Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

No Evidence of Association between Toxoplasma gondii Infection and Financial Risk Taking in Females

  • Lasha Lanchava ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Lasha Lanchava, Kyle Carlson

    Affiliation Center for Economic Research and Graduate Education and Economics Institute (CERGE-EI), Prague, Czech Republic

  • Kyle Carlson ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Lasha Lanchava, Kyle Carlson

    Affiliation Department of Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, United States of America

  • Blanka Šebánková ,

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, Charles University in Prague, Prague, Czech Republic

  • Jaroslav Flegr ,

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to this work.

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, Charles University in Prague, Prague, Czech Republic

  • Gideon Nave

    gnave@caltech.edu

    Affiliation Department of Computation & Neural Systems, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, United States of America

Abstract

Background

Past research linked Toxoplasma gondii (TG) infection in humans with neurological and mental disorders (e.g., schizophrenia, Alzheimer’s disease and attention disorders), irregularities of the dopaminergic and testosterone system, and increased likelihood of being involved in traffic accidents.

Methodology/Principal Findings

We test for an association between TG infection and financial decision-making (DM) using a case-control design in a sample of female Czech students (n = 79). We estimate each subject's risk attitude and loss aversion using an experimental economic task involving real monetary incentives. We find no significant evidence that either measure of decision-making is associated with TG infection.

Conclusion

We were unable to find evidence of an association between TG infection and financial decision-making in females.

Introduction

Accumulating evidence from the lab and field show that human decision-makers display a great level of individual heterogeneity in economic preferences [16]. Although the diversity of preferences is a well-documented phenomenon at the descriptive level, its roots are poorly understood. Accordingly, researchers seek to understand the origin of the observed variability and whether cultural, educational or biological factors underlie the heterogeneity of economic preferences [511]. In recent years, scholars have suggested that interplay of genetics and cultural influences is responsible for shaping personality traits and other economic preferences [1215]. Furthermore, biological factors such as circulating levels of the steroid hormones cortisol [16] and testosterone [1719], and individual differences in the functionality of neurotransmitter systems, e.g. dopamine and serotonin [2022] have been found to modulate decision-making (DM) under uncertainty.

A biological factor that might partially explain the heterogeneity of economic preferences and has so far been overlooked by researchers is the composition of parasitic microorganisms residing within the human body [23]. Evidence shows that parasites can cause significant behavioral changes in various species, from insects to mammals [2431], where a well-known ethological theory, the ‘behavioral manipulation hypothesis’ postulates that parasites specifically manipulate host behaviors so as to increase the fitness of the parasite [29,32]. For example, the parasite Toxoplasma gondii (TG), which is the subject of this study, must be transmitted from an intermediate host to the definite host, a feline predator, in order to sexually reproduce. Therefore, TG would increase its fitness by manipulating the intermediate hosts’ behavior so as to increase contact with felids.

We focus on TG for several reasons. First, TG has been shown to manipulate the behavior of some of its mammalian hosts such that the probability that a feline predator will capture them increases; for example, TG influences rodents’ vigilance and their ability to recognize novel stimuli, causes prolonged reaction times and even turns rodents’ innate fear of the cat odor into an attraction [3336]. While it is true that modern humans are rarely eaten by cats and therefore it is unlikely that TG-related changes in human behavior have an impact on predation risk, it is plausible that TG targets human brain tissues as a byproduct of its evolution in rodents. Humans’ immune responses to the latent infection may also induce behavioral changes. Furthermore, since in early stages of evolution apes constituted a significant share of the prey of feline predators, TG’s act on human behavior could also be an evolutionary remnant [37]. Finally, an estimated 30% of humans worldwide have been infected by TG, but this rate varies substantially between countries, making the parasite a candidate for generating behavioral variation between populations.

Although latent TG infection has been considered harmless for many years following its discovery (While latent infection was generally considered to be harmless, it is well-recognized that the acute TG infection can be harmful, especially for immunocompromised or fetuses [38]), studies over the past few decades have associated it with various psychiatric and neurological problems, including schizophrenia, Alzheimer’s disease, and attention disorders [37,3941]. Many of the behavioral effects of TG in rodents have been extended to humans. Thus, TG infected humans show prolonged response times in cognitive tasks and TG infection is suspected to underlie individual differences in various personality traits [26,42]. Furthermore, several independent studies associate TG infection with increased rates of traffic accidents, which might be mediated by impaired decision-making capabilities or changes in sensitivity to risk and losses [4345].

TG infection can cause functional changes in the dopaminergic system [37,4648], which plays a key role in DM and is particularly involved in shaping one’s attitudes towards risk and loss [20,22,49,50]. Moreover, the genome of TG contains two genes for the enzyme tyrosine hydroxylase, which is essential for dopamine synthesis [51]. Finally, TG is also associated with gender-specific individual differences in the circulating levels of the hormones testosterone and cortisol [52,53], which are correlated with individual differences in risk taking in economic DM [17,54]. Although causality has not been established in humans (due to methodological and ethical limitations) experiments on rodents shows that TG causes changes in testosterone and not vice-versa [55].

