Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Parasites and Pathogens of the Honeybee (Apis mellifera) and Their Influence on Inter-Colonial Transmission

  • Nadège Forfert ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Nadège Forfert, Myrsini E. Natsopoulou

    nadege.forfert@zoologie.uni-halle.de

    Affiliation Institute of Biology, Martin-Luther-University Halle-Wittenberg, Halle (Saale), Germany

  • Myrsini E. Natsopoulou ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Nadège Forfert, Myrsini E. Natsopoulou

    Affiliation Institute of Biology, Martin-Luther-University Halle-Wittenberg, Halle (Saale), Germany

  • Eva Frey,

    Affiliation University of Hohenheim, Apicultural State Institute, Stuttgart,Germany

  • Peter Rosenkranz,

    Affiliation University of Hohenheim, Apicultural State Institute, Stuttgart,Germany

  • Robert J. Paxton,

    Affiliation Institute of Biology, Martin-Luther-University Halle-Wittenberg, Halle (Saale), Germany

  • Robin F. A. Moritz

    Affiliations Institute of Biology, Martin-Luther-University Halle-Wittenberg, Halle (Saale), Germany, Department of Zoology and Entomology, University of Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa

Abstract

Pathogens and parasites may facilitate their transmission by manipulating host behavior. Honeybee pathogens and pests need to be transferred from one colony to another if they are to maintain themselves in a host population. Inter-colony transmission occurs typically through honeybee workers not returning to their home colony but entering a foreign colony (“drifting”). Pathogens might enhance drifting to enhance transmission to new colonies. We here report on the effects infection by ten honeybee viruses and Nosema spp., and Varroa mite infestation on honeybee drifting. Genotyping of workers collected from colonies allowed us to identify genuine drifted workers as well as source colonies sending out drifters in addition to sink colonies accepting them. We then used network analysis to determine patterns of drifting. Distance between colonies in the apiary was the major factor explaining 79% of drifting. None of the tested viruses or Nosema spp. were associated with the frequency of drifting. Only colony infestation with Varroa was associated with significantly enhanced drifting. More specifically, colonies with high Varroa infestation had a significantly enhanced acceptance of drifters, although they did not send out more drifting workers. Since Varroa-infested colonies show an enhanced attraction of drifting workers, and not only those infected with Varroa and its associated pathogens, infestation by Varroa may also facilitate the uptake of other pests and parasites.

Introduction

Host-parasite coevolutionary arms races are characterized by adaptations and counter-adaptations, including adaptive behavioral changes, to increase parasite fitness [1]. For example, parasites can alter host behavior to favor their transmission by increasing their contact rate with uninfected, susceptible hosts [1]. In contrast, hosts can modify their own behavior to limit the spread of parasites [1], resulting in classical host parasite arms races that are expected to be particularly dynamic in social insects [2]. Once inside the colony, transmission is typically swift among nestmates whereas transmission between colonies is often a major problem for the parasite.

The Western honeybee (Apis mellifera L.) represents an excellent model for studying infection-induced behavioral changes in pathogen and host because it can easily be experimentally manipulated [2]. In addition, the important ecosystem service of pollination that it provides through the pollination of crops and wild flora is nowadays threaten by the decline in the numbers of managed colonies in Europe and North America [3,4]. Drivers of colony losses are thought to include parasites, pathogens, pesticides and their interactions [58]. Thus, gaining information on factors influencing honeybee parasite and pathogen transmission may contribute to an understanding of colony losses.

Horizontal inter-colony transmission of honeybee pathogens can take various routes, including: (1) contact between infected individuals or infectious materials during robbing (robbing bees invade another colony to steal food resources); (2) contact between infected and uninfected individuals from different colonies during foraging; (3) contact with infectious material from the environment; and (4) ‘drifting’ of an infected bee from its own to another colony [9]. A suite of other factors in addition to pathogens is known to enhance drifting. In particular, artificially high colony density at the apiary, similarity in hive design, and apiary layout profoundly affect the drifting of bees [1012]. Thus, beekeepers have developed various techniques to reduce orientation errors of honeybees and facilitate their return to their home colony (e.g. spacing hives and using different hive entrance colors [1220].

Since orientation is essential for the successful return of foraging social insects to their colony, any interference in orientation and learning skills will enhance drifting [2123]. Indeed parasites (e.g. Varroa destructor and Nosema ceranae) have been shown to interfere with the homing ability of infested workers [2123]. However, an impaired homing ability alone is insufficient to address the potentially enhanced drifting of workers into other colonies, as workers not returning to the colony might simply die in the environment. Thus the acceptance of infected workers into foreign colonies needs to be confirmed in order to show the actual, field-realistic impact of drifting on transmission.

To address this question, we here use an experimental design that not only allows us to determine the effects of Nosema, Varroa and Varroa associated viruses on drifting behavior of workers, but also to identify colonies serving as sources and hosts of drifted workers. The specific aim of this study was to determine the extent to which Varroa associated viruses, Varroa or Nosema enhance their own intra-colonial transmission by favoring drifting behavior of their honeybee hosts.

