Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Lack of Association between Oxytocin Receptor (OXTR) Gene Polymorphisms and Alexithymia: Evidence from Patients with Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder

  • Min Jung Koh,

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, Bundang Jesaeng Hospital, Seongnam Gyeonggi, Republic of Korea, Department of Psychiatry, Graduate school, Yonsei University, Seoul, Republic of Korea

  • Wonji Kim,

    Affiliation Interdisciplinary Program of Bioinformatics, Seoul National University, Seoul, Republic of Korea

  • Jee In Kang,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry and Institute of Behavioral Science in Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, Republic of Korea

  • Kee Namkoong,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry and Institute of Behavioral Science in Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, Republic of Korea

  • Se Joo Kim

    kimsejoo@yuhs.ac

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry and Institute of Behavioral Science in Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, Republic of Korea

Abstract

Oxytocin receptor gene single nucleotide polymorphisms have been associated with structural and functional alterations in brain regions, which involve social-emotional processing. Therefore, oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms may contribute to individual differences in alexithymia, which is considered to be a dysfunction of emotional processing. The aim of this study was to evaluate the association between oxytocin receptor gene single nucleotide polymorphisms or haplotypes and alexithymia in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. We recruited 355 patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (234 men, 121 women). Alexithymia was measured by using the Toronto Alexithymia Scale. We performed single-marker and haplotype association analyses with eight single nucleotide polymorphisms (rs237885, rs237887, rs2268490, rs4686301, rs2254298, rs13316193, rs53576, and rs2268498) in the oxytocin receptor gene. There were no significant associations between any of the eight single nucleotide polymorphism of the oxytocin receptor gene and alexithymia. In addition, a six-locus haplotype block (rs237885-rs237887-rs2268490-rs4686301-rs2254298-rs13316193) was not significantly associated with alexithymia. These findings suggest that genetic variations in the oxytocin receptor gene may not explain a significant part of alexithymia in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder.

Introduction

Alexithymia is characterized by difficulties in recognizing and effectively expressing one's own feelings [1]. It is difficult for highly alexithymic individuals to distinguish or appreciate the emotions of others [2], and this trait results in an unempathic and ineffective emotional response [3]. These individuals feel less distressed on seeing others in pain, and such blunted emotions can be translated into impaired prosociality [4]. Effective emotional processing, which is known to be impaired in alexithymia [5], is a key component for successful social behavior.

Large-scale twin studies reported that genetic factors could account for 30–42% of the individual differences in alexithymia [6, 7]. Early twin studies reported that the heritabilities of empathy, altruism, and nurturance ranged between 56–72%, and the effect of the shared environment was negligible [8, 9]. These findings suggest that there are some genetic influences on various human traits related to emotional processing, including alexithymia and other prosocial behaviors.

Oxytocin (OT) is a neuropeptide synthesized primarily in the paraventricular and supraoptic nuclei of the hypothalamus and plays a role as both a neurotransmitter and a neuromodulator. It is an important regulator of complex social behavior and emotional states, such as empathy, attachment, trust, social cognition, and emotional regulation[1012] There is growing evidence suggesting a role of OT in the pathophysiology of several psychiatric conditions showing deficits in social functioning such as autism, mood disorder, schizophrenia, and so forth [13]. Intranasal administration of OT has been found to increase trust in unfamiliar persons, enhance facial affect recognition in autism [14], and improve the ability of affective “mind-reading” [15]. A recent study showed that oxytocin increased individuals’ willingness to share emotions [16]. Interestingly, the effect of OT on recognition of complex emotions is particularly pronounced in higher alexithymic individuals [17]. Therefore, OT may be associated with core characteristics of alexithymia such as decreased emotional recognition, expression, and emotional sharing [18].

The effects of OT are modulated by the expression and function of oxytocin receptors (OXTR). Several single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) on the OXTR gene have been documented in association with various aspects of social-affective behavior as well as psychopathology [1921]. OXTR SNPs have some influence on structural and functional changes in several brain regions involved in processing social-emotional information such as the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, amygdala, and hypothalamus [22], all of which are also important in the pathophysiology of alexithymia [23]. From this evidence, it is possible to speculate that the genetic variations of OXTR may influence individuals’ alexithymic traits.