Given the growing evidence of behavioral and physiological effects of TG infection in humans, we hypothesized that TG-infection would have an influence on economic DM. The current case-controlled study used a well-established experimental task [56,57] to investigate individual differences in two components of financial DM, namely risk and loss aversion. Risk aversion is the tendency to prefer a guaranteed payoff to a gamble with equivalent expected value [58,59]. By and large, people are willing to pay a premium in order to reduce the variance of their choice outcomes, even when the riskier option has a greater expected value. Loss aversion is human’s tendency to overweight losses relative to gains [60,61], a phenomenon that has been repeatedly documented in lab experiments using both money and goods [6264] and in field data [6568]. A large body of literature reports that biological factors such as gender, genetic variability and physiological state (such as hormone levels) underlie individual differences in people’s attitudes toward risk and loss [17,54,56,6971]. Furthermore, loss aversion and risk aversion exist across species [72,73], making them candidate descriptive measures for decision parameters that might be affected by biological factors.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Seventy-nine female subjects (mean age, 23.89; S.D. 3.65) participated in the experiment. Participants were recruited from the pool of current and former biology students of the Faculty of Science, Charles University, Prague. All subjects were previously tested for toxoplasmosis and rhesus (RhD) status (see detailed methods below). All participants provided a written informed consent. Subjects’ recruitment and data handling were performed in compliance with the Czech legislation in force and were approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the Faculty of Science, Charles University and the Department of Humanities and Social Science, Caltech. The student volunteers often take part in many studies, most of which are not related to TG infection. Crucially, TG was not mentioned in the study recruitment invitation, and the experiments were run at the University of Economics—not the usual location for TG-related experiments. Therefore, it is unlikely that subjects suspected that the experiment was related to TG research. Moreover, no data or hypotheses concerning possible effects of TG on the risk-taking behavior had been published before the study started.

Experimental Procedures

The experimental sessions took place in November and December 2013 in the Laboratory of Experimental Economics (LEE) in Prague. Upon arrival, participants were randomly assigned to isolated computer stations where they could not interact with each other. Subjects received a printed copy of the instructions (see supporting information (SI)). After reading the instructions, subjects were asked to complete a quiz to ensure their understanding of the task. The experimenter (same person in all sessions) was responsible for verifying the correctness of their answers and in case of a wrong answer provided an explanation, until subjects fully understood the task. After all subjects had answered the quiz questions correctly, they proceeded to the task. The task was designed to explore subjects’ attitudes towards monetary risk and potential losses and was programmed in z-Tree [74] in English. After the completion of the task and an additional, unrelated task, participants filled out a questionnaire about their socio-economic background, health, personality, and general attitude towards risk. The experimental sessions lasted approximately 90 minutes and on average subjects earned 350 Czech korunas (CZK), equivalent to 17.5 USD according to the exchange rate at that time. After making their decisions, subjects were privately paid in cash.

Financial decision-making task

Subjects were endowed with 250 CZK and made a series of 140 forced choices between different pairs of monetary gambles (S3 Table in SI lists the entire set of pay-offs used). Each pair contained a sure option (SO) which paid S CZK for sure, and a risky option (RO), which delivered a gain of G CZK and a loss L CZK each with an equal probability of 50% (Fig 1). The task was used to measure risk attitudes and loss aversion in previous studies [56,57]. Choices were presented in four pseudo-randomly ordered blocks of 35 decisions, such that block order was counterbalanced across participants. Gamble positions (left or right) and gamble outcomes were randomized across participant. Subjects received no immediate feedback about the lottery outcomes and were informed that at the end of the experiment, one trial would be selected at random and the payoff associated with the selected option would be implemented. The payoffs and initial endowment of 250 CZK were such that no subject could end with negative earnings.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Risk task: A sample screenshot from the study.

The two numbers on the left represent the gamble’s possible gain and loss amounts (Top and Bottom, respectively). The number on the right represents the guaranteed amount. Participants had to indicate which option they wanted to choose by clicking the corresponding button.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136716.g001

Immunological Tests for Toxoplasmosis

Serological tests took place between 2006 and 2013 in the National Reference Diagnostic Laboratory for Toxoplasmosis, National Institute of Public Health, Prague. Specific IgG and IgM antibody titres were determined by ELISA (IgG: SEVAC, Prague, IgM: TestLine, Brno), optimized for early detection of acute toxoplasmosis [75] and by complement fixation tests (CFT, SEVAC, Prague) which are more sensitive and therefore more suitable for the detection of latent TG infection [76]. The titre of anti-TG antibodies in sera was measured in dilutions between 1:8 and 1:1024. The subjects with negative results of IgM ELISA (positivity index<0.9) and both CFT titres higher than 1:8 and IgG ELISA>250 optical units, i.e. approximately 10 IU/ml, were considered latent toxoplasmosis positive. Individuals with ambiguous diagnosis, e.g., different result of CFT and ELISA, were excluded from the study.