Materials and Methods

Experimental apiaries

Two apiaries with 14 colonies each in Kenzingen (K) (48°11′30″N 7°46′6″E) and Simonswald (S) (48°6′1″N 8°3′21″E) in SW Germany (total N = 28 colonies) served as experimental sites. In both apiaries, colonies were placed in a line of 18m. We enhanced the probably of drifting by using hives with similar shape and color and flight entrance orientation in the same direction. These conditions may not be representative of all apiary conditions, since many beekeepers provide hive markings to decrease drifting [14,15,1719], but we needed a large sample size of drifted bees for conducting the experiment. Since the local orientation of workers is driven by local landmarks surrounding hive and the shape and color of a hive are more important for drifting than the linear distance between two hives [24], we present results from our analyses of the number of hives between colonies (hive sequence distance) rather than the linear distance between a pair of hives in our statistical analyses of drifting behavior. Biologically it seems more meaningful to use hive sequence distance because the linear distance would generate a false sense of precision less relevant to bee behavior. This was subsequently also reflected by a higher correlation between drifting and hive position rather than drifting and linear distance (see results). We note, however, that the apiary layout, with colonies distributed linearly, meant that ‘hive sequence distance’ and linear distance between a pair of colonies were themselves closely correlated.

In order to test for the impact of Varroa and its associated viruses on drifting, we used acaricide treatment to establish two groups of colonies, one group with a high and the other group with a low level of Varroa infestation. Initially, all colonies were treated before the experiment with two acaricides: CheckMite ® (active ingredient: 1.36 mg coumaphos; Bayer HealthCare AG, Leverkusen, Germany) and Bayvarol® (active ingredient: 4.0 mg flumethrin 90%; Bayer HealthCare AG) using one strip of each acaricide per hive box with brood to rid them of Varroa mites. On the 26th of July 2011, we stopped the ChekMite and the Bayvarol treatment in one half of the colonies per site but retained treatment for the other half of the colonies until December 2011. Hence, at each site there were two groups with seven colonies each, one with low Varroa mite infestation (i.e. with continued acaricide treatment) and another group of colonies with high Varroa mite infestation (i.e. with discontinued acaricide treatment). Comparing the two groups allowed us to test if Varroa infestation had any impact on inter-colony drifting and the spread of pathogens.

Sampling and genotyping

We aimed to collect an unbiased worker sample of both non-drifted and drifted workers from each colony, which we could genotype to differentiate between the two, drifted and non-drifted. Since workers can only drift once they fly and engage in tasks outside the hive, we sampled and genotyped old workers returning from foraging flights at the flight entrance. Only those passing guard bees and directly entering the hive on a straight flight path were sampled. We thereby avoided sampling foreign bees that might not have been accepted by the guard workers at the flight entrance and also workers on orientation flights that typically hover in front of the hive. Twelve (±1 s.e.) workers per colony (N = 328) were sampled on 26th September 2011 as described. At the same time, a piece of sealed brood (5x5cm) containing 12 pupae was cut from the comb of each colony, freeze killed in dry ice, and transferred to the laboratory, where it was stored at -80°C until genotyping. Using pupae allowed us to determine the colony’s natal bee genotypes. It also allowed us to determine the colony of origin of drifted bees.

Individual pupae were genotyped using two sets of tightly linked microsatellite loci on chromosomes 13 and 16 (Table 1), which allowed us to infer the queen genotypes of all sampled colonies. We used sets of linked loci to enhance the resolution of the markers [25]. DNA was extracted with a standard solvent extraction protocol (phenol/chloroform) and precipitated with ethanol. Extracted DNA was amplified by multiplex PCRs containing 10 ng DNA in 1μl DNA dilution buffer (Qiagen), 400 pM of each primer, 1.25x reaction buffer (Sigma), 200 μM of each dNTP, 1U of Taq-polymerase and HPLC water to a final volume of 10μl. The temperature profile for the PCR was as follows: 5 min denaturation at 95°C, 35 cycles of 30 sec each for denaturation (95°C), annealing Tm (see Table 1) and extension (72°C), followed by a final step of 5 min at 72°C. The amplified products were resolved in an automated DNA capillary sequencer (MegaBACE 1000, GE Healthcare Europe GmbH, Freiburg, Germany), including an internal size standard (ET-Rox 400, GE Healthcare Europe GmbH, Freiburg, Germany). Fragment sizes were analyzed using MegaBACE Fragment Profiler Version 1.2.

thumbnail
Table 1. Microsatellite markers for genotyping and detection of drifters.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0140337.t001

Detection of drifted workers

The DNA of the workers (N = 328) sampled at the flight entrance of each colony was extracted from a hind leg using a Chelex protocol [26]. The same genotyping procedures as for the brood were used. Natal and drifted workers were identified by comparing their genotypes with those of the colony’s queen, which was inferred from that of her diploid offspring (pupae). When an adult worker shared one of the queen’s alleles at both marker sets, it was considered to be the offspring of that queen. When an adult worker carried alleles different to those of the queen of that colony, it was considered to be a drifter. Its genotype was then compared to those of other queens of the apiary to find the matching one. In the few cases in which a drifter’s genotype matched with several queen genotypes (i.e. similar allele combinations at all loci of the two linkage groups), the genotypes of the siring males [27] of the respective queens were also used to infer the natal colony of that worker. Individuals with an allele combination that could not be assigned to any colony in the apiary were classified as drifters from “unknown” mother colonies.