With regard to obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), there have been several studies on the implications of alexithymia on OCD. Alexithymia is prevalent in OCD [24] and associated with poor insight [25], early age at onset, higher anxiety, and sexual/religious obsessions [26]. Studying the effect of the OXTR gene on alexithymia in OCD has several advantages. First, many of the previous genetic studies on alexithymia recruited participants from the normal population. This has limited such studies because of narrow variability in the alexithymic scores, which in turn reduced their power to detect differences. Therefore, it would be more fruitful to use samples with larger variability regarding alexithymia. Hence, we sought to investigate the association between OXTR genetic variants and alexithymia in patients with OCD, who supposedly have more variability in alexithymic traits than controls. Second, alexithymia is a candidate endophenotype for OCD because it is heritable [6, 7] and shares the core characteristics of OCD. Thus, elucidating the relationship between the OXTR gene and alexithymia may help identify predisposing genes for OCD.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

We recruited 355 patients (234 men, 121 women) from the OCD clinic at Severance Hospital, Yonsei University Health System (a tertiary referral hospital in Korea) from August 2006 to May 2015. All participants met the criteria for OCD, as determined by the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV (SCID) [27] assessed by a trained psychiatrist (S.J. KIM). This group was composed of patients in different stages of the illness and with different degrees of severity. All patients were taking psychotropic medications (mainly selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and/or low-dose benzodiazepines). All participants were Korean and gave written informed consent prior to the beginning of this study. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Severance hospital, Yonsei University Health System.

Assessment of alexithymia

The degree of alexithymia was measured with the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20), which is a 20-item self-report scale with a five-point Likert-type scale[28]. The TAS-20 comprises three subdimensions: 1) difficulty in identifying feelings (DIF, seven items), 2) difficulty in describing feelings (DDF, five items) and 3) externally oriented thinking (EOT, eight items). The TAS-20 was previously proven to be valid and reliable [28] and was validated for the Korean population [29]. All participants completed the Korean version of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale [30].

Measures of clinical symptoms

The clinical symptoms and the severity of OCD symptoms were evaluated by using the Yale-Brown obsessive-compulsive scale (Y-BOCS) [31]. The Y-BOCS is a reliable and valid 10-item scale administered by a clinician and used to assess the severity of obsessions and compulsions. Levels of depressive symptoms were assessed by using the Montgomery–Åsperg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS) [32], which is a well-known scale widely used by trained psychiatrists.

SNP selection and Genotyping

First, we selected OXTR SNPs which showed significant association with empathy, prosocial behavior, sociability, emotionality, social cognition, and social deficit by using a PubMed search of previous studies [19, 3342]. Nine SNPs were selected considering reported minor allele frequencies (MAF) greater than 0.1 from the 1000 Genomes Project database, JPT sample (June 2010 release). Among them, rs2268491 was excluded because it was in complete linkage disequilibrium (LD; D′ = 1, r2 = 0.97) with rs2254298 in our sample. Therefore, eight SNPs with an r2 threshold < 0.8 in ‘pair-wise tagging only’ mode using the ‘Tagger’ program in Haploview [43] were included in the final statistical analysis (Table 1; Fig 1). Peripheral blood samples were obtained from each subject, and genomic DNA was extracted from the leukocytes. The SNaPshot assay was performed according to the manufacturer’s instructions (ABI PRISM SNaPShot Multiplex kit, Foster City, CA, USA).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Selection of OXTR single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP).

Circle denote SNPs significantly associated with prosocial behavior in single-marker analyses of previous studies, with asterisks representing SNPs included the current association study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143168.g001

thumbnail
Table 1. Characteristics of SNP markers on the OXTR gene.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143168.t001

Statistical analyses

Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium values of the SNPs were tested using χ2 tests. Generalized estimating equations (GEE) were used to analyze the relationship between total or three subdimension scores of TAS-20 and SNPs, adjusting for age, total Y-BOCS, and MADRS scores. An additive model was applied by coding genotypes as 0, 1, or 2, depending on carrier status of the minor allele.