Six of the non-infected subjects were tested prior to 2010; nine subjects were tested between April 2010 and November 2010; seven subjects were screened from October 2011 to November 2011. The remaining 18 non-infected subjects were tested between March 2012 and April 2013. The seroprevalence of TG increases from 8% in women aged 12 to 19 to 15% in women aged 20 to 29 [77]; these numbers are comparable to the Czech Republic population [78]. As the age range of women in our study was 20 to 26 (mean 23.15; SD 1.64), and given that the non-TG women tested negative in prior years (12 to 19 years of age), it is safe to deduce that 7% is an upper bound for the likelihood that any of the control group subjects had contracted the parasite between the time of the serological examination and the behavioral task. All of the analysis replicates when we exclude the 6 subjects that were tested prior 2010 (see S5S7 Tables in supplementary material section).

RhD Examination

Several studies over the past years have that RhD phenotype moderates several effects of TG on human personality, reaction times and behavior [7982]: generally, RhD positive subject are highly resistant to TG related effects [83], where TG has opposite effects on RhD negative homozygotes and RhD positive heterozygotes [84]. Therefore, it is important to include RhD phenotype in the analysis as a covariate.

We used a standard agglutination method for RhD examination. A constant amount of anti-D serum (human monoclonal anti-D reagent; Seraclone, Immucor Gamma Inc.) was added to a drop of blood on white glass plate. Red cells of RhD-positive subjects were agglutinated within 2–5 minutes.

Results

Summary Statistics

We present summary statistics in Table 1. The experiment was a case-control study; thus, the prevalence of neither TG-infected nor RhD negative subjects reflected real frequencies in Czech population: According to a recent study [78] the prevalence of latent toxoplasmosis in women of childbearing age in the Czech Republic was 20% as of 2007. The rate of RhD negative subjects in the Caucasian population was about 16% [76]. Among 79 subjects, 39 were TG-infected and 40 were TG-free. There were about twice as many RhD positive subjects (N = 53) as RhD negataive ones (N = 26) and their distribution among TG-infected and TG-free subjects were identical. TG-infected subjects were slightly older (t(77) = -1.87, P<0.07).

Decision-making task

For each subject, we calculated a raw measure of risk attitude by calculating the frequency of risky choices (i.e., the proportion of trials in which the subject chose the risky option). Fig 2 shows the means of this variable in both TG-infected and TG-free subjects. The difference was not statistically significant (t(77) = 0.61, P>0.53). To further assess risk-seeking behavior, we examined subjects’ choices when the expected value of the gamble was less than the sure option; a two tailed t-test shows that the difference was not statistically significant (t(77) = -0.40, P>0.68, Fig 3, left panel). We also found no significant differences in the frequency of risky choices when the expected value of the gamble was more than the sure option (t(77) = 1.33, P>0.18, Fig 3, right panel). Additionally, we compared average response times (RTs) in seconds (per trial) across the two groups (see Fig 4). In accordance with previous studies reporting slower RTs in TG-infected subjects [42] the TG-infected group average RT was larger than the TG-free counterparts, though the difference was not statistically significant (t(77) = -1.10, P>0.27). We also found no differences in RTs between the two groups when separately examining choices of the risky option (t(77) = -1.62, P>0.10, Fig 5, left panel) and choices of the sure option (t(77) = -1.33, P>0.18, Fig 5, right panel).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Frequency of risky choices in Toxoplasma-infected and Toxoplasma-free subjects.

The graph shows arithmetic means, standard errors and a p-value.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136716.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Frequency of risky choices when the expected value of RO is less than SO (left) and the expected value of RO is greater than SO (right) in Toxoplasma-infected and Toxoplasma-free subjects.

The graph shows arithmetic means, standard errors and a p-value.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136716.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Decision response times (RT) in Toxoplasma-infected and Toxoplasma-free subjects.

The graph shows arithmetic means, standard errors and a p-value.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136716.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Decision response times (RT) when choosing RO (left) and SO (right) in Toxoplasma-infected and Toxoplasma-free subjects.

The graph shows arithmetic means, standard errors and a p-value.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136716.g005

We defined the variable incentive as the difference between expected value of the gamble and the sure amount and further analyzed the data using logistic regression with clustered standard errors at subject level. The binary outcome variable choice indicated (= 1) if the subject chose the risky option in a given trial. Column (1) in Table 2 reports the results of basic regression. As expected, we found a significant effect of incentive on participants’ decisions (X2(1, N = 79) = 138.14, P<0.01). Our results show no effect of TG-infection status on subjects’ DM under financial risk (X2(1, N = 79) = 0.38, P>0.53). In column (2) we added other explanatory variables (age, RhD status) to our basic regression. None of the new explanatory variables were statistically significant, and the magnitude and significance levels of the basic regression coefficients remained virtually unchanged. Next, we investigated the data using a model that also accounts for the variance of the risky option, by adding a variable representing the variance of the risky choice to the original regression. Columns (3) and (4) show the estimation results of the basic regression without interaction terms, with interaction terms, and with additional explanatory variables. Adding the variance did not affect the significance of the coefficients, and the results from columns (1) and (2) are not substantially changed.