Pathogen detection

We focused on a suite of common RNA viruses and the two microsporidian gut pathogens: Nosema ceranae and Nosema apis, as parasites that potentially impact drifting behavior. Total RNA was extracted with an RNeasy Mini Kit (Qiagen) using a Qiacube robot (Qiagen) from the same adult workers (N = 328) that had been sampled at the flight entrance and genotyped. The presence of 10 viruses and 2 Nosema spp. was determined in individual bees by employing Multiple Ligation-dependent Probe Amplification (MLPA) and RT-PCR respectively. MLPA was performed as described [28] using the probes designed for detecting six targets of the positive single strand RNA viruses: (i) Chronic Bee Paralysis Virus (CBPV), (ii) Deformed Wing Virus (DWV)/Varroa Destructor Virus-1 (VDV-1)/Kakugo Virus (KV), (iii) Acute Bee Paralysis Virus (ABPV)/Israeli Acute Paralysis Virus (IAPV)/ Kashmir Bee Virus (KBV), (iv) Black Queen Cell Virus (BQCV), (v) Slow Bee Paralysis Virus (SBPV), and (vi) Sacbrood Bee Virus (SBV), with β-actine (housekeeping gene) as a control for RNA extraction and PCR. Nosema spp. detection was done by RT-PCR on total cDNA synthesized with M-MLV Revertase (Promega) according to the manufacturer’s instructions and using 800 ng total RNA. Species-specific primers were used as described in vanEngelsdorp et al. (2009) [4] but with a modified annealing temperature of 54°C. Amplicons were electrophoretically separated and visualised in a QIAxcel instrument (Qiagen) and scored positive or negative using a threshold of 0.1 relative fluorescence units [29,30].

In addition to virus and Nosema spp. detection, we determined the Varroa infestation rate in a sample of ca. 150 worker bees per colony at the beginning of September, at the end of September and in mid-October [31]. We used the mean Varroa infestation determined over those three days of sampling in statistical analyses.

Network analysis

We used a network analysis to explore the flow of drifters between colonies. Each apiary was considered a separate network in which colonies were represented by “nodes” that were connected by “links”, referring to the number of drifters found in a non-natal colony [32]. Since any dispersal of workers was directional, with colonies receiving (sink colonies) and colonies sending drifters (source colonies), we generated a directional network in which we could differentiate between the outdegree centrality (high in a source colony) and the indegree centrality (high in a sink colony) as centrality indices for every colony [33]. Hence outdegree refers to the number of drifters sent from a given source colony, while indegree refers to the number of drifters received by a sink colony. These measures quantify how much a colony is central within the apiary in terms of sending and receiving drifters respectively. For example, indegree centrality may be driven by the scrutiny of guard bees screening incoming worker bees, or some other colony-based mechanism, but not necessarily by a trait of the individual drifting worker.

Statistical analyses

The Varroa mite infestation rate between the acaricide treated and untreated colonies was compared using a Mann-Whitney U-test. We used Chi2 tests to compare the proportion of drifters between both sites. To assess the relationship between drifting and the distance between sink and source colony in an apiary, we used the number of colonies between source and sink as the inter-hive distance; to do so, we conducted a Spearman's rank correlation between the number of drifted bees and the inter-hive distance; analyses were repeated using linear distance between two hives in place of inter-hive distance. The proportions of drifters between treated and untreated source and sink colonies were compared using a Chi2 test of homogeneity (all expected numbers >5). The level of infection by pathogens (viruses and Nosema spp.) between individual native and drifting bees of the same natal colonies was compared using a Fisher exact test (theoretical frequencies <5). Probabilities were adjusted for multiple comparisons using a Bonferroni post-hoc correction. Since workers were sampled after returning from a flight, the determination of individual mite infestation at take-off or during the flight could not be determined since mites may be lost during flight.

The outdegree centrality (OC) and indegree centrality (IC) for each colony at each site were computed using the software Visone 2.8. Since we wanted to discriminate between drifting that was a consequence of inter-hive distance and drifting that was a consequence of pathogens, we first determined the relationship between drifting and inter-hive distance using non-linear regression (Fig 1). Based on this function, we developed a network of expected drifting based on inter-hive distance alone to determine both OC and IC for each colony of each site. We then compared these expected OC and IC values with those of the network resulting from the observed drifting between colonies. We used the difference between the observed and expected values of OC and ICOC and ΔIC) to evaluate the effect of pathogens alone on drifting. Only parasites and pathogens that could potentially have affected drifting were included in this analysis. These comprised viruses and Nosema spp., which differed in prevalence between non-drifting and drifting bees, as well as Varroa, which differed in infestation across colonies through our experimental acaricide treatment. Since each site represents a unique network, we ran separate network analyses, one for each site. We used Mann-Whitney U-tests to assess if colony infection or Varroa infestation was associated with drifting. This approach allows for the comparison of both apiary networks in a single analysis. Statistical comparisons were conducted using R software version 3.1.1. [34].

thumbnail
Fig 1. Number of bees drifting from neighbouring colonies (N = 47).

