Moon SH, Pinto R, Pungello E, et al. Early childhood investments substantially boost adult health. Science. 2014;343(6178):1478–85. pmid:24675955
  31. 31. World Health Organisation. Physical Status:The use and interpretation of anthropometry. Geneva, Switzerland; 1995.
  32. 32. Martin SK, Eastman CI. Sleep logs of young adults with self-selected sleep times predict the dim light melatonin onset. Chronobiol Int. 2002;19(4):695–707. pmid:12182497
  33. 33. Pattinson CL, Staton SL, Smith SS, Thorpe KJ. Emotional climate and behavioral management during sleep time in early childhood education settings. Early Child Res Q. 2014;29(4):660–8.
  34. 34. Australian Bureau of Statistics. 2033.0.55.001 –Census of Population and Housing: Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas (SEIFA), Australia 2011 [Internet]. Canberra, 2013. Available from: http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/abs@.nsf/mf/2033.0.55.001/
  35. 35. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Ann Rev of Psych. 2002;53:371–99.
  36. 36. O’Dea J, Dibley M, Rankin N. Low sleep and low socioeconomic status predit high body mass index: A 4-year longitudinal study of Australian schoolchildren. Pediatric Obesity. 2012;7(4):295–303. pmid:22505236
  37. 37. Pinot de Moira A, Power C, Li L. Changing influences on childhood obesity: A study of 2 generations of the 1958 British birth cohort. Am J Epidemiol. 2010;171(12):1289–98. pmid:20488872
  38. 38. Irwin M, King L. Understanding the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC)–how can it inform healthy eating and physical activity programs in the NSW early childhood sector?. 2008. Available: http://sydney.edu.au/medicine/public-health/coo/pdf/Understanding_LSAC_V5_Aug08-1.pdf
  39. 39. Koninklijke Philips Electronics. Characterization of Light Sensor Performance for three Models of Actiwatch. N.V.; 2008. Available: https://www.cpapaustralia.com.au/media_files/actiwatch-light-sensor-pe.pdf
  40. 40. The International Commission on Illumination. Guide to recommended practice of daylight measurement. Vienna (AT); 1994. Technical Report No.: CIE 108–1994.
  41. 41. The International Commission on Illumination. Spatial Distribution of Daylight—Luminance Distributions of Various Reference Skies. Vienna (AT); 1994. Technical Report No.: CIE 110–1994.
  42. 42. Turner PL, Van Someren EJW, Mainster MA. The role of environmental light in sleep and health: Effects of ocular aging and cataract surgery. Sleep Med Rev. 2010;14(4):269–80. pmid:20056462
  43. 43. Figuerio MG, Overington D. Self-luminous devices and melatonin supression in adolescents. Lighting Res Technol. 2015 May 6.
  44. 44. Dennison BA, Erb TA, Jenkins PA. Television viewing and television in bedroom associated with overweight risk among low-income preschool children. Pediatrics. 2002;109(6):1028–35. pmid:12042539
  45. 45. Cox R, Skouteris H, Dell’Aquila D, Hardy LL, Rutherford L. Television viewing behaviour among pre‐schoolers: Implications for public health recommendations. J Paediatr Child H. 2013;49(2):e108–e111.
  46. 46. Stevens RG, Rea MS. Light in the built environment: Potential role of circadian disruption in endocrine disruption and breast cancer. Cancer Cause Control. 2001;12(3):279–87.
  47. 47. Dauchy RT, Xiang S, Mao L, Brimer S, Wren MA, Yuan L, et al. Circadian and melatonin disruption by exposure to light at night drives intrinsic resistance to tamoxifen therapy in breast cancer. Cancer Res. 2014;74(15):4099–110. pmid:25062775
  48. 48. Liu D, Fernandez BO, Hamilton A, Lang NN, Gallagher JMC, Newby DE. UVA irradiation of human skin vasodilates arterial vasculature and lowers blood pressure independently of nitric oxide synthase. J Invest Dermatol. 2014;134(7):1839–46. pmid:24445737
  49. 49. Yamauchi M, Jacono FJ, Fujita Y, Kumamoto M, Yoshikawa MY, Campanaro CK, et al. Effects of environment light during sleep on autonomic functions of heart rate and breathing. Sleep Breath. 2014;18(4):829–35. pmid:24522288
  50. 50. Hӧlker F, Moss T, Griefahn B, Kloas W, Voigt CC, Henckel D, et al. The dark side of light: A transdisciplinary agenda for light pollution policy. Ecol Soc. 2010;15(4).
  51. 51. Gaston KJ, Duffy JP, Gaston S, Bennie J, Davies TW. Human alteration of natural light cycles: causes and ecological consequences. Oecologia. 2014;176(4):917–31. pmid:25239105
  52. 52. Ebling FJP. On the value of seasonal mammals for identifying mechanisms underlying the control of food intake and body weight. Horm Behav. 2014;66(1):56–65. pmid:24681216
  53. 53. Simmen B, Darlu P, Hladik CM, Pasquet P. Scaling of free-ranging primate energetics with body mass predicts energy expenditure in humans. Physiol Behav. 2015 January;138:193–199. pmid:25447337
  54. 54. Hiscock H, Scalzo K, Canterford L, Wake M. Sleep duration and body mass index in 0–7-year olds. Arch Dis Child. 2011;96(8):735–9. pmid:21622998
  55. 55. Klingenberg L, Christensen LB, Hjorth MF, Zangenberg S, Chaput JP, Sjödin A, et al. No relation between sleep duration and adiposity indicators in 9–36 months old children: The SKOT cohort. Pediatric Obesity. 2013;8(1):e14–e18. pmid:23225774
  56. 56. Marshall NS, Glozier N, Grunstein RR. Is sleep duration related to obesity? A critical review of the epidemiological evidence. Sleep Med Rev. 2008;12(4):289–98. pmid:18485764
  57. 57. The International Organization of Standardization. 80000—Quantities and Units. 2009. Available from: http://www.iso.org. Accessed 2015 May 6.
  58. 58. Lucas RJ, Peirson SN, Berson DM, Brown TM, Cooper HM, Czeisler CA, et al. Measuring and using light in the melanopsin age. Trends Neurosci. 2014;37(1):1–9. pmid:24287308
  59. 59. Gooley JJ, Chamberlain K, Smith KA, Khalsa SBS, Rajaratnam SMW, Van Reen E, et al. Exposure to room light before bedtime supresses melatonin onset and shortens melatonin duration in humans. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2010;96(3):e463–e472. pmid:21193540
  60. 60. Rajaratnam SMW, Arendt J. Health in a 24-h society. Lancet. 2001;358(9286):999–1005. pmid:11583769
  61. 61. Obayashi K, Saeki K, Iwamoto J, Okamoto N, Tomioka K, Nezu S, et al. Exposure to light at night, nocturnal urinary melatonin excretion, and obesity/dyslipidemia in the elderly: A cross-sectional analysis of the HEIJO-KYO study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2012;98(1):337–44. pmid:23118419
  62. 62. Eisenmann JC, Heelan KA, Welk GJ. Assessing body composition among 3- to 8-year-old children: Anthropometry, BIA, and DXA. Obesity Research. 2004;12(10):1633–40. pmid:15536227