We analyzed the decision RT data using a mixed-effects linear model. Column (1) in Table 3 reports the results of the basic regression. Across groups, choices of the risky option took considerably longer (z(77) = 10.55, P<0.01). Additionally, when facing higher monetary incentives to take risks, decisions were slower (z(77) = 6.31, P<0.01). There was no difference in decision times between TG-infected and TG-free groups (z(77) = 1.17, P>0.24). In column (2) of Table 3 we repeated the regression by including interaction terms. The Toxoplasma-Choice interaction term was positive but insignificant (z(75) = 1.16, P>0.24). As expected, we also observed a significant negative effect of choice x incentive interaction (z(75) = -10.18, P<0.01), implying that when deciding to take a risk, subjects made the decision faster when the risky option was more attractive. In column (3) in Table 3 we extend our analysis by including additional explanatory variables (age, RhD). None of the new controls have a significant effect on RT.

As a final step, we estimated a parametric model inspired by prospect-theory [60] for measuring individual levels of risk aversion (ρ) and loss aversion (λ) using maximum likelihood estimation, in a similar manner to recent studies that investigated individual differences in risk and loss aversion applying the same experimental task [56,57] (see S2 Appendix for detailed method. Table 4 reports summary statistics of the parametric data (S1 and S2 Figs illustrate the model fit). A two-tailed t-test revealed no statistically significant differences in the mean values of ρ and λ between the TG-infected and TG-free groups. We also performed OLS regression analysis with additional explanatory variables and found no significant effect of any explanatory variable on either risk aversion or loss aversion parameters (columns 1 and 2 of Table 5). In addition, we excluded parameter values that were more than two standard deviations away from mean (see S4 Table in SI for individual parameter estimates, nine extreme values were excluded in total). The number of excluded subjects was significantly lower in the uninfected group (2 out of 40) compared to the infected group (7 out of 39, p = 0.04). These subjects were excluded because their risk aversion (ρ) and/or loss aversion (λ) parameters were unusually high or unusually low. The results were robust to excluding these extreme parameter values in the analysis (see S1 and S2 Tables).

Discussion

Although latent TG-infection in humans has been considered harmless for many years following its discovery, recent studies suggest effects on human behavior. TG is associated with an increased risk of psychological disorder, changes in personality traits, prolonged response times in cognitive tasks, and a higher likelihood of being involved in a car accident [26, 37,3945]. TG is also linked to functional changes in the dopaminergic and testosterone systems [5153], both of which are known to shape one’s attitudes towards risks and losses and partly explain individual differences in economic preferences. In light of these findings and based on the well-established animal literature, the present study is the first to examine a potential link between latent TG infection and financial DM, namely risk and loss aversion, in humans. Using a case-controlled design and a well-established experimental task that was previously used to investigate individual differences in economic DM, we explore two hypotheses relating TG infection to risk-taking and loss aversion. While our study fails to find such a direct link, we acknowledge several limitations to our work.

Our sample is comparable in size to other DM studies that used the same experimental paradigm to explore the influence of psychological and biological factors on financial DM [56,57]. Given the costs of constructing a case-controlled study (e.g. conducting blood-draws and bio-analysis in a large sample of candidate participants), we find this sample size adequate. Yet, as this is the first attempt to test the effects of parasitic infection on human DM, we had no means of conducting detailed power analysis ex-ante. Given the moderate sample size, we cannot rule out very small effects. However, our results are inconsistent with any large effect of TG infection.

Based on recent studies reporting gender-specific effects of TG on several psychological and physiological variables [32,52], the current study used only female subjects. Future studies should further illuminate the relationship between TG-infection and DM in males. In addition, TG-infection effects on DM could be moderated by several hidden factors that are not measured in the current study due to methodological limitations. For example, the RhD factor has been suggested as a moderator that increases the psychological effects of TG infection [8588]. Although we have specifically addressed this variable by verifying that the same amount of Rh-positive and Rh-negative subjects were included in both experimental groups, and by controlling for RhD status in our analysis, the low prevalence of RhD-negative types in the population restricted our capability to recruit a large amount of such subjects. Furthermore, we recognize that few simple cause and effect relationships exist in nature; thus, future studies should further explore possible variables that might moderate the effects of TG on human DM, such as the time since infection, personality, levels of testosterone and cortisol, and genotype.

Finally, the current study focuses on decisions from description (that is, subjects were told about the probabilities and payoffs) rather than from experience. As people show different behavioral patterns in these two types of DM experiments [89,90], future studies should explore TG-related effects on decisions from experience. Likewise, we only investigated the link between TG infection and financial DM. Arguably, TG-infection on DM could be domain-specific (e.g., driving or health risks), or might influence decision parameters that were not explored in the current experimental setting and might be consistent with previous findings, such as under-weighting of low probabilities [9193].

Supporting Information

S1 Fig. Propensity to choose the RO as a function of its net expected utility (x-axis is truncated to (-25, 25).