The distance of “one” means that the bees came from the neighbouring colony, “two” from the colony next to the neighbouring colony and so on. The equation for the relationship is: y = a + b/x where a = 0.716 and b = 13.96.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0140337.g001

Ethics statement

Both localities were private and permission was given by local beekeepers to enter them and sample from colonies. In Germany, specific permission for setting up an apiary is not required if the owner of the location agrees to it. The only requirement is a document stating that colonies are free of American foulbrood, which had been issued by veterinarian authorities for our experimental colonies. Also specific permission is not required for the sampling of honeybees for the diagnosis and analysis of pests and bee diseases (i.e. Varroa, Nosema, viruses). Our experiments were exclusively performed with honeybees, insects that are not under particular protection or belong to endangered or protected species.

Results

Colony Varroa mite infestation

As expected from our experimental design, bees in colonies that were treated against Varroa were significantly less infested (0.17% ± 1.21 s.e. Varroa mites per 150 workers) than in untreated ones (5.17% ± 0.06 s.e. Varroa mites per 150 workers) (U-test, p-value = 10−6). The ‘untreated’ colonies (high Varroa) had a 30 times higher Varroa mite infestation than the continuously treated ones (low Varroa).

Detection of drifted workers

From the 328 adult worker honeybees genotyped and screened for pathogens, 253 workers were native bees in their natal colony and 75 drifters (with an average of 17 ± 4% per colony). Significantly more drifters were identified at the Simonswald apiary (55 out of 167 workers) than at Kenzingen (20 out of 161 workers) (Chi2 = 18.41, df = 1, p < 0.001). Twenty-eight drifted workers did not have a genotype corresponding to any of the queens and fathering drones at an apiary, thus their colony of origin could not be assigned.

Factors affecting drifting

Inter-hive distance.

We first determined the relationship between inter-hive distance at the apiary and drifting of workers (N = 47). We found a significant negative correlation between drifting and inter-hive distance (Spearman rank correlation: rho = -0.79; p = 0.006). The relationship between drifting and linear distance between a pair of hives was also significantly negative (rho = -0.55, p = 0.022), but weaker than for inter-hive distance so we used inter-hive distance in further analyses. We estimated the effect of inter-hive distance on drifting using a non-linear regression (Fig 1) as a best fit model (R2 = 0.85). We therefore corrected for hive position in subsequent analyses when testing for the impact of parasites and pathogens on drifting.

Individual infection

We tested if drifters were more infected by the various tested pathogens (viruses and Nosema spp.) than non-drifters of a given source colony. From our 28 colonies, 14 were a source of drifters. Within the 14 colonies, we compared pathogen prevalence in a total of 123 native workers and 47 workers that had drifted out of these colonies into others within the same apiary. Among the six virus families, three were not detected in our samples: ABPV/IAPV/KBV, SBPV and SBV. In total, 34% of the drifters and 39% of the natives carried at least one pathogen. Our samples were a snap-shot; native workers might also drift later in their lives. There was no significant difference in the proportion of infected drifters versus infected native bees, either for all pathogens combined (Fisher’s exact tests: p-value = 0.13) or for individual pathogens (Fisher’s exact tests: DWV-Family: p-value = 0.60; BQCV: p-value = 0.33; CBPV: p-value = 0.17; N. ceranae: p-value = 0.60, see Fig 2). Natives were significantly more infected by ≥2 pathogens than drifters (Fisher’s exact test: p-value = 0.026). However, after Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, differences were not significant (p-value ≥ 0.05). The minimum detectable prevalence in a sample size of n = 123 for natives and n = 47 for drifters is thus 1 out of 47 (ca. 6%; binomial error at p<0.05). This may lead to a slight inaccuracy in the estimation of pathogen prevalence and the effect of pathogens on drifting.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Infection of drifter and native bees.

To assess the effect of pathogens on drifting behavior, viruses and Nosema spp. infections were compared between the drifters and the native bees of the sink colonies (N = 14 colonies).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0140337.g002

Varroa infestation.

The majority of the drifters (68%) with an assigned colony of origin (N = 47) came from colonies with high mite infestation and significantly less (32%) from colonies with low mite infestation (Chi2 = 5.72, df = 1, p = 0.017). In addition, significantly more drifters were sampled from colonies with high Varroa mite infestation (Chi2 = 17.38, df = 1, p = 0.00003). However, drifting was strongly affected by hive position (Fig 1), which confounds the effect of Varroa alone on drifting. We therefore used a network approach to correct for inter-hive distance and extract the residual impact of Varroa infestation on drifting from the data set.

