Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Facial Features: What Women Perceive as Attractive and What Men Consider Attractive

Abstract

Attractiveness plays an important role in social exchange and in the ability to attract potential mates, especially for women. Several facial traits have been described as reliable indicators of attractiveness in women, but very few studies consider the influence of several measurements simultaneously. In addition, most studies consider just one of two assessments to directly measure attractiveness: either self-evaluation or men's ratings. We explored the relationship between these two estimators of attractiveness and a set of facial traits in a sample of 266 young Spanish women. These traits are: facial fluctuating asymmetry, facial averageness, facial sexual dimorphism, and facial maturity. We made use of the advantage of having recently developed methodologies that enabled us to measure these variables in real faces. We also controlled for three other widely used variables: age, body mass index and waist-to-hip ratio. The inclusion of many different variables allowed us to detect any possible interaction between the features described that could affect attractiveness perception. Our results show that facial fluctuating asymmetry is related both to self-perceived and male-rated attractiveness. Other facial traits are related only to one direct attractiveness measurement: facial averageness and facial maturity only affect men's ratings. Unmodified faces are closer to natural stimuli than are manipulated photographs, and therefore our results support the importance of employing unmodified faces to analyse the factors affecting attractiveness. We also discuss the relatively low equivalence between self-perceived and male-rated attractiveness and how various anthropometric traits are relevant to them in different ways. Finally, we highlight the need to perform integrated-variable studies to fully understand female attractiveness.

Introduction

What makes a person attractive? This could be one of the most addressed questions in a broad range of fields in the social and natural sciences. The question is interesting beyond personal considerations, given the importance of attractiveness in human social interactions [1, 2]. Attractiveness often places attractive people in advantageous positions [3, 4]. For example, attractiveness can lead to both men and women receiving higher tips [5], benefitting from leniency bias in court decisions [6], and achieving higher long-term socioeconomic status [7]. In fact, evidence shows that our species tends to see attractive people as the embodiment of many positive qualities [1, 810]. This is the so-called “what is beautiful is good” stereotype [11].

Beauty is a complex construct that has been studied to a great extent throughout history [12]. It is commonly defined as an intrinsic property of some individuals that generates a number of emotions in those perceiving it [13]. Of these, pleasure is probably ubiquitous [12, 13]. Although pleasure elicited by the perception of beauty is not immediately linked to an expected utility [14], both beauty and its pleasurable effect in the opposite sex are relevant to the process of attracting potential mates [15, 16]. However, it is important to note that, regardless of sexual orientation, both sexes perceive male and female facial attractiveness similarly [17]. [39]

Several anthropometric traits have been associated with attractiveness [2, 18, 19]. Many of these features have been studied in detail in human faces, since the face is the most important part of the body in social interactions [20] and facial features are important determinants of overall attractiveness [21]. The facial features associated with attractiveness are: 1) Fluctuating asymmetry (FA; [2225]), which measures the deviation from bilateral symmetry attributed to the individual, but not to the population [22, 26]; 2) facial averageness [2730], which refers to the closeness of a face to the population average, 3) femininity of a female face [2, 15, 31, 32], which refers to the facial features that differentiate an individual as a woman; and 4) youthfulness [18, 32], which refers to the degree to which facial features relate to an early age. These features have been proposed as being linked to attractiveness, since they are indicators of desirable properties in the potential partners. For example, FA would be an indicator for developmental stability [33, 34], averageness for heterozygosity [35, 36] and both femininity [2, 37] and youthfulness [3840] would signal fertility and health. The link of these features to fitness is well established, even when some alternative explanations have been proposed for their relation to attractiveness [16, 4144].

In addition to facial features, features measured on the body, like the waist to hip ratio (WHR) [45] and the body mass index (BMI) [46], relate to attractiveness reliably. However, there is some controversy regarding the relative importance of these two [4750], as both variables are usually correlated [46, 51]. In any case, both measurements are very often associated with attractiveness [47, 5255], although there are some exceptions [56]. Women with a WHR of around .70 are classified as the most attractive by men of most cultures [47], and low values in BMI within the standard range, i.e., approximately 20, are typically regarded as more appealing [46, 51]. Values perceived as more attractive in both variables are usually related to better health [5759] and higher fertility [6063].

We investigated the link of a set of facial markers of female attractiveness to attractiveness in a group of young Spanish women. These were facial FA, facial averageness, facial sexual dimorphism (femininity) and facial maturity (youthfulness). We sought to measure how strongly these variables affect attractiveness when considered together. Furthermore, we controlled for age, WHR and BMI, as they have been described as influencing both measurements of attractiveness [53]. Hence, we evaluated simultaneously most of the key anthropometric traits in the study of women’s attractiveness described so far [2, 18, 15, 64, 65], since our main objective was to study the concurrent effect of all the variables, as they are perceived in real faces. Although it is very useful to isolate the effect of a single variable for the purpose of analysis, in real interactions it is impossible to separate the effect of different traits on the perception of attractiveness. Any attempt to fully understand how different features influence the perception of attractiveness should consider which trait, if any, is more influential, or whether their effects overlap or are independent. This would lead to an accurate assessment of the relevance of each variable to the perception of attractiveness. Furthermore, many of the studies previously mentioned evaluate the importance of the variables employing digitally manipulated faces (changing the studied features artificially) instead of using real faces. This calls into question the direct applicability of their findings to natural evaluations of real faces [2, 15, 18]. Therefore, a re-evaluation of the importance of said findings is necessary employing stimuli that are as close as possible to those that are present in real interaction.

Two methods have been previously employed to measure attractiveness. One measures women’s attractiveness according to male subjects' ratings [21, 47, 52, 53, 56]. The other method employs women’s perception of their own attractiveness [5, 53, 6670]. Both variables are strongly related and show reciprocal influence, but they also show fundamental differences. Self-perceived and Male-rated scores correlate positively in several studies [53, 68], negatively in others [66] or do not correlate at all [69, 71]. For this reason, it has been proposed that both measurements of attractiveness should be treated as being related but independent [10], as in this study. Thus, we aimed to measure how strongly facial FA, facial averageness, facial maturity and facial sexual dimorphism affect both attractiveness evaluations, i.e., Self-perceived and Male-rated attractiveness, and to see to what extent these influences are similar.