The figure shows the proportion of risky choices across a range of net expected utility values. The net expected utility of each binary choice (faced by each subject) was calculated by subtracting the utility of the sure option from the expected utility of the risky option. Utilities were calculated according to each subject's estimated prospect theory parameters. The choices were grouped into bins of width 5 for the purpose of calculating the proportions of risky choices. The fitted curves were obtained by estimating logit models (one model for the pooled data from each group) where the probability of choosing the risky option depends only on the net expected utility.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136716.s004

(PNG)

S2 Fig. Propensity to choose the RO as a function of its net expected utility (x-axis is truncated to (-20, 20).

The figure shows the proportion of risky choices across a range of net expected utility values. The net expected utility of each binary choice (faced by each subject) was calculated by subtracting the utility of the sure option from the expected utility of the risky option. Utilities were calculated according to each subject's estimated prospect theory parameters. The choices were grouped into bins of width 5 for the purpose of calculating the proportions of risky choices. The fitted curves were obtained by estimating logit models (one model for the pooled data from each group) where the probability of choosing the risky option depends only on the net expected utility.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136716.s005

(PNG)

S1 Table. Summary statistics of the parametric data (full sample).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136716.s006

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Regression analysis of individual parameters (full sample).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136716.s007

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Binary choices used in the experiment as measured in CZK.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136716.s008

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Individual parameter estimates.

Values in bold were excluded from the analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136716.s009

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Logistic Regression.

Robustness analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136716.s010

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Mixed-effects linear regression.

Robustness analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136716.s011

(DOCX)

S7 Table. Regression analysis of individual parameters.

Robustness analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136716.s012

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Miroslav Zajicek, the director of the Laboratory of Experimental Economics (LEE) at The University of Economics, Prague (VSE).

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: LL KC JF GN. Performed the experiments: LL BS JF. Analyzed the data: LL GN. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: LL KC BS JF GN. Wrote the paper: LL KC JF GN.