Network analysis

As only Varroa infestation–yet none of the screened pathogens–yielded a significant association with drifting, we focused the network analysis on Varroa with respect to outdegree (OC) and indegree centrality (IC). After correcting for inter-hive distance, we found that colonies with high Varroa infestation accepted significantly more drifters (ΔIC = 2.76) than those with low Varroa infestation (ΔIC = -2.76; U-test p = 0.036) (Fig 3 (A) and 3 (B)). The level of Varroa infestation of source colonies had a negative but non-significant effect on OC (high Varroa: ΔOC = 2.15, low Varroa: ΔOC = -2.15, U-test: p>0.05); in other words, there was a non-significant trend for high Varroa colonies to send out more drifters than low Varroa colonies.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Network map of colonies from Simonswald on accepting drifters according to their indegree centrality.

Here we represent the observed indegree centrality based on the actual number of drifters. Each square represents a colony (“+”high Varroa; “-” low Varroa) whilst the number refers to the colony position at the apiary, with +1 and -1 being the two central hives and +7 and -7 those at the two ends of the row. The arrows represent the flow of drifters and their width is proportional to the number of drifters (from 1 to 4) going from one colony to another. Only Simonswald network is represented since more drifters were found in this site than in Kenzingen, which illustrates better our results.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0140337.g003

Discussion

For a honeybee parasite, a crucial point in its life cycle is transmission between colonies [2]. A general strategy to enhance transmission is to manipulate host behavior so as to be vectored from one host to another [1]. We here studied the impact of Varroa mites, Varroa-associated viruses and Nosema on one of the major routes for honeybee pathogens to be transmitted between colonies, honeybee drifting [9]. Only Varroa mite enhanced drifting, with infested colony accepting more drifter than colonies with low Varroa infestation, while neither Varroa-associated viruses nor Nosema seem to have an impact on drifting behavior.

We conducted our experiment in an apicultural setting, where colony density is several orders of magnitude higher than in wild honeybee populations [3537]. In such a context, drifting of infected bees has been shown to be a major transmission pathway [1013]. Though the rate of drifting between colonies in natural populations is not known, it is suggested to be substantially lower than in managed apiaries [10,11]. As a consequence, it has been proposed that the lower level of infection (e.g. infestation by Varroa mites) of scattered and isolated feral honeybee colonies than those at apiaries may be driven by diminished drifting and inter-colony pathogen transmission in the latter [10,38,39].

Most beekeepers adopt techniques to minimize the impact of drifting on the health of their colonies [12,13,15,16,20]. We did not use hive markings enhancing visual cues for bee orientation as we aimed to generate many drifters to obtain satisfactory sample sizes. Yet it has been shown that specific hive colors and a more spaced apiary layout can only partly overcome the effects of the artificially high colony density at large commercial apiaries. The apiary as such is inevitably a prime facilitator of inter-colony drift [16,40].

We found significant differences in the frequency of drifted workers between our two experimental sites. However, since we had only two sites we can only speculate on potential reasons, which may include resource abundance and local landmarks. Yet it was not our aim to look at general environmental effects on drifting but rather to dissect those factors that are relevant at the within-apiary level.

The position of the hive in the apiary was most important and explained 79% of the variance in observed drifting, which primarily occurred among neighboring colonies. Hence efforts to prevent drifting by improved apiary layout or by providing extra landmark (e.g. unique entrance marks) may not just prevent drifting and the transport of Varroa in late summer but may reduce pathogen transmission among colonies throughout the season [11,31]. Packing colonies in tight rows in the apiary will lead to enhanced transmission.

None of the viruses found in our study (BQCV, the DWV-family and CBPV) or Nosema spp. showed significant associations with drifting. Since the Varroa treatment successfully allowed us to create two different groups of colonies with low and high Varroa infestation, we could more clearly infer the effect of this parasite on drifting (see S1 Table). Only infestation with V. destructor contributed significantly to drifting. However, this was not at the level of the individual worker. Since we only screened for drifted workers that had been accepted into a sink colony, any mites on these bees at sampling were neither informative about its original source colony nor about the infestation of the worker bee when it had actually drifted. Hence, the effects of Varroa we measured were at the colony level rather than at the level of the individually drifting worker.

The network analyses allowed us to correct for hive position and extract the effects of Varroa mites on drifting in more detail. On the one hand, drifting of bees is driven by the individual behavior of workers coming from source colonies. This can either be due to impaired orientation of workers returning from flights, or altered behavior induced by the pathogen or pest to enhance its inter-colony transmission [2]. On the other hand drifting also requires the host colony accepting the foreign worker. The network analyses strongly indicated that any effect of Varroa on the individual drifting bee is less important (a reduced but non-significant outdegree centrality of the low Varroa colonies) but rather operates at the level of the colony when accepting drifted workers (sink colony: significantly higher indegree centrality in high Varroa colonies).