When considering the variables individually, we expected to find a relationship in line with the results reported in previous studies, meaning that women with low facial FA, a younger facial appearance, and faces closer to the average and more feminine, will be perceived as more attractive both by men and by themselves. We also expected both measurements of attractiveness to correlate negatively with age. Additionally, we expected a woman with a WHR close to .70 and a BMI lower than 25 and higher than 19 to be considered attractive. We were not able to predict results when all variables are considered simultaneously, since all previous studies consider at most two variables concurrently [7274]. Moreover, the different features have been measured in different ways, from morphometric computation to population ratings, and their effect on attractiveness has been estimated using different methods (real, constructed or manipulated images).

The three objectives of this work can be summarized as follows: 1) To study the simultaneous effect of variables previously studied separately to account for their relative importance and possible cross-effects; 2) To analyse the importance of these variables in a study with unmodified faces, which are a more natural stimulus; 3) To assess the effect of various facial features on Self-perceived and on Male-rated attractiveness. The differences are especially interesting given that the two variables are expected to show different effects on daily human interactions even when they are closely related.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The sample was composed of 266 female undergraduate students from the Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain), ages 18 to 30 (21.60 ± 2.56). The number of participants had to be at least 215 to achieve a statistical power of .8, considering the previously calculated effect size of facial FA on facial attractiveness [22]. The participants received a payment of 10 € for their participation in the study.

Ethics statement

The experimental procedures received the written consent of the Comité de Ética de la Investigación (CEI) (Research Ethics Committee) of the Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Permit number: CEI-25-575). Each participant signed an informed consent form granting authorisation for the use of the data for research purposes.

Anthropometric measurements

Facial Fluctuating Asymmetry, Facial Averageness, Facial Dimorphism (facial sexual dimorphism) and Facial Maturity were measured employing Geometric Morphometrics. Studying the shape using Geometric Morphometrics provides the advantage that the geometric information contained in the original shape is retained during the analysis [7578]. The method employs a number of landmark coordinates that are placed directly on the face, rather than distances or angles, avoiding some of the well-documented problems of traditional morphometry: inconsistencies in the size correction methods followed, the lack of homology between measurements or issues with the relative positions of the traits employed as references [75, 78, 79]. Additionally, its statistical properties have proven to be better for the analysis of facial shape than those of traditional methods [8081].

We measured Facial Fluctuating Asymmetry (Facial FA) following a previously described procedure [82] that has been successfully replicated [83, 84]. Participants were prompted to look straight into the camera (Nikon D90) with a neutral expression. To obtain the pictures, we used standardised conditions of light and head orientation (i.e., a frontal position). Facial FA was calculated from 39 facial landmarks that were identified on each face (for details see [82]) using the TPS software toolkit (available in http://www.life.bio.sunysb.edu/morph). Each author placed landmarks independently (i.e., they were placed four times) allowing the software to correct errors in landmark placement. The MorphoJ software [85] (available in http://www.flywings.org.uk/MorphoJ_page.htm) was used to calculate facial symmetry from the Procrustes distances between each landmark and the corresponding landmark of its mirror image. These distances were decomposed into directional asymmetry and FA by means of the Procrustes ANOVA method [86, 87]. As a measurement of FA we employed the Mahalanobis distance [88]. The error in landmark positioning was not significant (Procrustes ANOVA, error SS = 7.015 × 10−2, df = 59052, F = 0.108, p = 0.999).

We measured Facial Averageness by calculating the Procrustes distance between the landmark distribution of each participant’s average face, determined as described below, and a reference face [82, 88, 89]. We understand this feature as the difference in shape between an individual face and a prototype average face their socialmedia. ions with ins with ibut there strives fundamental differences among them Self-perceived is basically a learni(the reference face, see below), in line with other authors in different contexts [37, 9092]. Each participant’s average face was computed from two of her photos and their mirror images to reduce the possible effects on each of these measurements of both landmark placement imprecision and the asymmetry of the particular faces. The reference female shape was obtained by averaging 358 images of self-reported White female students and their mirror images. All of the photographs employed in addition to the participants’ images to compute this and the other morphometric measurements were obtained from previously published and unpublished experiments [82, 84, 88, 89, 93]. Note that, in the case of this variable, lower scores correspond to higher averageness.

Facial Dimorphism and Facial Maturity were measured comparing the faces of the participants with, respectively, a set of male and girls’ photos in order to obtain a measurement that differentiated masculine from feminine faces (Facial Dimorphism) and adult from childlike female faces (Facial Maturity). We performed a PCA on the face shapes by using Geometric Morphometrics and then we included the significant factors (which account for at least 1% of variance) in a posterior discriminant analysis. These discriminant scores are employed as a measurement of Facial Dimorphism when male faces are included in the analysis [92, 94] and as a measurement of Facial Maturity when girls’ photos are included. We employed 320 facial images of self-reported White male students and 62 images of self-reported White schoolgirls aged between 11 and 14 years for this purpose. Male and girls faces were also averaged (using the sum of each image and its mirror images) to reduce any effect of differences in asymmetry in these measurements. The landmarks were the same employed in previous analyses. MorphoJ [85] allowed us to superimpose the shapes with a generalized least-square Procrustes fit. The covariance matrix was computed from these data of variation among individuals, and the PCAs were carried out on it.

For Facial Dimorphism computation we chose the first eleven PCs, which together accounted for 91.71% of the variance in facial landmark configuration. The resulting discriminant function classified correctly 98.3% of the faces. The discriminant function scores constitute an index of Facial Dimorphism, with small scores corresponding to more masculine faces.

For Facial Maturity computation we chose the first thirteen PCs, which together accounted for 90.12% of the variance. The resulting discriminant function classified correctly 93.6% of the faces. We employed the discriminant function scores as a measurement of Facial Maturity. In the case of this variable, lower scores correspond to mature faces and higher values correspond to more childlike faces.

Waist-to-hip Ratio (WHR) was obtained by dividing waist perimeter by hip perimeter. The waist of participants was measured in the lower girth region of the natural waist, directly above the umbilicus. The hip was measured along the widest point of the gluteal region. During measurements, participants stood with their feet together, arms relaxed to their sides, breathing normally, and with their body weight uniformly distributed to minimise the error caused by clothing. To calculate the body mass Index (BMI), each participant’s weight and height was measured barefoot and without heavy clothing. Height was measured with a manual stadiometer; weight was measured with a digital balance. In consideration to the participants, all measurements used to calculate WHR and BMI were taken only once and privately by a female researcher.