References

  1. 1. Curcuru S, Heaton J, Lucas D, Moore D (2004) Heterogeneity and portfolio choice: Theory and evidence. Handbook of Financial Econometrics 1.
  2. 2. Dohmen T, Falk A, Huffman D, Sunde U, Schupp J, et al. (2011) Individual risk attitudes: Measurement, determinants, and behavioral consequences. Journal of the European Economic Association 9: 522–550.
  3. 3. Gächter S, Johnson EJ, Herrmann A (2010) Individual-level loss aversion in riskless and risky choices. CeDEx discussion paper series.
  4. 4. Guiso L, Haliassos M, Jappelli T (2002) Household portfolios: MIT press.
  5. 5. Callen M, Isaqzadeh M, Long JD, Sprenger C (2014) Violence and risk preference: Experimental evidence from Afghanistan. The American Economic Review 104: 123–148.
  6. 6. Bauer M, Cassar A, Chytilová J, Henrich J (2014) War’s enduring effects on the development of egalitarian motivations and in-group biases. Psychological science 25: 47–57. pmid:24220626
  7. 7. Benjamin DJ, Cesarini D, Chabris CF, Glaeser EL, Laibson DI, et al. (2012) The Promises and Pitfalls of Genoeconomics*. Annual Review of Economics 4: 627. pmid:23482589
  8. 8. Chen MK, Lakshminarayanan V, Santos LR (2006) How basic are behavioral biases? Evidence from capuchin monkey trading behavior. Journal of Political Economy 114: 517–537.
  9. 9. Glimcher PW, Fehr E (2013) Neuroeconomics: Decision making and the brain: Academic Press.
  10. 10. Henrich J, Boyd R, Bowles S, Camerer C, Fehr E, et al. (2004) Foundations of human sociality: Economic experiments and ethnographic evidence from fifteen small-scale societies: Oxford University Press.
  11. 11. Sapienza P, Zingales L, Guiso L (2006) Does culture affect economic outcomes?: National Bureau of Economic Research.
  12. 12. Feldman MW, Laland KN (1996) Gene-culture coevolutionary theory. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 11: 453–457.
  13. 13. Harris JR (1995) Where is the child's environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychological review 102: 458.
  14. 14. Dohmen T, Falk A, Huffman D, Sunde U (2012) The intergenerational transmission of risk and trust attitudes. The Review of Economic Studies 79: 645–677.
  15. 15. Bisin A, Verdier T (2005) Cultural transmission. The New Palgrave Dictionary of Economics.
  16. 16. Kandasamy N, Hardy B, Page L, Schaffner M, Graggaber J, et al. (2014) Cortisol shifts financial risk preferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111: 3608–3613.
  17. 17. Apicella CL, Dreber A, Campbell B, Gray PB, Hoffman M, et al. (2008) Testosterone and financial risk preferences. Evolution and Human Behavior 29: 384–390.
  18. 18. Apicella CL, Dreber A, Mollerstrom J (2014) Salivary testosterone change following monetary wins and losses predicts future financial risk-taking. Psychoneuroendocrinology 39: 58–64. pmid:24275004
  19. 19. Mehta PH, Josephs RA (2006) Testosterone change after losing predicts the decision to compete again. Hormones and Behavior 50: 684–692. pmid:16928375
  20. 20. Campbell-Meiklejohn D, Wakeley J, Herbert V, Cook J, Scollo P, et al. (2010) Serotonin and dopamine play complementary roles in gambling to recover losses. Neuropsychopharmacology 36: 402–410. pmid:20980990
  21. 21. Norbury A, Manohar S, Rogers RD, Husain M (2013) Dopamine modulates risk-taking as a function of baseline sensation-seeking trait. The Journal of Neuroscience 33: 12982–12986. pmid:23926253
  22. 22. Onge JRS, Chiu YC, Floresco SB (2010) Differential effects of dopaminergic manipulations on risky choice. Psychopharmacology 211: 209–221. pmid:20495787
  23. 23. Kramer P, & Bressan P (2015) Humans as Superorganisms How Microbes, Viruses, Imprinted Genes, and Other Selfish Entities Shape Our Behavior. Perspectives on Psychological Science,10(4), 464–481. pmid:26177948
  24. 24. Adamo SA (2003) Modulating the modulators: parasites, neuromodulators and host behavioral change. Brain, behavior and evolution 60: 370–377.
  25. 25. Fjerdingstad EJ, Gertsch PJ, Keller L (2002) Why do some social insect queens mate with several males? testing the sex‐ratio manipulation hypothesis in Lasius niger. Evolution 56: 553–562. pmid:11989685
  26. 26. Flegr J (2010) Influence of latent toxoplasmosis on the phenotype of intermediate hosts. Folia Parasitol (Praha) 57: 81–87.
  27. 27. Klein SL (2003) Parasite manipulation of the proximate mechanisms that mediate social behavior in vertebrates. Physiology & behavior 79: 441–449.
  28. 28. Libersat F, Gal R (2013) What can parasitoid wasps teach us about decision-making in insects? The Journal of experimental biology 216: 47–55. pmid:23225867
  29. 29. Moore J, Gotelli N, Barnard C, Behnke J (1990) A phylogenetic perspective on the evolution of altered host behaviours: A critical look at the manipulation hypothesis. Parasitism and host behaviour: 193–233.
  30. 30. Poulin R (1994) The evolution of parasite manipulation of host behaviour: a theoretical analysis. Parasitology 109: S109–S118. pmid:7854845
  31. 31. Poulin R (2010) Parasite manipulation of host behavior: an update and frequently asked questions. Advances in the Study of Behavior 41: 151–186.
  32. 32. Schmid-Hempel P (2011) Evolutionary parasitology: the integrated study of infections, immunology, ecology, and genetics: Oxford University Press New York.
  33. 33. Berdoy M, Webster JP, Macdonald D (2000) Fatal attraction in rats infected with Toxoplasma gondii. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 267: 1591–1594. pmid:11007336
  34. 34. Vyas A, Kim S- K, Giacomini N, Boothroyd JC, Sapolsky RM (2007) Behavioral changes induced by Toxoplasma infection of rodents are highly specific to aversion of cat odors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104: 6442–6447.
  35. 35. Vyas A, Sapolsky R (2010) Manipulation of host behaviour by Toxoplasma gondii: what is the minimum a proposed proximate mechanism should explain? Folia parasitologica 57: 88–94. pmid:20608470
  36. 36. Webster JP, McConkey GA (2010) Toxoplasma gondii-altered host behaviour: clues as to mechanism of action. Folia parasitologica 57: 95–104. pmid:20608471
  37. 37. Flegr J (2013) How and why Toxoplasma makes us crazy. Trends in parasitology 29: 156–163. pmid:23433494
  38. 38. Jones JL, Kruszon-Moran D, Wilson M, McQuillan G, Navin T, et al. (2001) Toxoplasma gondii infection in the United States: seroprevalence and risk factors. American journal of epidemiology 154: 357–365. pmid:11495859
  39. 39. Miman O, Mutlu EA, Ozcan O, Atambay M, Karlidag R, et al. (2010) Is there any role of Toxoplasma gondii in the etiology of obsessive—compulsive disorder? Psychiatry research 177: 263–265. pmid:20106536
  40. 40. Pearce BD, Kruszon-Moran D, Jones JL (2012) The Relationship Between Toxoplasma Gondii Infection and Mood Disorders in the Third National Health and Nutrition Survey. Biological psychiatry 72: 290–295. pmid:22325983
  41. 41. Torrey EF, Bartko JJ, Lun Z- R, Yolken RH (2007) Antibodies to Toxoplasma gondii in patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophrenia bulletin 33: 729–736. pmid:17085743
  42. 42. Havlicek J, Gašová Z, Smith AP, Zvára K, Flegr J (2001) Decrease of psychomotor performance in subjects with latent ‘asymptomatic’toxoplasmosis. Parasitology 122: 515–520. pmid:11393824
  43. 43. Flegr J, Havlícek J, Kodym P, Malý M, Smahel Z (2002) Increased risk of traffic accidents in subjects with latent toxoplasmosis: a retrospective case-control study. BMC infectious diseases 2: 11. pmid:12095427
  44. 44. Kocazeybek B, Oner YA, Turksoy R, Babur C, Cakan H, et al. (2009) Higher prevalence of toxoplasmosis in victims of traffic accidents suggest increased risk of traffic accident in Toxoplasma-infected inhabitants of Istanbul and its suburbs. Forensic science international 187: 103–108. pmid:19356869
  45. 45. Yereli K, Balcioğlu IC, Özbilgin A (2006) Is Toxoplasma gondii a potential risk for traffic accidents in Turkey? Forensic science international 163: 34–37. pmid:16332418
  46. 46. Berenreiterová M, Flegr J, Kuběna AA, Němec P (2011) The distribution of Toxoplasma gondii cysts in the brain of a mouse with latent toxoplasmosis: implications for the behavioral manipulation hypothesis. PLoS One 6: e28925. pmid:22194951
  47. 47. Prandovszky E, Gaskell E, Martin H, Dubey J, Webster JP, et al. (2011) The neurotropic parasite Toxoplasma gondii increases dopamine metabolism. PLoS One 6: e23866. pmid:21957440
  48. 48. Hodková H, Kodym P, Flegr J (2007) Poorer results of mice with latent toxoplasmosis in learning tests: impaired learning processes or the novelty discrimination mechanism? Parasitology 134: 1329–1337. pmid:17445326
  49. 49. Eisenegger C, Knoch D, Ebstein RP, Gianotti LR, Sándor PS, et al. (2010) Dopamine receptor D4 polymorphism predicts the effect of L-DOPA on gambling behavior. Biological psychiatry 67: 702–706. pmid:19914604
  50. 50. Driver-Dunckley E, Samanta J, Stacy M (2003) Pathological gambling associated with dopamine agonist therapy in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 61: 422–423. pmid:12913220
  51. 51. Gaskell EA, Smith JE, Pinney JW, Westhead DR, McConkey GA (2009) A unique dual activity amino acid hydroxylase in Toxoplasma gondii. PLoS One 4: e4801. pmid:19277211
  52. 52. Flegr J, Lindová J, Kodym P (2008) Sex-dependent toxoplasmosis-associated differences in testosterone concentration in humans. Parasitology 135: 427–431. pmid:18205984
  53. 53. Shirbazou SS, Abasian L, Talebi Meymand F (2011) Effects of Toxoplasma gondii infection on plasma testosterone and cortisol level and stress index on patients referred to Sina hospital, Tehran. Jundishapur Journal of Microbiology 4: 167–173.
  54. 54. Coates JM, Herbert J (2008) Endogenous steroids and financial risk taking on a London trading floor. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences 105: 6167–6172.
  55. 55. Kaňková Š, Kodym P, Flegr J (2011) Direct evidence of Toxoplasma-induced changes in serum testosterone in mice. Experimental parasitology 128: 181–183. pmid:21458453
  56. 56. Frydman C, Camerer C, Bossaerts P, Rangel A (2011) MAOA-L carriers are better at making optimal financial decisions under risk. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 278: 2053–2059. pmid:21147794
  57. 57. Sokol-Hessner P, Hsu M, Curley NG, Delgado MR, Camerer CF, et al. (2009) Thinking like a trader selectively reduces individuals' loss aversion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106: 5035–5040.
  58. 58. Epstein LG, Zin SE (1991) Substitution, risk aversion, and the temporal behavior of consumption and asset returns: An empirical analysis. Journal of Political Economy: 263–286.
  59. 59. Pratt JW (1964) Risk aversion in the small and in the large. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society: 122–136.
  60. 60. Kahneman D, Tversky A (1979) Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society: 263–291.
  61. 61. Tversky A, Kahneman D (1991) Loss aversion in riskless choice: A reference-dependent model. The Quarterly Journal of Economics: 1039–1061.
  62. 62. Gneezy U, Kapteyn A, Potters J (2003) Evaluation periods and asset prices in a market experiment. The Journal of Finance 58: 821–838.
  63. 63. Kahneman D, Knetsch JL, Thaler RH (1990) Experimental tests of the endowment effect and the Coase theorem. Journal of political Economy: 1325–1348.
  64. 64. Thaler RH, Tversky A, Kahneman D, Schwartz A (1997) The effect of myopia and loss aversion on risk taking: An experimental test. The Quarterly Journal of Economics: 647–661.
  65. 65. Benartzi S, Thaler RH (1993) Myopic loss aversion and the equity premium puzzle. National Bureau of Economic Research.
  66. 66. Camerer C, Babcock L, Loewenstein G, Thaler R (1997) Labor supply of New York City cabdrivers: One day at a time. The Quarterly Journal of Economics: 407–441.
  67. 67. Genesove D, Mayer C (2001) Loss aversion and seller behavior: Evidence from the housing market. National bureau of economic research.
  68. 68. Hardie BG, Johnson EJ, Fader PS (1993) Modeling loss aversion and reference dependence effects on brand choice. Marketing Science 12: 378–394.
  69. 69. Croson R, Gneezy U (2009) Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic literature: 448–474.
  70. 70. Porcelli AJ, Delgado MR (2009) Acute stress modulates risk taking in financial decision making. Psychological Science 20: 278–283. pmid:19207694
  71. 71. Slovic P, Finucane ML, Peters E, MacGregor DG (2004) Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: Some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk analysis 24: 311–322. pmid:15078302
  72. 72. Hamm SL, Shettleworth SJ (1987) Risk aversion in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 13: 376.
  73. 73. Kagel JH, MacDonald DN, Battalio RC, White S, Green L (1986) Risk aversion in rats (Rattus norvegicus) under varying levels of resource availability. Journal of comparative psychology 100: 95.
  74. 74. Fischbacher U (2007) z-Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental economics 10: 171–178.
  75. 75. Pokorny J, Fruhbauer Z, Polednakova S, Sykora J, Zastera M, Fialova D (1989) Stanoveni antitoxoplasmickych protilatek IgG metodou ELISA (Assessment of antitoxoplasmatic IgG antibodies with the ELISA method). Ceskoslovenská Epidemiologie 38:355–361.
  76. 76. Warren J, Sabin AB (1942) The complement fixation reaction in toxoplasmic infection. Experimental Biology and Medicine 51: 11–14.
  77. 77. Jones J. L., Kruszon-Moran D., Wilson M., McQuillan G., Navin T., & McAuley J. B. Toxoplasma gondii infection in the United States: seroprevalence and risk factors. American journal of epidemiology, 154(4) (2001): 357–365.
  78. 78. Flegr J., Prandota J., Sovičková M., & Israili Z. H. Toxoplasmosis—a global threat. Correlation of latent toxoplasmosis with specific disease burden in a set of 88 countries. PloS ONE 9(3) (2014): e90203. pmid:24662942
  79. 79. Flegr J., Preiss M., Klose J. “Toxoplasmosis-associated difference in intelligence and personality in men depends on their Rhesus blood group but not ABO blood group.” PLoS ONE 8(4) (2013): e61272.
  80. 80. Flegr J., Geryk J., Volný J., Klose J., Černochová D. “Rhesus factor modulation of effects of smoking and age on psychomotor performance, intelligence, personality profile, and health in Czech soldiers”. PLoS ONE 7(11) (2012): e49478.
  81. 81. Holub D., Bankovská M., Dragomirecká E., Rodriguez M., Preiss M., Novák T., Čermák J., Horáček J., Libiger J., Flegr J. “Possible protective function of Rh factor in schizophrenia”. Psychiatrie 15 (2011): 37–42.
  82. 82. Flegr J., Novotná M., Fialová A., Kolbeková P., Gasová Z. “The influence of RhD phenotype on toxoplasmosis- andage-associated changes in personality profile of blood donors”. Folia Parasitologica 57 (2010): 143–150.
  83. 83. Kanková S., Sulc J., Flegr J. “Increased pregnancy weight gain in women with latent toxoplasmosis and RhD-positivity protection against this effect”. Parasitology, 137 (2010): 1773–1779.
  84. 84. Novotná M., Havlíček J., Smith A. P., Kolbeková P., Skallová A., Klose J., & Flegr J. Toxoplasma and reaction time: role of toxoplasmosis in the origin, preservation and geographical distribution of Rh blood group polymorphism. Parasitology, 135 (2008): 1253–1261.
  85. 85. Flegr J, Novotná M, Fialová A, Kolbeková P, Gašová Z (2010) The influence of RhD phenotype on toxoplasmosis-and age-associated changes in personality profile of blood donors. Folia Parasitol 57: 143–150. pmid:20608477
  86. 86. Flegr J, Klose J, Novotná M, Berenreitterová M, Havlíček J (2009) Increased incidence of traffic accidents in Toxoplasma-infected military drivers and protective effect RhD molecule revealed by a large-scale prospective cohort study. BMC infectious diseases 9: 72. pmid:19470165
  87. 87. Flegr J, Novotná M, Lindová J, Havlicek J (2008) Neurophysiological effect of the Rh factor. Protective role of the RhD molecule against Toxoplasma-induced impairment of reaction times in women. Neuroendocrinology Letters 29: 475. pmid:18766148
  88. 88. Novotná M, Havlíček J, Smith A, Kolbeková P, Skallová A, et al. (2008) Toxoplasma and reaction time: role of toxoplasmosis in the origin, preservation and geographical distribution of Rh blood group polymorphism. Parasitology 135: 1253–1261. pmid:18752708
  89. 89. Hertwig R, Erev I (2009) The description—experience gap in risky choice. Trends in cognitive sciences 13: 517–523. pmid:19836292
  90. 90. Erev I, Ert E, Yechiam E (2008) Loss aversion, diminishing sensitivity, and the effect of experience on repeated decisions. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 21: 575–597.
  91. 91. Hertwig R, Barron G, Weber EU, Erev I (2004) Decisions from experience and the effect of rare events in risky choice. Psychological Science 15: 534–539. pmid:15270998
  92. 92. Tversky A, Kahneman D (1992) Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty. Journal of Risk and uncertainty 5: 297–323.
  93. 93. Prelec D (1998) The probability weighting function. Econometrica 66:497–527.