Enhanced acceptance of drifters may have been due to an impaired ability to scrutinize incoming foreign workers by guard bees. Recently, Annoscia et al. (2015) [41] showed that Varroa infestation during the pupal stage alters the in-hive behavior of ensuing adult honeybees, resulting in reduced activity and participation in hive duties. If this were also true for guarding behavior, the higher acceptance of drifters by Varroa infested colonies may be linked to reduced guarding efficiency of resident workers. Thus, in light of our results it seems that Varroa may affect the colony at a global level or at least the behaviour of its guarding bees to favor the transmission of new incoming Varroa mites. Indeed it might be adaptive for the mites to enhance their population in the colony to escape its rigid reproductive system of inbreeding. This extreme inbreeding can only be avoided if a host pupa is infected with more than a single mite lineage, as seen in the Asian honeybee Apis cerana [42].

We cannot completely exclude a direct effect of the acaricide treatment with coumaphos and flumethrin on adult worker bees in the low Varroa colonies. Since these compounds have been reported to impair olfactory learning and memory [43,44], one might expect such colonies to send out more drifters because of their reduced orientations skills. Moreover, one might expect guard bees in these colonies, which identify incoming workers based on learned odor cues, to be less efficient. However, our results show exactly the opposite, which suggests that acaricide treatment as such had little, if any, effect on drifting. It seems unreasonable to assume that acaricides could enhance the ability of guard workers in rejecting foreign drifters from drifting into the low Varroa colonies, since acaricides impair and not enhance olfactory learning [43,44]. Also, the acaricides we used to generate low Varroa colonies did not promote workers drifting out of the colony into others.

However, we cannot exclude that the acaricides had a repellent effect, reducing the drifting of workers from other colonies into low Varroa colonies. In spite of this caveat, we consider repellence unlikely because both acaricides have low volatility and rather operate by contact. If active as repellent compounds, they would probably require contact between native and foreign workers. However, from an applied perspective, even if they repel drifting workers, it would represent an added benefit to acaricide treatment by reducing horizontal transmission of pests and associated pathogens among colonies. In any case, it is advantageous to keep Varroa infestation low in late summer not only to prevent reaching the Varroa damage threshold [45] but also to reduce horizontal transmission of pests and associated pathogens among colonies.

Krajl and Fuchs (2006) [21] reported on Varroa mites impairing homing efficiency of foragers. They released infested foragers at some distance from the hive and noted that these took longer to return home than non-infested ones, or did not return at all. The authors interpreted their results as an adaptive behavior of the bees to remove parasites or pathogens from the colony. In a subsequent study, Kralj and Fuchs (2010) [22] investigated the effect of Nosema spp. on the flight behavior of forager bees using a similar experimental paradigm and found similar results. Also Wolf et al. (2014) [46] reported that Nosema-infected bees failed to return home. The increased homing failure of Nosema-inoculated bees was explained though energetic stress induced by the infection leading to bees running out of energy on their path back home. If Varroa infested or Nosema infected returning bees simply disappear in the environment and do not return at all to any colony, then drifting would not be enhanced.

Whereas these former studies [21,22,46] could not draw firm conclusions on drifting per se since only impaired flight or homing failure were assessed, our study directly assessed drifting events; we found that drifters did not show higher Nosema infection than native bees. Hence, although Nosema interferes with flight behavior, orientation and the ability of bees to return to their home colony, we found no evidence that Nosema increases drifting of infected bees.

Varroa is not only a bee parasite but also acts as vector for several honeybee pathogens. Hence any increased transmission through drifting workers induced by Varroa or its associated pathogens would not only be beneficial to the mite itself but also to the pathogens it carries. In spite of this theoretical selective advantage, we were not able to detect such increased drifting induced by any of the Varroa-associated pathogens in our data set. Only, Varroa infestation at the colony level elevated the drifting of foreign workers into that colony. Although Varroa-associated pathogens did not show a direct effect on drifting of workers, Varroa infested colonies are nevertheless likely to acquire more pathogens since their probability of receiving drifters and hence also pathogens was greater than in colonies with low Varroa infestation. At the same time, pathogens are easily spread from bee to bee within the colony via Varroa as a vector. In addition Varroa has been hypothesized to serve as a replicator for pathogens in the colony [47]. These colonies will therefore eventually serve as sources of pathogens for healthy colonies, eventually spreading pathogens into neighbouring colonies and across the apiary.

Apiary layout and density, by facilitating inter-colonial transmission, uncouples the trade-off between virulence and transmission typically seen among pathogens and parasites [48]. The apiary provides an ideal ground for highly virulent pathogens such as DVW because transmission is also by Varroa parasitism, reducing the ability of a colony to stem inter-colony transmission.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. Viruses and Nosema infection, Varroa infestation and proportion of honeybee drifters (Apis mellifera) detected in acaricides treated or untreated colonies in two different apiaries sites, Kenzingen (K) and Simonswald (S), Germany.