Haploview v4.0 (http://www.broadinstitute.org/haploview/haploview) [43] was used to estimate the pairwise LD of SNP markers. The default confidence interval algorithm of the Haploview program identified a single haplotype block consisting of SNP1 (rs237885), SNP2 (rs237887), SNP3 (rs2268490), SNP4 (rs4686301), SNP5 (rs2254298), and SNP6 (rs13316193) from our data (Fig 2). The associations between OXTR gene haplotypes and the TAS-20 total scores and scores of the three subdimensions were examined using the “haplo.score” function of the program ‘haplo.stats’ (http://cran.r-project.org/src/contrib/Descriptions/haplo.stats.html) controlling for age, total Y-BOCS and MADRS scores. Permutation tests (n = 10,000) were performed to estimate the global significance of the results for all haplotypes analyzed and to validate the expectation-maximization values.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Linkage disequilibrium (LD) structure of the single-nucleotide polymorphisms and a haplotype block analyzed in the current study (Block1:SNP1-SNP6).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143168.g002

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics of the subjects are presented in Table 2. The mean age of subjects was 30.48 ± 11.13 years old. The mean age of onset of obsessive-compulsive symptoms was 18.82 ± 9.21. The mean scores of Y-BOCS, and MADRS were 22.16 ± 8.70, and 19.07 ± 8.99, respectively. Because the patients’ total scores and some subdimension scores on the TAS-20 were significantly correlated with the total Y-BOCS and MADRS scores, and age, although the size of the correlation between the TAS-20 scores (total and subdimension scores) and the Y-BOCS or age were negligible (absolute r = 0.13–0.19, data not present), we used these factors as covariates in subsequent analyses to control for their potential effects on alexithymia.

thumbnail
Table 2. Demographic and clinical characteristics of subjects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143168.t002

We used GEE approaches to examine the effect of the OXTR gene polymorphisms on the total or subdimension scores of the TAS-20 for each of eight SNPs. There were no significant associations found between any of the OXTR SNPs and alexithymia. In addition, a six-locus haplotype block (rs237885-rs237887-rs2268490-rs4686301-rs2254298-rs13316193) also was not significantly associated with the severity of alexithymia. The results are shown in Tables 3, 4 and 5.

thumbnail
Table 3. The association between the OXTR functional SNPs and total TAS-20 scores divided by genotypes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143168.t003

thumbnail
Table 4. The association between the OXTR functional SNPs and TAS-20 subdimension scores divided by genotypes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143168.t004

thumbnail
Table 5. Results of haplotype-based quantitative trait-association analysis between TAS-20 scores and six SNP markers in the OXTR gene.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143168.t005

Discussion

Initially, we hypothesized that OXTR variants might account for the individual differences of alexithymic traits seen in OCD patients. However, contrary to our initial hypothesis, there were no differences of alexithymic traits according to alleles or genotypes of eight SNPs within OXTR. Haplotype analyses also did not show any association between various haplotypes of OXTR and alexithymia. Alexithymic individuals show impaired emotional experience, deficits of emotional interpretation and emotional face recognition, and lack of empathy [44] and are associated with various psychiatric conditions including autistic disorders and psychopathy [45, 46]. Although there have been no reports which directly investigated the influence of OXTR genetic variants on alexithymic traits, there are a number of studies of the association between OXTR polymorphisms and those various alexithymia-related traits (e.g., empathy, emotional facial recognition, and so forth) and psychiatric disorders (e.g., autism spectrum disorders, psychopathy, and so forth).

Recently, Laursen et al. found that subjects with the CC genotype at OXTR rs2268498 and AA genotype at OXTR rs53576 showed higher empathic accuracy [47]. Meanwhile, Uzefovsky et al. reported that the A allele of OXTR rs53576 predicted lower emotional empathy [48]. Melcher et al. found that T allele carriers of OXTR rs2268498 showed more accurate facial emotional recognition skill [49]. In addition, subjects with the A allele of OXTR rs2254298 showed deficient deactivation of the dorsal anterior cingulate gyrus during an emotional face matching task [50]. The SNP rs13316193 C allele of the OXTR gene has been associated with empathy [33], whereas the T allele has been linked to decreased expression of oxytocin receptors in the brain, depressive mood, and greater risk for autism spectrum disorder [19]. One recent meta-analysis study found significant association between autism spectrum disorder and the rs7632287, rs237887, rs2268491, and rs2254298 SNPs of OXTR [51]. Psychopathy has a higher incidence of alexithymic traits, and both psychopathy and alexithymia have common core features of a lack of empathy, insight, and introspection [52]. Several neuroimaging studies suggested that the genetic variants of OXTR modulate the activities of limbic circuits including the amygdala, the hypothalamus, and the cingulate gyrus [53], which are also associated with alexithymia [23].