Direct attractiveness measurements

Self-perceived attractiveness: Each participant reported an estimation of their own physical attractiveness in a 1-to-7 Likert-type scale in which 1 was the lowest score and 7 the highest (“How physically attractive do you consider yourself?”/ ¿Cómo de físicamente atractiva te consideras?”).

Male-rated attractiveness: A group of 44 heterosexual men (M±SD, age: 31.63±6.00) rated the attractiveness of the participants from their pictures in a Likert 10-point scale (0 = “not attractive at all”/ “nada atractiva” to 10 = “extremely attractive”/”extremadamente atractiva”). Black and white photos were used in an attempt to reduce the effect of colour information. These images were presented randomly in nine different sessions in which raters evaluated 35 photos or fewer to avoid saturation. Not all the raters participated in each session and, moreover, they were instructed not to rate a woman if they could identify her. Consequently, not all of the evaluators rated all of the 266 photographs. We constructed an attractiveness index for each picture, averaging collected ratings for each photo. Only 40 of the 266 female photographs were rated by each of the 44 evaluators, but the average numbers of ratings per photo was 30.25. The consistency of the ratings was ICC(p) = 0.926, which is large enough to consider the process to be reliable [95].

Statistical Analysis

Facial FA and Facial Dimorphism were normally distributed, and Male-rated attractiveness, Facial Averageness, and Facial Maturity followed the normal distribution after log conversion. However, there were four other variables that did not fit the normal distribution: Self-perceived attractiveness, age, WHR, and BMI. We transformed all variables to Z scores to compare easily the effect of variables with different scales [96]. The full data are available as electronic supplementary material (S1 Dataset).

We employed non-parametric Spearman’s Rank correlations and linear regression models followed by an analysis of the normality of residuals. All final models selected satisfy the normality condition. Variables included in the final two models we report were chosen through an automatic stepwise selection process. As some of the variables had been shown previously to correlate [97], we included the tolerance values (all of the values larger than .75) in the regression models. Furthermore, the condition indexes are lower than 2, indicating few problems of collinearity among the variables included in the different models [98]. All of the analyses described above were performed with the SPSS 15.0 package. To compare the effect of Facial FA on both Self-perceived attractiveness and Male-rated attractiveness, we computed two models including all of the variables with the exception of Facial FA. Then, we performed two simple regression models with the residuals of the models without Facial FA as the dependent variable and with Facial FA as the independent variable. Finally, we compared the coefficients obtained for Facial FA in these models following the method described by Zar [99].

Results

Descriptive statistics of the analysed features and correlations with both measurements of attractiveness are included in Table 1. Correlation analyses showed that all anthropometric variables were associated with at least one of the two measurements of attractiveness (i.e., Self-perceived or Male-rated), except for Facial Dimorphism (See Table 1). There was a positive but weak relationship between Self-perceived and Men-rated attractiveness (Table 1). As some of the variables were significantly correlated, we computed the tolerance values of these variables in further regression analyses.

thumbnail
Table 1. Descriptive values of all variables, and Spearman’s Rank correlation between Self-perceived and Male-rated attractiveness with anthropometric traits.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132979.t001

To analyse the effect of the different variables on Self-perceived and Male-rated attractiveness, we performed a series of linear regressions including all the anthropometric variables. First, we analysed the relationship of Self-perceived attractiveness to the different facial features (see Table 2), controlling for age, BMI and WHR. We performed backward and forward stepwise regression tests, ultimately choosing a model including Facial FA and the three control variables (BMI, WHR, and age). Facial FA and the BMI and WHR control variables were negatively associated with Self-perceived attractiveness. Age was positively associated, showing that older participants perceived themselves as being more attractive than younger ones. From this model, we tested the interactions between Facial FA and the control variables. None of these interactions were significant.

thumbnail
Table 2. Different models obtained from linear regression with enter and stepwise methods of Self-perceived attractiveness with facial measurements of averageness, dimorphism, maturity, Facial FA and controlling for age, BMI and WHR.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132979.t002

Facial Averageness and Facial Maturity are log transformed. FA (Fluctuating Asymmetry); BMI (Body mass index); WHR (Waist to hip ratio).

Regarding Male-rated attractiveness, we also generated a model including the four morphometric variables and the three control variables (Table 3). The model finally chosen after both back and forward stepwise regression (see Table 3) included Facial FA, Facial Averageness and Facial Maturity. Of the control variables, only BMI remained. The variables included were negatively associated with this measurement of attractiveness, with the exception of Facial Maturity. This means that faces closer to the average and with low values for maturity and asymmetry were highly rated. To test the interactions between morphometric and control variables, we included the three control variables in the model. None of the interactions between morphometric variables and the control variables yielded a significant result.

thumbnail
Table 3. Different models obtained from linear regression with enter and stepwise methods, of Male-rated attractiveness with facial measurements of averageness, dimorphism, maturity, Facial FA and controlling for age, BMI and WHR.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132979.t003

Since Facial FA was shown to have an effect on both Self-perceived attractiveness and Male-rated attractiveness, we were interested in comparing the strength of the effect on each model. To that end, first we computed the residuals after adjusting both final models for all of the variables except for Facial FA. After this, we computed a simple regression model with the residuals as dependent variables and Facial FA as the independent variable (residuals Male-rated attractiveness and Facial FA: R2 = .017, beta = -.130, p < .05; residuals Self-perceived attractiveness and Facial FA: R2 = .015, beta = -.124, p < .05) and compared the betas. We did not find differences in the betas (t(528) = .065, p = .948). Thus, Facial FA has a similar effect in the perception of attractiveness assessed in both Self-perceived and Male-rated tests.

Discussion

The present study explores the effect of several facial features on two direct measurements of attractiveness: women’s self-perception and men’s rating of women's attractiveness. Our results show that these facial traits independently influence these two types of attractiveness measurements when real faces are employed. Facial FA affects both estimations of attractiveness with a similar intensity. However, while no other facial feature significantly influences Self-perceived attractiveness, Male-rated attractiveness is importantly affected by both Facial Maturity and Facial Averageness. This supports, at least partially, the hypothesis that self-perceived and third-party measurements of attractiveness are independent variables [10].

It might seem remarkable that Facial FA shows an equivalent effect on the two direct measurements of attractiveness, particularly considering that they are also influenced by different facial features. This observation confirms the importance of Facial FA in determining attractiveness. On the other hand, it may not be particularly surprising, given that symmetry is extensively considered a sign of attractiveness [10, 18, 19]. It is also an important factor in mating success in many species [34, 100]. To our knowledge, the effect of FA has never before been studied simultaneously on self-assessed and externally assessed attractiveness. The link between attractiveness and FA is usually attributed to the role of the latter as a reliable indicator of developmental stability [2]. However, there is also evidence that the preference for symmetrical partners is not necessarily related to potential mating benefits [42, 101], although this issue is under debate [102]. In any case, our results support past studies and confirm the inverse relationship between Facial FA and attractiveness [23, 25]. Our results are also consistent with a recent meta-analysis that finds a significant albeit weak link between the two [22]. Surprisingly, studies employing real faces that considered FA together with a second morphometric variable, like averageness [73] or masculinity [74], showed that FA was irrelevant in determining female attractiveness than the other variable. One possible explanation for this discrepancy might be an unexpected influence of uncontrolled variables that could be masking the true effect of FA. There are also procedural differences between these studies and ours that might provide an explanation. One of the studies followed traditional morphometry procedures [72], and presented, as described before, some of the usual problems associated with these procedures [75, 78, 79], like the variations in the method followed to standardize. The other studies [73, 74] employ Geometric Morphometrics, but the landmarks chosen are not exactly the same as ours. However, the main difference between our study and others is the number of participants: 266 women participated in our study, compared to 62 in [72], 48 in [73] and 112 in [74]. When one considers an expected effect size of Facial FA on facial attractiveness of .19 (according to [22]), together with the desired power of 80% and p-values lower than .05, one concludes that the sample size must be larger than 215.

More interesting is the influence of Facial Maturity and Facial Averageness on the attractiveness evaluations. The former variable measures the dissimilarity of the female participants’ faces to the faces of girls aged 11–14. The latter measures the closeness of the participants’ faces to the average female face. According to our results, Facial Maturity affects women’s attractiveness as rated by men, suggesting that men focus on cues for youth when evaluating female faces. This is in line with previous results showing that for men, the most attractive versions of adult women’s faces are those proportioned like young faces [103]. This result is also consistent with the widely described tendency of men to prefer younger women [5, 38, 104, 105]. It is likely that, in showing a preference for younger faces (i.e., a faces with fewer signs of maturity), men obtain highly reliable information about women’s reproductive value, fertility, and heritable disease resistance [40].

A higher Facial Averageness score (lesser averageness) shows a negative effect on attractiveness as evaluated by men, meaning that men prefer faces with shapes closer to the population average. This variable has been previously associated with female attractiveness in other studies [15]. Many of these worked with modified images [106108], and it was difficult to assess the importance of the variable in real interactions. Some studies employed real faces, usually measuring Facial Averageness considering evaluators’ ratings [35, 109]. These studies also found a clear relationship between Facial Averageness and such different properties as attractiveness and health. However, morphometric measurements of averageness performed with classical morphometric methods do not show a significant correlation with attractiveness (a meta-review in [15]). This result is usually attributed to an inaccuracy of classic morphometric methods that makes them unable to represent the shapes properly. Therefore we decided to employ Geometric Morphometrics to compute Facial Averageness, given that this method of defining the facial shape has been successful in determining shape differences in many other species [110112]. Ours is not the first attempt to measure averageness in human species employing Geometric Morphometrics [73]. We obtained similar results regarding Facial Averageness, but with a larger sample and considering more facial features in the analyses [73]. Moreover, we also replicated previous results showing a reduced importance for FA as compared to Facial Averageness employing both modified [108] and unmodified faces [72, 73].

Facial Dimorphism does not show a significant influence on any of the attractiveness measurements. In contrast, previous studies usually performed using measurements of attractiveness and femininity as evaluated by a third party [15], support this association. However, a reduction in the importance of femininity on attractiveness is observed when some variables are considered simultaneously in the analyses [113]. This result is interesting when one considers the effect of Facial Maturity on Male-rated attractiveness. These two variables, Facial Maturity and Facial Dimorphism, are expected to correlate strongly, since adult female faces are more similar to children’s faces than are male faces [18]. However, Facial Maturity has an effect on Male-rated attractiveness, while Facial Dimorphism does not. Moreover, in our study these two variables correlate negatively. This is not surprising, because although during adolescence male faces change more drastically than female faces [18], there are still many traits in a woman’s face that change during this period, under the influence of sex hormones [114]. These traits differentiate adult women’s faces from those of young girls, especially lip thickness [72, 115], pronounced cheekbones and narrow cheeks [31, 116, 117]. Hence, such secondary characteristics are the features that draw a clear line between a feminine face and an immature face, that is, between Facial Dimorphism and Facial Maturity. Given that our measurement method maximizes the differences between groups, it is clear that Facial Dimorphism considers mainly those feminine features unrelated to a childlike appearance (given that those two correlate negatively). This suggests that, when rating females' facial attractiveness, men attach less importance to facial features that distinguish a female from a male face (e.g., thickening of the lips), than to facial features signalling youth. This particular analysis has been possible because our Facial Dimorphism measurement is not significantly affected by childlike features. Therefore, our measurement procedure allows us to discriminate between feminine features related to the retainment of childlike features from those features specifically developed in women, a distinction that usual measurements of femininity do not make [74, 94]. It is remarkable that this preference for youth indicators over sexual dimorphic features is in contrast with the widely known tendency of men to prefer female bodies with a clear feminine shape [8082, 86, 91]. However, it is important to note that, for the most part, sexual dimorphism and sexual maturity are indicated by the same features in the female body.

Our results replicate previous studies [5, 53] that demonstrate that body traits (BMI and WHR) affect the self-perception of attractiveness. This confirms the results from studies that considered these traits separately [46, 47], and to a certain extent, those that studied them simultaneously [48, 50, 51, 63]. According to a previous study [118], BMI, as does the percentage of body fat, has an effect on Male-rated facial attractiveness, but WHR does not. It is noteworthy that facial adiposity is predicted more precisely by BMI than by WHR [54, 119], and that facial adiposity is negatively associated with health [54, 120]. Therefore, the effect of BMI on the facial rating of attractiveness by men might be understood as a factor affecting men’s perception of women’s health, based on facial adiposity. The lack of association between WHR and Male-rated attractiveness can be explained by the fact that male raters did not observe the body of participants, whereas it stands to reason that women consider all their body traits in their self-perception. However, several past studies have shown that signs of attractiveness are correlated [31, 121123] (but see [124]), and, specifically, that women's faces and bodies both influence attractiveness scores strongly [56]. Therefore, we would expect variables affecting body attractiveness, like WHR, to affect facial attractiveness as well. However, we are not able to detect this relationship, which stresses the necessity for further study of the correlation between the variables related to attractiveness.

Finally, age also shows an effect on Self-perceived attractiveness. However, in contrast with the literature [38, 105], we found a positive association between the two. This apparently counter-intuitive result can be explained by considering that our participants were a homogeneous sample of very young women (see Table 1). Therefore, the positive effect of age on attractiveness may be due to the particular age of the older women in our sample, who are closer to their sexual maturity and fecundity peaks [125, 126].

Interestingly, we find a weak relationship between Self-perceived attractiveness and Male-rated attractiveness (Table 1), which has also been reported in previous studies [53, 68, 69] (though others do not find this relationship [66, 71]). In fact, some authors have proposed that these two measurements should be considered to be completely different variables [10]. Such a suggestion is supported by our results, as different morphometric variables have a different effect on each of these two measurements of attractiveness. This is not surprising, given that the latter show fundamental differences. Male-rated attractiveness depends basically on physical features (particularly if the rating is based on photos). On the other hand, Self-perceived attractiveness is a feature built in the course of an individual’s life, and it is based at least partially on the individual’s interactions with their social environment. Therefore, considering that Facial FA is not expected to change significantly after puberty [127, 128], its effect on Self-perceived attractiveness is not a surprising result, given that it has had a longer time than other variables to establish itself as an attractive or unattractive feature for other people. However, Facial Averageness, which is equally stable, has failed to show an effect on Self-Perceived attractiveness. These results confirm the difficulty of making clear predictions on the combined effect of variables not studied together before.

As a general conclusion, and in accordance with our results, we suggest that future studies on attractiveness should measure the importance of any morphometric variable in real faces, given that currently there are enough morphometric tools to properly measure it. We recommend a cautious use of modified images, given that some variables render different results when artificial, computer manipulated images are used, potentially introducing confusion to the scientific discussion (for a review of the differences between results with modified and non-modified images see [18]). We also recommend analysing the effect of different variables simultaneously instead of one by one, since the inclusion of some variables reduces considerably the significance of others. Similarly, we suggest to include in any experimental design the self-perceived attractiveness score in addition to the attractiveness scores from third-party raters, because the two are distinctively different variables and because the former is likely to have greater importance on any conscious effect of attractiveness on individual behaviour.

Finally, we want to point out that the present study, like many others in the field, was carried out exclusively with a university population of self-described White women from a western culture. For this reason, before generalizing our findings to the rest of the human species, it would be necessary to extend the analysis to a greater range of ages, socio-cultural strata and ethnic groups. This is especially important considering that many of the analysed variables have shown different importance in different societies [32, 47, 129].

Supporting Information

S1 Dataset. Complete facial features dataset.

The dataset includes all the variables considered and information about the computing process for Facial Maturity, Facial Dimorphism and Male-rated Attractiveness.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132979.s001

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

The authors thank I. Monedero, M. Arjona, C Martin-Elola and M. Losada for their help with the experiments and to S. Sanchez-Pages and M. Magariños for their constructive comments on the paper.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: ET MP JAMR. Performed the experiments: JAMR MIJ. Analyzed the data: JAMR MIJ MP ET. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: ET JAMR. Wrote the paper: JAMR MIJ MP ET.

References

  1. 1. Langlois JH, Kalakanis L, Rubenstein AJ, Larson A, Hallamm M, Smoot M. Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychol Bull. 2000; 126: 390–423. pmid:10825783
  2. 2. Little AC, Jones BC, DeBruine LM. Facial attractiveness: Evolutionary based research. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2011; 366: 1638–1659. pmid:21536551
  3. 3. Beall AE. Can a new smile make you look more intelligent and successful? Dent Clin N Am. 2007; 51: 289–297. pmid:17532913
  4. 4. Singh D. Mating strategies of young women: Role of physical attractiveness. J Sex Res. 2004; 41: 43–54. pmid:15216423
  5. 5. Lynn M. Determinants and consequences of female attractiveness and sexiness: Realistic tests with restaurant waitresses. Arch Sex Behav. 2009; 38: 737–745. pmid:18543091
  6. 6. Mazzella R, Feingold A. The effects of physical attractiveness, race, socioeconomic status, and gender of defendants and victims on judgments of mock jurors: A meta-analysis. J Appl Soc Psychol. 1994; 24: 1315–1344.
  7. 7. Benzeval M, Green M, Macintyre S. Does perceived physical attractiveness in adolescence predict better socioeconomic position in adulthood? Evidence from 20 years of follow up in a population cohort study. PLoS ONE. 2013; 8: e63975. pmid:23717520
  8. 8. Brand RJ, Bonatsos A, D'Orazio R, DeShong H. What is beautiful is good, even online: Correlations between photo attractiveness and text attractiveness in men’s online dating profiles. Comput Hum Behav. 2012; 28: 166–170.
  9. 9. Lorenzo GL, Biesanz JC, Human LJ. What is beautiful is good and more accurately understood: Physical attractiveness and accuracy in first impressions of personality. Psychol Sci. 2010; 21: 1777–1782. pmid:21051521
  10. 10. Feingold A. Good looking people are not what we think. Psychol Bull. 1992; 111: 304–341.
  11. 11. Dion K, Berscheid E, Walster EJ. What is beautiful is good. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1972; 24: 285–290. pmid:4655540
  12. 12. Reber R, Schwarz N, Winkielman P. Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver's processing experience? Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2004; 8: 364–382. pmid:15582859
  13. 13. Etcoff N. Survival of the Prettiest: The Science of Beauty. New York: Anchor/ Doubleday; 1999.
  14. 14. Santayana G. The sense of beauty. New York: Dover; 1955. (Original work published in 1896)
  15. 15. Rhodes G. The Evolutionary Psychology of facial beauty Annu Rev Psychol. 2006; 57: 199–226. pmid:16318594
  16. 16. Puts DA. Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. Evol Hum Behav. 2010; 31, 157–175.
  17. 17. Kranz F, Ishai A. Face perception is modulated by sexual preference. Curr Biol. 2006; 16: 63–68. pmid:16401423
  18. 18. Kościński K. Facial attractiveness: General patterns of facial preferences. Anthropol Rev. 2007; 70: 45–79.
  19. 19. Gangestad SW, Scheyd GJ. The evolution of human physical attractiveness. Annu Rev Anthropol. 2005; 34: 523–548.
  20. 20. Farmer H, McKay R, Tsakiris M. Trust in me trustworthy others are seen as more physically similar to the self. Psychol Sci. 2004; 25: 290–292.
  21. 21. Currie TE, Little AC. The relative importance of the face and body in judgments of human physical attractiveness. Evol Hum Behav. 2009; 30: 409–416.
  22. 22. van Dongen S, Gangestad SW. Human fluctuating asymmetry in relation to health and quality: a meta-analysis. Evol Hum Behav. 2011; 32: 380–398.
  23. 23. Brown WM, Price ME, Kang J, Pound N, Zhao Y, Yu H. Fluctuating asymmetry and preferences for sex-typical bodily characteristics. Proc Nat Acad Sci. 2008; 105: 12938–12943. pmid:18711125
  24. 24. Fink B, Thanzami V, Seydel H, Manning JT. Digit ratio and hand-grip strength in German and Mizos men: Cross-cultural evidence for an organizing effect of prenatal testosterone on strength. Am J Hum Biol. 2006; 18: 776–782. pmid:17039475
  25. 25. Perrett DI, Burt DM, Penton-Voak IS, Lee KJ, Rowland DA, Edwards R. Symmetry and human facial attractiveness. Evol Hum Behav. 1999; 20: 295–307.
  26. 26. van Valen LV. A study on fluctuating asymmetry. Evolution. 1962; 16: 125–142.
  27. 27. Langlois JH, Roggman LA. Attractive faces are only average. Psychol Sci. 1990; 1: 115–121.
  28. 28. Halberstadt J, Rhodes G. The attractiveness of nonface averages: Implications for an evolutionary explanation of the attractiveness of average faces. Psychol Sci. 2000; 11: 285–289. pmid:11273386
  29. 29. Halberstadt J, Rhodes G. It’s not just average faces that are attractive: Computer-manipulated averageness makes birds, fish, and automobiles attractive. Psychon B Rev. 2003; 10: 149–156.
  30. 30. Winkielman P, Halberstadt J, Fazendeiro T, Catty S. Prototypes are attractive because they are easy on the mind. Psychol Sci. 2006; 17: 799–826. pmid:16984298
  31. 31. Koehler N, Simmons LW, Rhodes G, Peters M. The relationship between sexual dimorphism in human faces and fluctuating asymmetry. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2004; 271: 233–236.
  32. 32. Jones D, Brace CL, Jankowiak W, Laland KN, Musselman LE, Langlois JH, et al. Sexual selection, physical attractiveness, and facial neoteny: Cross-cultural evidence and implications. Curr Anthropol. 1995; 36: 723–748.
  33. 33. Møller AP. A review of developmental instability, parasitism and disease infection, genetics and evolution. Infect Genet Evol. 2006; 6: 133–140. pmid:16269271
  34. 34. Møller AP, Swaddle JP. Asymmetry, developmental stability and evolution. New York: Oxford University Press; 1997.
  35. 35. Lie HC, Rhodes G, Simmons LW. Genetic diversity revealed in human faces. Evolution. 2008; 62: 2473–2486. pmid:18691260
  36. 36. Mitton JB, Grant MC. Associations among proteins heterozygosity, growth rate, and developmental homeostasis. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1984; 15: 479–499.
  37. 37. Perrett DI, Lee KJ, Penton-Voak I, Rowland D, Yoshikawa S, Burt DM, et al. Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature. 1998; 394: 884–887. pmid:9732869
  38. 38. Buss DM. Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behav Brain Sci. 1989; 12: 1–49.
  39. 39. Mathes EW, Brennan SM, Haugen PM, Rice HB. Ratings of physical attractiveness as a function of age. J Soc Psychol. 1985; 125: 157–168. pmid:4057968
  40. 40. Scheyd GJ, Garver-Apgar CE, Gangestad SW. Physical attractiveness: Signals of phenotypic quality and beyond. In: Crawford CB, Krebs D, editors. Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 2008. pp. 239–260.
  41. 41. Ghirlanda S, Jansson L, Enquist M. Chickens prefer beautiful humans. Hum Nature-Int Bios. 2002; 13: 383–389.
  42. 42. Jansson L, Forkman B, Enquist M. Experimental evidence of receiver bias for symmetry. Anim Behav. 2002; 63: 617–621.
  43. 43. DeBruine LM, Jones BC, Unger L, Little AC, Feinberg DR. Dissociating averageness and attractiveness: attractive faces are not always average. J Exp Psychol Human. 2007; 33: 1420–1430.
  44. 44. Edler RJ. Background considerations to facial aesthetics. J Orthod. 2001; 28: 159–168. pmid:11395532
  45. 45. Singh D. Adaptive significance of female physical attractiveness: Role of waist-to-hip-ratio. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993; 65: 293–307. pmid:8366421
  46. 46. Tovée MJ, Reinhardt S, Emery JL, Cornelissen PL. Optimum body-mass index and maximum sexual attractiveness. Lancet. 1998; 352: 548. pmid:9716069
  47. 47. Singh D, Dixson BJ, Jessop TS, Morgan B, Dixson AF. Cross-cultural consensus for waist—hip ratio and women's attractiveness. Evol Hum Behav. 2010; 31: 176–181.
  48. 48. Holliday IE, Longe OA, Thai NJ, Hancock PJB, Tovée MJ. BMI not WHR modulates BOLD fMRI responses in a sub cortical reward network when participants judge the attractiveness of human female bodies. PLoS ONE. 2011; 6: e27255. pmid:22102883
  49. 49. Manning JT, Trivers RL, Singh D, Thornhill A. The mystery of female beauty. Nature. 1999; 399: 214–215.
  50. 50. Platek SM, Singh D. Optimal waist-to-hip ratios in women activate neural reward centers in men. PLoS ONE. 2010; 5: e9042. pmid:20140088
  51. 51. Tovée MJ, Maisey DS, Emery JL, Cornelissen PL. Visual cues to female physical attractiveness. Proc R Soc Lond B. 1999; 266: 211–218.
  52. 52. Kościński K. Attractiveness of women’s body: body mass index, waist—hip ratio, and their relative importance. Behav Ecol. 2013; 24: 914–925.
  53. 53. Weeden J, Sabini J. Subjective and objective measures of attractiveness and their relation to sexual behavior and sexual attitudes in university students. Arch Sex Behav. 2007; 36: 79–88. pmid:17136591
  54. 54. Coetzee V, Chen J, Perrett DI, Stephen ID. Deciphering faces: Quantifiable visual cues to weight. Perception. 2010; 39: 51–61. pmid:20301846
  55. 55. Rosenblum GD, Lewis M. The relations among body image, physical attractiveness and body mass in adolescence. Child Dev. 1999; 70: 50–64. pmid:10191514
  56. 56. Thornhill R, Grammer K. The body and face of woman: One ornament that signals quality? Evol Hum Behav. 1998; 202: 105–120.
  57. 57. Singh D. Female mate value at a glance: Relationships of waist-to-hip-ratio to health, fecundity and attractiveness. Neuro Endocrinol Lett. 2002; 23: 81–91. pmid:12496738
  58. 58. Flegal KM, Graubard BI, Williamson DF, Gail MH. Excess deaths associated with underweight, overweight, and obesity. J Amer Med Assoc. 2005; 293: 1861–1867.
  59. 59. Ritz BW, Gardner EM. Malnutrition and energy restriction differentially affect viral immunity. J Nutr. 2006; 136: 1141–1144. pmid:16614394
  60. 60. Zaadstra B, Seidell J, Van Noord P, Te Velde E, Habbema J, Wrieswijk B. Fat and female fecundity: Prospective study of body fat distribution in conception rates. Brit Med J. 1993; 306: 484–487. pmid:8448457
  61. 61. Jasieńska G, Ziomkiewicz A, Ellison P, Lipson S, Thune I. Large breasts and narrow waists indicate high reproductive potential in women. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2004; 281: 1213–1217.
  62. 62. Lake JK, Power C, Cole TJ. Women's reproductive health: the role of body mass index in early and adult life'. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 1997; 21: 432–438. pmid:9192225
  63. 63. Pawłowski B, Dunbar RIM. Waist to hip ratio versus BMI as predictors of fitness in women. Hum Nature. 2005; 16: 164–177.
  64. 64. Pisanski K, Feinberg DR. Cross-cultural variation in mate preferences for averageness, symmetry, body size, and masculinity. Cross-Cult Res. 2013; 47: 162–197.
  65. 65. Buggio L, Vercellini P, Somigliana E, Viganò P, Frattaruolo MP, Fedele L. “You are so beautiful”: Behind women’s attractiveness towards the biology of reproduction: A narrative review. Gynecol Endocrin. 2012; 28: 753–757.
  66. 66. Clark AP. Self-perceived attractiveness and masculinization predict women's sociosexuality. Evol Hum Behav. 2004; 25: 113–124.
  67. 67. Little AC, Burt DM, Penton-Voak IS, Perrett DI. Self-perceived attractiveness influences human female preferences for sexual dimorphism and symmetry in male faces. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2001; 268: 39–44.
  68. 68. Marcus DK, Miller RS. Sex differences in judgments of physical attractiveness: A social relations analysis. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2003; 29: 325–335. pmid:15273010
  69. 69. Mulford M, Orbell J, Shatto C, Stockard J. Physical attractiveness, opportunity, and success in everyday exchange. Am J Sociol. 1998; 103: 1565–1592.
  70. 70. Vukovic J, Feinberg DR, Jones BC, DeBruine LM, Welling LLM, Little AC, et al. Self-rated attractiveness predicts individual differences in women’s preference for masculine men’s voices. Pers Indiv Differ. 2008; 45: 451–456.
  71. 71. Gabriel MT, Critelli JW, Ee JS. Narcissistic illusions in self-evaluations of intelligence and attractiveness. J Pers. 1994; 62: 143–155.
  72. 72. Baudouin JY, Tiberghien G. Symmetry, averageness, and feature size in the facial attractiveness of women. Acta Psychol. 2004; 117: 313–332.
  73. 73. Komori M, Kawamura S, Ishihara S. Averageness or symmetry: which is more important for facial attractiveness? Acta Psychol. 2009; 131: 136–142.
  74. 74. van Dongen S. Associations among facial masculinity, physical strength, fluctuating asymmetry and attractiveness in young men and women. Ann Hum Biol. 2014; 41: 205–213. pmid:24555492
  75. 75. Adams DC, Rohlf FJ, Slice DE. Geometric morphometrics: ten years of progress following the ‘revolution’. Ital J Zool. 2004; 71: 5–16.
  76. 76. Fink B, Grammer K, Mitteroecker P, Gunz P, Schaefer K, Bookstein FL, et al. Second to fourth digit ratio and face shape. P Roy Soc Lond B Bio. 2005; 272: 1995–2001.
  77. 77. Schaefer K, Fink B, Grammer K, Mitteroecker P, Gunz P, Bookstein FL. Female appearance: facial and bodily attractiveness as shape. Psychol Sci. 2006; 48: 187–204.
  78. 78. Slice DE. Geometric morphometrics. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 2007; 36: 261–281.
  79. 79. Rohlf FJ, Marcus LF. A revolution morphometrics. Trends Ecol Evol. 1993; 8: 129–132. pmid:21236128
  80. 80. Rohlf FJ. Statistical power comparisons among alternative morphometric methods. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2000; 111: 463. pmid:10727966
  81. 81. Rohlf FJ. Bias and error in estimates of mean shape in geometric morphometrics. J Hum Evol. 2003; 44: 665–683. pmid:12799158
  82. 82. Sanchez-Pages S, Turiegano E. Testosterone, facial symmetry and cooperation in the prisoners' dilemma. Physiol Behav. 2010; 99: 355–361. pmid:19954750
  83. 83. Holtzman NS, Augustine AA, Senne AL. Are pro-social or socially aversive people more physically symmetrical? Symmetry in relation to over 200 personality variables. J Res Pers. 2011; 45: 687–691.
  84. 84. Muñoz-Reyes JA, Gil-Burmann C, Fink B, Turiegano E. Facial asymmetry and aggression in Spanish adolescents. Pers Indiv Differ. 2012; 53: 857–861.
  85. 85. Klingenberg CP. MorphoJ: an integrated software package for geometric morphometrics. Mol Ecol Resour. 2011; 11: 353–357. pmid:21429143
  86. 86. Klingenberg CP, Barluenga M, Meyer A. Shape analysis of symmetric structures: Quantifying variation among individuals and asymmetry. Evolution. 2002; 56: 1909–1920. pmid:12449478
  87. 87. Klingenberg CP, McIntyre GS. Geometric morphometrics of developmental instability: analyzing patterns of fluctuating asymmetry with Procrustes methods. Evolution. 1998; 52: 1363–1375.
  88. 88. Muñoz-Reyes JA, Pita M, Arjona M, Sanchez-Pages S, Turiegano E. Who is the fairest of them all? The independent effect of attractive features and self-perceived attractiveness on cooperation among women. Evol Hum Behav. 2014; 35: 118–125.
  89. 89. Sanchez-Pages S, Turiegano E. Two studies on the interplay between social preferences and individual biological features. Behaviour. 2013; 150: 313–315.
  90. 90. Cornwell RE, Smith MJ, Boothroyd LG, Moore FR, Davis HP, Stirrat M, et al. Reproductive strategy, sexual development and attraction to facial characteristics. Philos T Roy Soc B. 2006; 361: 2143–2154.
  91. 91. Penton-Voak IS, Jones BC, Little AC, Baker S, Tiddeman B, Burt DM, et al. Symmetry, sexual dimorphism in facial proportions and male facial attractiveness. P Roy Soc Lond B Bio. 2001; 268: 1617–1623.
  92. 92. Scott IM, Pound N, Stephen ID, Clark AP, Penton-Voak IS. Does masculinity matter? The contribution of masculine face shape to male attractiveness in humans. PLoS ONE. 2010; 5: e13585. pmid:21048972
  93. 93. Muñoz-Reyes JA, Gil-Burmann C, Turiegano E. Digit Ratio 2D: 4D, facial masculinization and aggressiveness in Spanish adolescents. Estud Psicol. 2014; 35: 319–340.
  94. 94. Sanchez-Pages S, Rodriguez-Ruiz C, Turiegano E. Facial Masculinity: How the choice of measurement method enables to detect its influence on behaviour. PloS ONE. 2014; 9: e112157. pmid:25389770
  95. 95. McGraw KO, Wong SP. Forming inferences about some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychol Methods. 1996; 1: 30–46.
  96. 96. Sell A, Tooby J, Cosmides L. Formidability and the logic of human anger. Proc Nat Acad Sci. 2009; 106: 15073–15078. pmid:19666613
  97. 97. Peters M, Rhodes G, Simmons LW. Contributions of the face and body to overall attractiveness. Anim Behav. 2007; 73: 937–942.
  98. 98. Belsley DA. Conditioning diagnostics: Collinearity and weak data in regression. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1991.
  99. 99. Zar JH. Biostatistical analysis. 4th ed. London: Prentice-Hall International; 1998.
  100. 100. Møller AP, Thornhill R. Bilateral symmetry and sexual selection: a meta-analysis. Amer Nat. 1998; 151: 174–192.
  101. 101. Swaddle JP, Cuthill IC. Asymmetry and human facial attractiveness: Symmetry may not always be beautiful. Proc R Soc Lond B. 1995; 261: 111–116.
  102. 102. Little AC, Jones BC. Evidence against perceptual bias views for symmetry preferences in human faces. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2003; 270: 1759–1763.
  103. 103. Johnston VS, Franklin M. Is beauty in the eye of the beholder? Ethol Sociobiol. 1993; 14: 183–199.
  104. 104. Peters J, Shackelford TK, Buss DM. Understanding domestic violence against women: Using evolutionary psychology to extend the feminist functional analysis. Violence Vict. 2002; 17: 255–264. pmid:12033558
  105. 105. Schwarz S, Hassebrauck M. Sex and age differences in mate selection preferences. Hum Nature-Int Bios. 2012; 23: 447–466.
  106. 106. Apicella CL, Little AC, Marlowe FW. Facial averageness and attractiveness in an isolated population of hunter-gatherers. Perception. 2007; 36: 1813–1820. pmid:18283931
  107. 107. Rhodes G, Zebrowitz LA, Clark A, Kalick SM, Hightower A, McKay R. Do facial averageness and symmetry signal health? Evol Hum Behav. 2001; 22: 31–46. pmid:11182573
  108. 108. Griffey JA, Little AC. Similarities in human visual and declared measures of preference for opposite-sex faces. Exp Psychol. 2014; 61: 301–309. pmid:24351988
  109. 109. Rhodes G, Simmon LW, Peter M. Attractiveness and sexual behavior: Does attractiveness enhance mating success? Evol Hum Behav. 2005; 26: 186–201.
  110. 110. Klingenberg CP. Evolution and development of shape: integrating quantitative approaches. Nat Rev Genet. 2010; 11: 623–635. pmid:20697423
  111. 111. Viscosi V, Cardini A. Leaf morphology, taxonomy and geometric morphometrics: a simplified protocol for beginners. PLoS ONE. 2011; 6: e25630. pmid:21991324
  112. 112. Zelditch ML, Swiderski DL, Sheets HD. Geometric morphometrics for biologists: a primer. London: Academic Press; 2012.
  113. 113. Moore FR, Smith ML, Taylor V, Perrett DI. Sexual dimorphism in the female face is a cue to health and social status but not age. Pers indiv Differ. 2011; 50: 1068–1073.
  114. 114. Law-Smith MJ, Perrett DI, Jones BC, Cornwell RE, Moore FR, Feinberg DR, et al. Facial appearance is a cue to oestrogen levels in women. Proc R Soc Lond B. 2006; 273: 135–140.
  115. 115. Bisson M, Grobbelaar A. The esthetic properties of lips: A comparison of models and non models. Angle Orthod. 2004; 74: 162–166. pmid:15132441
  116. 116. Cunningham MR, Barbee AP, Pike CL. What do women want? Facial metric assessment of multiple motives in the perception of male facial physical attractiveness. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1990; 59: 61–72. pmid:2213490
  117. 117. Cunningham MR, Roberts AR, Barbee AP, Druen PB, Wu CH. “Their ideas of beauty are, on the whole, the same as ours”: Consistency and variability in the cross-cultural perception of female physical attractiveness. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995; 68: 261–279.
  118. 118. Rantala MJ, Coetzee V, Moore FR, Skrinda I, Kecko S, Krama T, et al. Facial attractiveness is related to women's cortisol and body fat, but not with immune responsiveness. Biol letters. 2013; 9: 20130255.
  119. 119. Tintlin RM, Watkins CD, Welling LLM, DeBruine LM, Al-Dujaili EAS, Jones BC. Perceived facial adiposity conveys information about women’s health. Brit J Psychol. 2012; 104: 235–248. pmid:23560669
  120. 120. Reither EN, Hauser RM, Swallen KC. Predicting adult health and mortality from adolescent facial characteristics in yearbook photographs. Demography. 2009; 46: 27–41. pmid:19348107
  121. 121. Little AC, Jones BC, