Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Theory of Mind and Emotional Awareness in Chronic Somatoform Pain Patients

  • Matthias Zunhammer,

    Affiliation Neurologie, Lehrstuhl für Neuroimaging, Universitätsklinikum Essen, Hufelandstraße 55, 45147 Essen, Germany

  • Agnes Halski,

    Affiliation Klinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, Universitätsklinikum Regensburg, Universitätsstr. 84, 93053 Regensburg, Germany

  • Peter Eichhammer,

    Affiliation Klinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, Universitätsklinikum Regensburg, Universitätsstr. 84, 93053 Regensburg, Germany

  • Volker Busch

    volker.busch@medbo.de

    Affiliation Klinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, Universitätsklinikum Regensburg, Universitätsstr. 84, 93053 Regensburg, Germany

Abstract

Objective

The present study aimed at investigating whether chronic pain patients are impaired in Theory of Mind (ToM), or Emotional Awareness.

Methods

Thirty inpatients suffering from chronic somatoform pain, as well as thirty healthy controls matched for age, sex, and education were recruited. ToM abilities were measured using the Frith-Happé animation task, in which participants interpret video-clips depicting moving geometric forms that mimic social interactions. The responses given were scored for appropriateness and the degree of inferred intentionality according to established protocols. Emotional awareness was measured using the Levels of Emotional Awareness Scale (LEAS), for which participants provide written descriptions of feelings in imaginary emotional situations. Standardized scoring was performed to capture the number and quality of emotional terms used.

Results

Responses lengths were similar in both groups and for both tasks. Patients attained significantly lower intentionality but not appropriateness scores when interpreting ToM interactions. No significant group differences were found when interpreting goal directed interactions. Emotional awareness scores were significantly lower in patients compared to healthy controls.

Conclusions

Our results suggest that chronic pain patients are impaired in mentalizing and emotional awareness. Future studies are needed to determine whether these ToM and emotional awareness deficits contribute to the etiology of somatoform pain and whether addressing these deficits in therapeutic interventions can improve polymodal pain therapy.

Introduction

Theory of Mind (ToM), or mentalizing, is the ability to infer on mental states, such as intentions, beliefs, or emotions in others [1]. Two studies recently suggested that patients with somatoform disorders may be impaired in ToM [2,3]. Both studies used the Frith-Happé-Animation Task (FHAT) as a measure of ToM [1] and authors of both studies suggested that ToM-deficits might impair proper mental representation of emotions, in turn leading to somatoform symptoms.

The ability of patients with somatoform disorder to label and represent emotions has been investigated for several decades: A wealth of literature indicates that somatoform disorders are associated with alexithymia, the inability to identify and label emotions [4]. However, the validity of the main measure of alexithymia, the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS), has been questioned [5] and it has been proven difficult to establish a causal relationship between alexithymia and somatization in longitudinal studies [6,7]. By using the “Levels of Emotional Awareness Scale” (LEAS) [8], a performance based measure of emotional awareness, Subic-Wrana et al. could show that somatoform patients use fewer emotional terms than other psychiatric inpatients [9] and healthy controls [2]. Two other studies using the LEAS did not find such emotional awareness deficits when comparing somatoform patients with healthy controls [10] and patients with medically explained symptoms [3].

The aim of the present study was to follow up these interesting findings and to extend the existing knowledge by focusing on chronic somatoform pain, the most prevalent sub-form of somatization disorder [11]. Chronic somatoform pain is characterized by distressing but medically unexplained pain in combination with abnormal beliefs and behaviors regarding the pain, with a minimum duration of 6 months. We aimed at investigating ToM and emotional introspection in chronic pain patients in comparison to a healthy control group matched for sex, age, and education. We hypothesized that chronic pain patients would show lower ToM abilities, as measured by the FHAT and lower emotional awareness, as measured by the LEAS.

Methods

The present study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Regensburg (Approval Number: 08/132) and conforms to the Declaration of Helsinki [12]. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Subjects

Thirty inpatients of the Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy at the University of Regensburg were recruited. Main inclusion criterion was the diagnosis “Pain Disorder associated with Psychological Factors” according to DSM-IV Code 307.80. An experienced physician performed the diagnosis and the evaluation of exclusion criteria, using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (Axis I) [13].

Thirty healthy controls were recruited successively and matched for sex, age, and education. Exclusion criteria for both groups were: major internal or neurological disorders, organic and inflammatory pain disorders, schizophrenia, major depression, current use of sedative medication, and insufficient German language skills. Exclusion criteria were evaluated in a structured interview. For the control groups the short-version of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (Axis I) was used for screening, in order to exclude any mental disorders [13].

Material

The Frith-Happé-Animation-Takss.

The Frith-Happé-Animation task (FHAT) is a performance measure of ToM, based on animated video-clips of 34 to 45 seconds, in which two triangles exhibit the movement patterns of living beings [1]. Healthy individuals automatically identify objects showing such movements as intentional agents [14]. The FHAT therefore allows probing participant’s ability to recognize ToM in interpersonal interactions, while excluding most of the potentially confounding cues and contexts typically present in social settings. The FHAT has been successfully applied to detect impaired ToM abilities in autistic children [1] and depressive patients [15].

The FHAT comprises four video-clips, in which two triangles display interactions suggesting „Surprising“, „Coaxing“, „Mocking“, and „Seducing”behavior—all of which require ToM for correct interpretation (ToM-condition) [1]. As a control, the FHAT includes four further Goal-Directed video-clips (GD-condition), in which two triangles display interactions suggesting „Fighting“, „Following“, „Chasing“, and „Dancing“, i.e. behaviors which do not require higher-order mentalizing abilities for recognition [1]. After each video-clip, participants are asked to describe what they observed. The responses are recorded and subsequently scored on the FHAT-intentionality and the FHAT-appropriateness scale [16]. The FHAT-intentionality score reflects to which extent deliberate actions and intentions are ascribed to the triangles on a 5-point scale (for details see: [16]): A score of 0 is given for a description of non-deliberate action (“bouncing”, “moving around”), whereas a score of 5 is given for a description of a deliberate action aimed at affecting another’s mental state (“The blue triangle wanted to surprise the red one.”) [16]. The FHAT-appropriateness scale is a measure of how close the given response matches the content of the video, as intended by the designers [16]. A score of 0 is given for no answer, or “I don’t know”, whereas a score of 3 is given for a correct interpretation (see: [1]).

The Levels of Emotional Awareness Scale.

The Levels of Emotional Awareness Scale (LEAS) is a performance test [8], which comprises twenty scenes (vignettes), each describing an imaginary situation in which the participant interacts with a second person. Participants are asked to give a written description of the emotions the situation may invoke in them and in the second person involved [8]. Each response is rated on a five-point scale, corresponding to one of five hypothesized “levels of emotional awareness” [8,17]. The construct of emotional awareness is hereby defined as “type of cognitive processing which undergoes five levels of structural transformation along a cognitive-developmental sequence derived from an integration of theories of Piaget and Werner. The five levels of structural transformation are awareness of 1) bodily sensations, 2) the body in action, 3) individual feelings, 4) blends of feelings, and 5) blends of blends of feelings.” [18]. Put simple, the LEAS has been proposed to measure the “ability to identify and describe emotions” [19].

Additional clinical measures.

Symptoms in the patient group were characterized using the German Pain Questionnaire (“Deutscher Schmerzfragebogen”, GPQ) [20]. The GPQ, includes the SF-12, a questionnaire measure of quality of life [21]. Both patients and controls completed Beck’s Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) [22] to quantify depressive symptoms and the German 26-item version of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-26) [23] to quantify alexithymia.

Procedure

After study inclusion, each participant was administered one of the two LEAS half-forms (LEAS A, LEAS B) at random. Each half-form comprises ten of the twenty LEAS items [17]. Participants were free to choose the length and style of response and were instructed according to the German LEAS manual (Subic-Wrana, unpublished). Subsequently, each participant was shown the eight FHAT-animations as used by Abell et al. (2000). The GD and the ToM video-clips were presented in mixed order. After each video-clip participants were asked: “What just happened in the cartoon?” No feedback, or help was given by the experimenter, except for general encouragement [1]. Participants’ responses were recorded using a video camera. The procedure was practiced once before the actual test, using a video-clip with randomly moving triangles [1]. Hereafter, participants were administered the remaining questionnaires.

Analysis

Responses from both the LEAS and the FHAT were transcribed to written electronic form. The LEAS was scored according to the instructions and glossary for the German manual (Subic-Wrana, unpublished). FHAT-transcripts were scored on the “intentionality” and the “appropriateness” scale, according to [16].

Two investigators scored both the LEAS and the FHAT, independently: one (A.H.) had performed data acquisition and was not blinded in respect to the study group, whereas the other (M.Z.) was blinded to grouping conditions. Analyses and conclusions were based on the blinded rater’s score.

The within-subject scores for all GD and all ToM video-clips were pooled before further analysis. Mann-Whitney-U tests were used to determine potential group differences in all variables. Pearson’s correlation coefficient was used to as a measure of inter-rater reliability for both the LEAS and the FHAT. Word-counts for FHAT and LEAS responses were compared between groups and correlated with FHAT and LEAS scores to screen for potential motivational confounds.

Associations between LEAS, FHAT, TAS-26, BDI-II, current pain severity, and quality of life (SF-12), were tested using the non-parametric correlation coefficient Kendall’s tau-b. Means are reported ± SD if not stated otherwise. SPSS 21.0.0.0 for Mac was used for analysis.

Results

All data are available in comma-separated format as S1 File. Mean age was 50.2 ± 8.6 in the patient and 47.2 ± 8.9 years in the control group. In both groups, the proportion of females was 67%. In both groups, the proportion of low, middle, and high educational status was 50, 30, and 20%, respectively.

Table 1 provides description of patient characteristics as obtained with the GPQ.

FHAT

The FHAT scores obtained by the blinded and the un-blinded investigator were strongly correlated for intentionality (r = .82) and for appropriateness scores (r = .86), denoting an acceptable inter-rater reliability. Intentionality scores were found to be significantly lower in patients, compared to controls in the ToM-, but not the GD-condition (Table 2). No significant group differences for the appropriateness scores were found—neither for the ToM-, nor the GD-condition (Table 2). Both FHAT intentionality (r = .402, p = .002) and FHAT appropriateness (r = .343, p = .006) scores were significantly correlated with FHAT word count, yet, FHAT word count did not significantly differ between groups (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Results for performance measures and questionnaires.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0140016.t002

LEAS

The LEAS scores obtained by the blinded and the un-blinded investigator were highly correlated (r = .93), denoting a good inter-rater reliability. Patients had a significantly lower score on all LEAS sub-scales compared to controls; effect sizes were moderate (Table 2). LEAS total scores were significantly correlated with LEAS word count (r = .451, p = .001), but LEAS word count did not significantly differ between groups (Table 2). The scores obtained with the half-forms LEAS-A (30.6 ± 7.7) and LEAS-B (31.1 ± 6.2) did not differ significantly (U = 449.0, p = .63).

Measures of depression and alexithymia

TAS-26 and BDI-II results indicate, that patients rated themselves as more alexithymic and depressed than controls (Table 2). Patients and controls differed on the TAS-26 sub-scales “difficulty identifying feelings” and “difficulty describing feelings”, whereas no difference in “externally oriented thinking style” was found. The differences in TAS-26 and BDI-II scores between groups had a large effect size.

Associations between measures

Correlation analyses were used to explore the relationships between LEAS, FHAT, TAS-26, BDI-II, and clinical parameters within the patient group (see: Table 3). Quality of life, as measured by the to the SF-12, was associated positively with ToM intentionality-scores and negatively with TAS-alexithymia, BDI-depression, and VAS-ratings of clinical pain.

thumbnail
Table 3. Associations between measures of ToM, Emotional Awareness, and clinical parameters within the patient group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0140016.t003

Discussion

Further evidence for introspection deficits in chronic somatoform pain

The present results show that chronic somatoform pain patients make significantly fewer references to mental states when describing social interactions involving ToM. In contrast, patients did not differ significantly from controls when describing goal directed (i.e. non-ToM) interactions (FHAT-intentionality score, Table 2). For both types of social interaction, patients and controls did not differ significantly in their ability to correctly name the depicted interaction (FHAT-appropriateness score, Table 2). Our findings indicate that somatoform pain patients are subtly impaired in their mentalizing abilities. These results consolidate previous observations obtained in mixed somatoform samples [2,3].

Our study further replicates the finding that somatoform patients use significantly fewer words denoting emotional awareness when writing about emotional situations as compared to healthy controls (LEAS-results, Table 2) [2,9]. Together, these results indicate that chronic somatoform pain patients are impaired in inferring on mental states, as well as describing emotional states experienced by themselves and/or others. The fact that two previous studies did not find emotional awareness deficits in mixed somatoform samples using the LEAS [3,10] may be explained by differences in sample composition. For one, the two studies included different somatization sub-populations, such as, somatization disorder (ICD-10, F45.0) [10], somatoform autonomic dysfunction (ICD-10, F45.3) [10], somatoform pain (ICD-10, F45.4) [10], conversion disorder (DSM-IV, 300.11) [3,10], and functional somatic syndrome [3], while the present study focused on somatoform pain patients. Moreover, one of the studies [3] recruited outpatients in the US, while our sample consisted of in-patients in Germany. Therefore, systematic differences in socio-demographic features and disease severity may explain the observed differences in results.

Potential causes and clinical perspectives

An exploration of associations between measures of introspection (LEAS, FHAT, TAS-26) and clinical parameters (BDI-II, VAS-pain, SF-12) yielded mixed results. Quality of life in patients was found to be correlated with FHAT intentionality score (see: Table 3), indicating that mentalizing abilities are associated with patients’ well-being. Although depressive symptoms were more prevalent in patients (see: Table 2), depression severity was not significantly correlated with FHAT and LEAS scores (see: Table 3). The observed introspection deficits in somatoform pain patients may therefore not be mere epiphenomena of depression. Of note, LEAS scores were not found to be associated with any other measure. This result replicates previous findings showing that LEAS sores are largely independent of BDI and TAS scores [10,24].

No significant associations between chronic pain-severity and any other measure were found, including quality of life. Finally, both BDI-depression and TAS-alexithymia showed a significant negative association with quality of life, which adds evidence to the notion that psychopathological rather than somatic processes may play the central role in somatoform pain.

Taken together, these results indicate that ToM abilities might be relevant determinants of quality of life in somatoform pain and a worthy target for future studies. Quasi-experimental studies in which mentalizing skills are trained are necessary to determine whether these findings can be utilized clinically.

Sample

The present patient sample showed several characteristics typical for somatoform pain (see: Table 1). All patients suffered from pain although no adequate somatic explanation was detectable. Most patients reported chronic pain in more than one bodily domain, but also suffered from symptoms of depression. Most patients had a protracted medical history and consulted various practitioners, receiving various treatments. Accordingly, the use of analgesic and psychotropic medication in the patient group was omniprevalent. In contrast, the healthy control sample was free of chronic pain, as well as analgesic and psychotropic medication, by definition.

Limitations

The cross-sectional design and particularities of our samples are the main limitation of our present study. No causal inferences should be drawn from cross-sectional studies: The observed impairments in emotional introspection may affect the etiology and maintenance of somatoform pain, or—to the contrary—be a consequence of ongoing pain. Further, systematic group differences may explain the observed results. For example, the observed mentalizing and emotional awareness deficits may be explained by the fact that literally all patients were receiving analgesic and/or psychotropic medication at the time of testing. Accordingly, we cannot rule out that the observed introspection deficits in somatoform pain patients are side-effects of analgesic and/or psychotropic medication [25].

Of note, we found that LEAS and FHAT scores correlated significantly with the length of participants’ responses. These correlations indicate that participants’ motivation and/or individual differences in verbal and literal expression capabilities are confounding factors for both testing instruments. Future studies with representative sample-sizes and adequate control-measures are needed to exclude that these confounds jeopardize the validity of the LEAS and the FHAT. However, the risk for confound by these factors is low for the present study, since both groups reached a similar word count on both scales.

Conclusions

The present study provides evidence for deficits in mentalizing abilities and emotional awareness in chronic somatoform pain. Longitudinal studies are needed to determine whether these deficits play a causal or maintaining role in the etiology of the disease, or whether these deficits are epiphenomena related to ongoing pain and/or medication. Controlled trials are needed to examine whether therapeutic interventions tailored to improve patient’s introspection deficits provide additional therapeutic benefits when included in multimodal pain therapy.

Supporting Information

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: VB AH PE. Performed the experiments: AH. Analyzed the data: MZ. Wrote the paper: MZ AH PE VB.

References

  1. 1. Abell F, Happé F, Frith U. Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cogn Dev 2000;15: 1–16.
  2. 2. Subic-Wrana C, Beutel ME, Knebel A, Lane RD. Theory of mind and emotional awareness deficits in patients with somatoform disorders. Psychosom Med 2010;72: 404–411. pmid:20223925
  3. 3. Stonnington CM, Locke DEC, Hsu C-H, Ritenbaugh C, Lane RD. Somatization is associated with deficits in affective Theory of Mind. J Psychosom Res 2013;74: 479–485. pmid:23731744
  4. 4. De Gucht V, Heiser W. Alexithymia and somatisation: quantitative review of the literature. J Psychosom Res 2003;54: 425–434. pmid:12726898
  5. 5. Kooiman CG, Spinhoven P, Trijsburg RW. The assessment of alexithymia: a critical review of the literature and a psychometric study of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20. J Psychosom Res 2002;53: 1083–1090. pmid:12479990
  6. 6. Mattila AK, Kronholm E, Jula A, Salminen JK, Koivisto A-M, Mielonen R-L, et al. Alexithymia and somatization in general population. Psychosom Med 2008;70: 716–722. pmid:18596251
  7. 7. Zunhammer M, Eberle H, Eichhammer P, Busch V. Somatic symptoms evoked by exam stress in university students: the role of alexithymia, neuroticism, anxiety and depression. PLoS One 2013;8: e84911. pmid:24367700
  8. 8. Lane RD, Quinlan DM, Schwartz GE, Walker PA, Zeitlin SB. The Levels of Emotional Awareness Scale: a cognitive-developmental measure of emotion. J Pers Assess 1990; 55: 124–134. pmid:2231235
  9. 9. Subic-Wrana C, Bruder S, Thomas W, Lane RD, Köhle K. Emotional awareness deficits in inpatients of a psychosomatic ward: a comparison of two different measures of alexithymia. Psychosom Med 2005;67: 483–489. pmid:15911914
  10. 10. Waller E, Scheidt CE. Somatoform disorders as disorders of affect regulation: a study comparing the TAS-20 with non-self-report measures of alexithymia. J Psychosom Res 2004;57: 239–247. pmid:15507250
  11. 11. Lieb R, Zimmermann P, Friis RH, Höfler M, Tholen S, Wittchen H-U. The natural course of DSM-IV somatoform disorders and syndromes among adolescents and young adults: a prospective-longitudinal community study. Eur Psychiatry 2002;17: 321–331. pmid:12457742
  12. 12. 59th WMA General Assembly. Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. J Indian Med Assoc. 2009;107: 403–405. pmid:19886379
  13. 13. Wittchen H-U, Zaudig M, Fydrich T. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV (SKID-I). Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1997.
  14. 14. Scholl B, Tremoulet P. Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4: 299–309. pmid:10904254
  15. 15. Ladegaard N, Larsen ER, Videbech P, Lysaker PH. Higher-order social cognition in first-episode major depression. Psychiatry Res 2014;216: 37–43. pmid:24524945
  16. 16. Castelli F, Happé F, Frith U, Frith C. Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage 2000; 12: 314–325. pmid:10944414
  17. 17. Subic-Wrana C, Thomas W, Huber M, Köhle K. Levels of Emotional Awareness Scale (LEAS). Psychotherapeut 2001;46: 176–181.
  18. 18. Lane RD, Schwartz GE. Levels of emotional awareness: a cognitive-developmental theory and its application to psychopathology. Am J Psychiatry 1987;144: 133–143. pmid:3812780
  19. 19. Ciarrochi J, Caputi P, Mayer JD. The distinctiveness and utility of a measure of trait emotional awareness. Pers Individ Dif 2003;34: 1477–1490.
  20. 20. Nagel B, Gerbershagen HU, Lindena G, Pfingsten M. Development and evaluation of the multidimensional German pain questionnaire. Schmerz. 2002;16: 263–270. pmid:12192435
  21. 21. Gandek B, Ware JE, Aaronson NK, Apolone G, Bjorner JB, Brazier JE, et al. Cross-Validation of Item Selection and Scoring for the SF-12 Health Survey in Nine Countries: Results from the IQOLA Project. J Clin Epidemiol. 1998;51: 1171–1178. pmid:9817135
  22. 22. Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri W. Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories -IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. J Pers Assess. 1996;67: 588–597. pmid:8991972
  23. 23. Kupfer J, Brosig B, Brähler E. Toronto-Alexithymia-Scale-26. German Version (TAS-26). Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2001.
  24. 24. Lane RD, Carmichael C, Reis HT. Differentiation in the momentary rating of somatic symptoms covaries with trait emotional awareness in patients at risk for sudden cardiac death. Psychosom Med. 2011;73: 185–192. pmid:21257980
  25. 25. Price J, Cole V, Goodwin GM. Emotional side-effects of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors: qualitative study. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195: 211–217. pmid:19721109