At each apiary, seven colonies were treated against Varroa and seven were untreated. Honeybee foragers were sampled flying back to the hive after passing the guarding bees. Among them, individuals were identified as drifters. For some of them, their source colony could not be identified. The level of viruses and Nosema infections is based on the infection of the native foraging bees. Varroa infestation was determined from an independent sample of 150 in-hive bees.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0140337.s001

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Silvio Erler for assistance in colony sampling, Anja Miertsch and Vincent Maibach for laboratory assistance, and Panagiotis Theodorou for help in data analysis. We also thank the anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments, which helped to improve an earlier version of the manuscript. This study was supported by the Federal Ministry of Food, Agriculture and Consumer Protection (Germany): Fit Bee project (grant 511–06.01-28-1-71.007–10).

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: RJP RFAM PR EF MEN. Performed the experiments: EF MEN. Analyzed the data: MEN NF. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: NF EF MEN RJP RFAM PR. Wrote the paper: NF MEN RJP RFAM PR.

References

  1. 1. Combes C. L’art d’être parasite: Les associations du vivant.Paris: Flammarion; 2003.
  2. 2. Schmid-Hempel P. Parasites in social insects. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1998.
  3. 3. Moritz RFA, de Miranda J, Fries I, Le Conte Y, Neumann P, Paxton RJ. Research strategies to improve honey bee health in Europe. Apidologie. 2010; 41: 227–242.
  4. 4. Potts SG, Roberts SPM, Dean R, Marris G, Brown MA, Jones HR, et al. Declines of managed honey bees and beekeepers in Europe. J Apic Res. 2010; 49(1): 15–22.
  5. 5. Oldroyd BP. What's killing American honey bees? PLoS Biol. 2007; 5(6): e168. pmid:17564497
  6. 6. vanEngelsdorp D, Evans JD, Saegerman C, Mullin C, Haubruge E, Nguyen BK, et al. Colony Collapse Disorder: a descriptive study. PloS ONE. 2009; 4(8): e6481. pmid:19649264
  7. 7. Williams GR, Tarpy DR, vanEngelsdorp D, Chauzat MP, Cox-Foster DL, Delaplane KS, et al. Colony Collapse Disorder in context. Bioessays. 2010; 32: 845–846. pmid:20730842
  8. 8. Vanbergen AJ, the Insect Pollinators Initiative. Threats to an ecosystem service: pressures on pollinators. Front Ecol Environ. 2013; 11: 251–259.
  9. 9. Fries I, Camazine S. Implications of horizontal and vertical pathogen transmission for honey bee epidemiology. Apidologie. 2001; 32(3): 199–214.
  10. 10. Bailey L. Wild honeybees and disease. Bee world, 1958; 39: 93–95.
  11. 11. Jay SC, Warr D. Sun position as a possible factor in the disorientation of honeybees in the southern hemisphere. J Apic Res. 1984; 23: 143–147.
  12. 12. Free JB, Spencer-Booth Y. Further experiments on the drifting of honey-bees. J Agric Sci. 1961; 54: 153–158.
  13. 13. Betts AD. Drifting. Bee World. 1932; 13: 70–71.
  14. 14. Cooke VA. Drifting of bees: effect of hives placed in a circle. NZ J Agric. 1962; 105: 407.
  15. 15. Free JB. The drifting of honey-bees. J Agric Sci. 1958; 51: 294–306.
  16. 16. Jay SC. Drifting of honeybees in commercial apiaries. I. Effect of various environmental factors. J Apic Res. 1965; 4: 167–175.
  17. 17. Jay SC. Drifting of honeybees in commercial apiaries. III. Effect of apiary layout. J Apic Res. 1966; 5: 137–148.
  18. 18. Jay SC. Drifting of honeybees in commercial apiaries. IV. Further studies of the effect of apiary layout. J Apic Res. 1968; 7: 37–44.
  19. 19. Jay SC, Dixon D. Drifting behaviour and honey production of honeybee colonies maintained on pallets. J Apic Res. 1988; 27(4): 213–218.
  20. 20. Rauschmayer F. Das Verfliegen der Bienen und die optische Orientierung an Bienenstand. Archiv Bienenkunde. 1928; 9: 249–322.
  21. 21. Kralj J, Fuchs S. Parasitic mites influence flight duration and homing ability of infested Apis mellifera foragers. Apidologie. 2006; 37: 577–587.
  22. 22. Kralj J, Fuchs S. Nosema sp. influences flight behaviour of infected honey bee (Apis mellifera) foragers. Apidologie. 2010; 41: 21–28.
  23. 23. Dussaubat C, Maisonnasse A, Crauser D, Beslay D, Costagliola G, Soubeyrand S, et al. Flight behavior and pheromone changes associated to Nosema ceranae infection of honeybee workers (Apis mellifera) in field conditions. J Invertebr Pathol. 2013; 113: 42–51. pmid:23352958
  24. 24. Von Frisch K. The dance language and orientation of bees. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 1967.
  25. 25. Shaibi T, Lattorff HMG, Moritz RFA. A microsatellite DNA toolkit for studying population structure in Apis mellifera. Mol Ecol Resour. 2008; 8: 1034–1036. pmid:21585963
  26. 26. Walsh PS, Metzqer DA, Higuchi R. Chelex 100 as a medium for simple extraction of DNA for PCR-based typing from forensic material. Biotechniques. 1991; 10: 506–512. pmid:1867860
  27. 27. Estoup A, Solignac M, Cornuet JM. Precise assessment of the number of matings and of relatedness in honey bee colonies. Proc R SOC Lon. B Biol. 568 Sci. 1994; 258: 1–7.
  28. 28. De Smet L, Ravoet J, Wenseleers T, De Graaf DC. BeeDoctor, A versatile MLPA-based diagnostic tool for screening bee viruses. PLoS ONE. 2012; 7(10): e47953. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0047953. pmid:23144717
  29. 29. Sint D, Raso L, Kaufmann R, Traugott M. Optimizing methods for PCR-based analysis of predation. Mol. Ecol. Resour. 2011; 11: 795–801. pmid:21507208
  30. 30. Sint D, Raso L, Niederklapfer B, Kaufmann R, Traugott M. Group-specific multiplex PCR detection systems for the identification of flying insect prey. PLoS ONE. 2014; 9:
  31. 31. Frey E, Rosenkranz P. Autumn invasion rates of Varroa destructor (Mesostigmata: Varroidae) into honey bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae) colonies and the resulting increase in mite populations. J Econ Entomol. 2014; 107(2): 508–15. pmid:24772528
  32. 32. Wasserman S, Faust K. Social network analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1994.
  33. 33. Freeman LC. A set of measures of centrality based on betweenness. Sociom. 1977; 40: 35–41.
  34. 34. R Development Core Team (2008). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing,Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0, URL http://www.R-project.org.
  35. 35. Moritz RFA, Dietemann V, Crewe RM. Determining colony densities in wild honeybee populations (Apis mellifera) with linked microsatellite DNA markers, J Insect Conserv. 2007; 12: 455–459.
  36. 36. Hinson EM, Duncan M, Lim J, Arundel J, Oldroyd BP. The density of feral honey bee (Apis mellifera) colonies in South East Australia is greater in undisturbed than in disturbed habitats. Apidologie. 46(3): 403–413.
  37. 37. Jaffé R, Dietemann V, Allsopp MH, Costa C, Crewe RM, Dall’Olio R, De la Rúa P, El-Niweiri MAA, Fries I, Kezic N, Meusel MS, Paxton RJ, Shaibi T, Stolle E, Moritz RFA. Estimating the density of honeybee colonies across their natural range to fill the gap in pollinator decline censuses. Conserv Biol. 2010; 24(2): 583–593. pmid:19775273
  38. 38. Gonçalves LS, De Jong D, Nogueira RH. Infestation of feral honey bee colonies in Brazil by Varroa jacobsoni. Am Bee J. 1982; 122: 249–251.
  39. 39. Ratnieks FLW, Nowakowski J. Honey bee swarms accept bait hives contaminated with American foulbrood. Ecol Ent. 1989; 14: 475–478.
  40. 40. Pfeiffer KJ, Crailsheim K. Drifting of honeybees. Insect Soc. 1998; 45: 151–167.
  41. 41. Annoscia D, Del Piccolo F, Covre F, Nazzi F. Mite infestation during development alters the in-hive behaviour of adult honeybees. Apidologie. 2015; 46(3), 306–314.
  42. 42. Beaurepaire AL, Truong TA, Fajardo AC, Dinh TQ, Cervancia C, Moritz RF. Host Specificity in the Honeybee Parasitic Mite, Varroa spp. in Apis mellifera and Apis cerana. PloS ONE. 2015; 10(8), e0135103. pmid:26248192
  43. 43. Williamson SM, Wright GA. Exposure to multiple cholinergic pesticides impairs olfactory learning and memory in honeybees. J Exp Biol. 2013; 216, pp. 1799–1807. pmid:23393272
  44. 44. Tan K, Yang S, Wang Z, Menzel R. Effect of flumethrin on survival and olfactory learning in Honeybees. PLoS ONE. 2013; 8(6): e66295. pmid:23785490
  45. 45. Currie RW, Gatien P. Timing acaricide treatments to prevent Varroa destructor (Acari: Varroidae) from causing economic damage to honey bee colonies. Can Entomol. 2006; 138(2): 238–252.
  46. 46. Wolf S, McMahon DP, Lim KS, Pull CD, Clark SJ, Paxton RJ, et al. So near and yet so far: harmonic radar reveals reduced homing ability of Nosema infected honeybees. PLoS ONE. 2014; 9(8): e103989. pmid:25098331
  47. 47. Bowen-Walker PL, Martin SJ, Gunn A (1999) The transmission of Deformed Wing Virus between honeybees (Apis mellifera L.) by the ectoparasitic mite Varroa jacobsoni Oud. Journal of Invertebrate Pathology 73, 101–106. pmid:9878295
  48. 48. Schmid-Hempel P. Evolutionary parasitology: the integrate study of infections. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011.