However, not all of the studies consistently reported a positive association between OXTR variants and alexithymia-related traits. Nyffeler et al. did not find any associations between single SNPs of OXTR (rs2301261, rs53576, rs2144298, or rs2268494) and autism [54]. Tansey et al. reported no association between 18 SNPs of OXTR and samples of autistic individuals from Ireland, Portugal, and the United Kingdom [55]. Also, a meta-analyses study consisting of a large number of participants (N ≥ 17000 for rs53576 and N ≥ for rs2254298) failed to support the impact of these two OXTR gene variants on five domains of human functioning including biology, personality, social behavior, psychopathology, and autism [56].

However, the resulting lack of association in our study should be interpreted with caution because there are several factors, which are required to be considered. First, the ethnic and cultural backgrounds might influence the results of our study. In fact, several inconclusive results about autism have been reported from different ethnic and cultural backgrounds across studies. In Japanese [57] and Chinese [58] populations, the rs2254398 A allele of OXTR has been reported to be linked to autism. However, in Caucasian autism trios, the rs2254395 G allele of OXTR was overtransmitted to probands with autistic disorders [59]. In addition, as mentioned earlier, one study of a Caucasian sample found no influence of various SNPs of the OXTR gene on autism [55], although they did not include the rs2254398. We did not control the potential effects of undetected population stratification. Notwithstanding, the Korean population is considered to be much more ethnically and culturally homogenous than other populations, due to its distinct language and culture. Although the Koreans are assumed to be relatively free from stratification [60], there is still potential risk of biased results from undetected population stratification. Second, the participants in this study were all OCD patients, and the disease status of the sample might influence the results. For example, in schizophrenic patients, the A allele carriers of the rs2254298 SNP of OXTR had higher empathic concern than non-A allele carriers, whereas in healthy controls, this difference was not found [35]. Therefore, although patients with OCD tend to be highly alexithymic with a relatively large variability, which can increase statistical power, the results cannot be generalized to a non-clinical sample or to other psychiatric conditions. Third, we did not consider gene and environment interaction effects. However, there has been evidence suggesting an OXTR gene by environment interaction on various human traits [56] (e.g., emotional dysregulation and attachment style) or psychiatric diseases [61] (e.g., depression). Lastly, while our sample size was relatively larger than those in previous studies examining associations between OXTR SNPs and alexithymia-related conditions, our power is still limited.

When we simulated the power of our study (5,000 trial runs) using the JPT + CHB panel of a 1,000 genome database and phenotypic variance-covariance matrix estimated from this study, the power of the sample size in this study was only 0.342, and was sufficient for detecting only an effect size larger than 0.9, as its regards to an association between rs237885 (the lowest GEE p-value in this study) and three subdimension scores of TAS-20. However, the effect size of rs237885 in this study was only 0.454 (standardized regression coefficient, β), suggesting the possibility of type II errors. Therefore, to confirm our reported findings, a study with a much larger number of samples with stronger statistical power is needed.

Therefore, it would be necessary to evaluate the effects of OXTR variants on alexithymia with particular consideration towards those confounding factors and limitations.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. Dataset of all subjects used in this study.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143168.s001

(PDF)

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MJK SJK JIK KN. Performed the experiments: MJK SJK JIK. Analyzed the data: WK MJK SJK. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: WK MJK SJK JIK KN. Wrote the paper: MJK SJK.

References

  1. 1. Taylor GJ. Recent developments in alexithymia theory and research. Can J Psychiatry. 2000;45(2):134–42. pmid:10742872.
  2. 2. Taylor GJ, Bagby RM, Parker JDA. Disorders of affect regulation: alexithymia in medical and psychiatric illness. Cambridge; New York: Cambridge University Press; 1997. xxii, 359 p. p.
  3. 3. Bernhardt BC, Singer T. The neural basis of empathy. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2012;35:1–23. pmid:22715878.
  4. 4. Feldmanhall O, Dalgleish T, Mobbs D. Alexithymia decreases altruism in real social decisions. Cortex. 2013;49(3):899–904. pmid:23245426.
  5. 5. Constantinou E, Panayiotou G, Theodorou M. Emotion processing deficits in alexithymia and response to a depth of processing intervention. Biological psychology. 2014;103:212–22. pmid:25270514.
  6. 6. Picardi A, Fagnani C, Gigantesco A, Toccaceli V, Lega I, Stazi MA. Genetic influences on alexithymia and their relationship with depressive symptoms. Journal of psychosomatic research. 2011;71(4):256–63. pmid:21911104.
  7. 7. Jorgensen MM, Zachariae R, Skytthe A, Kyvik K. Genetic and environmental factors in alexithymia: a population-based study of 8,785 Danish twin pairs. Psychother Psychosom. 2007;76(6):369–75. pmid:17917473.
  8. 8. Rushton JP. Genetic and environmental contributions to pro-social attitudes: a twin study of social responsibility. Proc Biol Sci. 2004;271(1557):2583–5. Epub 2004/12/24. pmid:15615684; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPmc1691905.
  9. 9. Rushton JP, Fulker DW, Neale MC, Blizard RA, Eysenck HJ. Altruism and genetics. Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma). 1984;33(2):265–71. Epub 1984/01/01. pmid:6540960.
  10. 10. Heinrichs M, Domes G. Neuropeptides and social behaviour: effects of oxytocin and vasopressin in humans. Progress in brain research. 2008;170:337–50. pmid:18655894.
  11. 11. Insel TR. The challenge of translation in social neuroscience: a review of oxytocin, vasopressin, and affiliative behavior. Neuron. 2010;65(6):768–79. pmid:20346754; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2847497.
  12. 12. Neumann ID. Brain oxytocin: a key regulator of emotional and social behaviours in both females and males. Journal of neuroendocrinology. 2008;20(6):858–65. pmid:18601710.
  13. 13. Cochran DM, Fallon D, Hill M, Frazier JA. The role of oxytocin in psychiatric disorders: a review of biological and therapeutic research findings. Harvard review of psychiatry. 2013;21(5):219–47. pmid:24651556; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4120070.
  14. 14. Guastella AJ, Einfeld SL, Gray KM, Rinehart NJ, Tonge BJ, Lambert TJ, et al. Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biological psychiatry. 2010;67(7):692–4. pmid:19897177.
  15. 15. Domes G, Heinrichs M, Glascher J, Buchel C, Braus DF, Herpertz SC. Oxytocin attenuates amygdala responses to emotional faces regardless of valence. Biological psychiatry. 2007;62(10):1187–90. pmid:17617382.
  16. 16. Lane A, Luminet O, Rime B, Gross JJ, de Timary P, Mikolajczak M. Oxytocin increases willingness to socially share one's emotions. International journal of psychology: Journal international de psychologie. 2013;48(4):676–81. pmid:22554106.
  17. 17. Luminet O, Grynberg D, Ruzette N, Mikolajczak M. Personality-dependent effects of oxytocin: greater social benefits for high alexithymia scorers. Biological psychology. 2011;87(3):401–6. pmid:21641964.
  18. 18. Samur D, Tops M, Schlinkert C, Quirin M, Cuijpers P, Koole SL. Four decades of research on alexithymia: moving toward clinical applications. Frontiers in psychology. 2013;4:861. pmid:24312069; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3832802.
  19. 19. Lerer E, Levi S, Salomon S, Darvasi A, Yirmiya N, Ebstein RP. Association between the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and autism: relationship to Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales and cognition. Molecular psychiatry. 2008;13(10):980–8. pmid:17893705.
  20. 20. Montag C, Brockmann EM, Bayerl M, Rujescu D, Muller DJ, Gallinat J. Oxytocin and oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms and risk for schizophrenia: a case-control study. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2013;14(7):500–8. pmid:22651577.
  21. 21. Wermter AK, Kamp-Becker I, Hesse P, Schulte-Korne G, Strauch K, Remschmidt H. Evidence for the involvement of genetic variation in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) in the etiology of autistic disorders on high-functioning level. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2010;153B(2):629–39. pmid:19777562.
  22. 22. van der Velde J, Servaas MN, Goerlich KS, Bruggeman R, Horton P, Costafreda SG, et al. Neural correlates of alexithymia: a meta-analysis of emotion processing studies. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. 2013;37(8):1774–85. pmid:23886515.
  23. 23. Kano M, Fukudo S. The alexithymic brain: the neural pathways linking alexithymia to physical disorders. BioPsychoSocial medicine. 2013;7(1):1. pmid:23302233; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3563604.
  24. 24. Kang JI, Namkoong K, Yoo SW, Jhung K, Kim SJ. Abnormalities of emotional awareness and perception in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of affective disorders. 2012;141(2–3):286–93. Epub 2012/05/01. pmid:22542863.
  25. 25. De Berardis D, Campanella D, Gambi F, Sepede G, Salini G, Carano A, et al. Insight and alexithymia in adult outpatients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2005;255(5):350–8. pmid:15711867.
  26. 26. Roh D, Kim WJ, Kim CH. Alexithymia in obsessive-compulsive disorder: clinical correlates and symptom dimensions. The Journal of nervous and mental disease. 2011;199(9):690–5. pmid:21878784.
  27. 27. First MB, Spitzer R, L GM, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Clinical Version (SCID-CV). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Press, Inc.; 1996.
  28. 28. Bagby RM, Parker JD, Taylor GJ. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale—I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of psychosomatic research. 1994;38(1):23–32. pmid:8126686.
  29. 29. Seo SS, Chung US, Rim HD, Jeong SH. Reliability and validity of the 20-item toronto alexithymia scale in korean adolescents. Psychiatry investigation. 2009;6(3):173–9. pmid:20046392; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2796065.
  30. 30. Lee Y, Rim H, Lee J. Development and validation of a Korean version of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20K). J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc. 1996;35:888–99.
  31. 31. Goodman W, Rasmussen S, Price L, Mazure L, Heninger G, Charney D. Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS). Verhaltenstherapie. 1991;1(3):226–33.
  32. 32. Montgomery SA, Asberg M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. The British journal of psychiatry: the journal of mental science. 1979;134:382–9. pmid:444788.
  33. 33. Wu N, Li Z, Su Y. The association between oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism (OXTR) and trait empathy. Journal of affective disorders. 2012;138(3):468–72. pmid:22357335.
  34. 34. Poulin MJ, Holman EA, Buffone A. The neurogenetics of nice: receptor genes for oxytocin and vasopressin interact with threat to predict prosocial behavior. Psychol Sci. 2012;23(5):446–52. pmid:22457427.
  35. 35. Montag C, Brockmann EM, Lehmann A, Muller DJ, Rujescu D, Gallinat J. Association between oxytocin receptor gene polymorphisms and self-rated 'empathic concern' in schizophrenia. PloS one. 2012;7(12):e51882. Epub 2013/01/04. pmid:23284802; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPmc3527477.
  36. 36. Krueger F, Parasuraman R, Iyengar V, Thornburg M, Weel J, Lin M, et al. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation promotes human trust behavior. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;6:4. pmid:22347177; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3270329.
  37. 37. Brune M. Does the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) polymorphism (rs2254298) confer 'vulnerability' for psychopathology or 'differential susceptibility'? Insights from evolution. BMC Med. 2012;10:38. pmid:22510359; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3386011.
  38. 38. Montag C, Fiebach CJ, Kirsch P, Reuter M. Interaction of 5-HTTLPR and a variation on the oxytocin receptor gene influences negative emotionality. Biological psychiatry. 2011;69(6):601–3. pmid:21183159.
  39. 39. Kogan A, Saslow LR, Impett EA, Oveis C, Keltner D, Rodrigues Saturn S. Thin-slicing study of the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and the evaluation and expression of the prosocial disposition. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(48):19189–92. pmid:22084107; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3228468.
  40. 40. Park J, Willmott M, Vetuz G, Toye C, Kirley A, Hawi Z, et al. Evidence that genetic variation in the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene influences social cognition in ADHD. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2010;34(4):697–702. pmid:20347913.
  41. 41. Kim HS, Sherman DK, Sasaki JY, Xu J, Chu TQ, Ryu C, et al. Culture, distress, and oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) interact to influence emotional support seeking. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(36):15717–21. pmid:20724662; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2936623.
  42. 42. Rodrigues SM, Saslow LR, Garcia N, John OP, Keltner D. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation relates to empathy and stress reactivity in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(50):21437–41. pmid:19934046; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2795557.
  43. 43. Barrett JC, Fry B, Maller J, Daly MJ. Haploview: analysis and visualization of LD and haplotype maps. Bioinformatics. 2005;21(2):263–5. pmid:15297300.
  44. 44. Bird G, Cook R. Mixed emotions: the contribution of alexithymia to the emotional symptoms of autism. Translational psychiatry. 2013;3:e285. pmid:23880881; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3731793.
  45. 45. Berthoz S, Hill EL. The validity of using self-reports to assess emotion regulation abilities in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Eur Psychiatry. 2005;20(3):291–8. pmid:15935431.
  46. 46. Sayar K, Ebrinc S, Ak I. Alexithymia in patients with antisocial personality disorder in a military hospital setting. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci. 2001;38(2):81–7. pmid:11475919.
  47. 47. Laursen HR, Siebner HR, Haren T, Madsen K, Gronlund R, Hulme O, et al. Variation in the oxytocin receptor gene is associated with behavioral and neural correlates of empathic accuracy. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8:423. pmid:25538588; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4257152.
  48. 48. Uzefovsky F, Shalev I, Israel S, Edelman S, Raz Y, Mankuta D, et al. Oxytocin receptor and vasopressin receptor 1a genes are respectively associated with emotional and cognitive empathy. Hormones and behavior. 2015;67:60–5. pmid:25476609.
  49. 49. Melchers M, Montag C, Markett S, Reuter M. Relationship between oxytocin receptor genotype and recognition of facial emotion. Behavioral neuroscience. 2013;127(5):780–7. pmid:24128365.
  50. 50. Tost H, Kolachana B, Verchinski BA, Bilek E, Goldman AL, Mattay VS, et al. Neurogenetic effects of OXTR rs2254298 in the extended limbic system of healthy Caucasian adults. Biological psychiatry. 2011;70(9):e37–9; author reply e41-2. pmid:21872215.
  51. 51. LoParo D, Waldman ID. The oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is associated with autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analysis. Molecular psychiatry. 2014. pmid:25092245.
  52. 52. Haviland MG, Sonne JL, Kowert PA. Alexithymia and psychopathy: comparison and application of California Q-set Prototypes. Journal of personality assessment. 2004;82(3):306–16. pmid:15151806.
  53. 53. Kumsta R, Heinrichs M. Oxytocin, stress and social behavior: neurogenetics of the human oxytocin system. Current opinion in neurobiology. 2013;23(1):11–6. pmid:23040540.
  54. 54. Nyffeler J, Walitza S, Bobrowski E, Gundelfinger R, Grunblatt E. Association study in siblings and case-controls of serotonin- and oxytocin-related genes with high functioning autism. Journal of molecular psychiatry. 2014;2(1):1. pmid:25408912; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4223888.
  55. 55. Tansey KE, Brookes KJ, Hill MJ, Cochrane LE, Gill M, Skuse D, et al. Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) does not play a major role in the aetiology of autism: genetic and molecular studies. Neuroscience letters. 2010;474(3):163–7. Epub 2010/03/23. pmid:20303388.
  56. 56. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH. A sociability gene? Meta-analysis of oxytocin receptor genotype effects in humans. Psychiatric genetics. 2014;24(2):45–51. pmid:23921259.
  57. 57. Liu X, Kawamura Y, Shimada T, Otowa T, Koishi S, Sugiyama T, et al. Association of the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene polymorphisms with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in the Japanese population. Journal of human genetics. 2010;55(3):137–41. pmid:20094064.
  58. 58. Wu S, Jia M, Ruan Y, Liu J, Guo Y, Shuang M, et al. Positive association of the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) with autism in the Chinese Han population. Biological psychiatry. 2005;58(1):74–7. pmid:15992526.
  59. 59. Jacob S, Brune CW, Carter CS, Leventhal BL, Lord C, Cook EH Jr. Association of the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) in Caucasian children and adolescents with autism. Neuroscience letters. 2007;417(1):6–9. pmid:17383819; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2705963.
  60. 60. Lee JK, Kim HT, Cho SM, Kim KH, Jin HJ, Ryu GM, et al. Characterization of 458 single nucleotide polymorphisms of disease candidate genes in the Korean population. Journal of human genetics. 2003;48(5):213–6. pmid:12768436.
  61. 61. McQuaid RJ, McInnis OA, Stead JD, Matheson K, Anisman H. A paradoxical association of an oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism: early-life adversity and vulnerability to depression. Frontiers in neuroscience. 2013;7:128. pmid:23898235; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3721019.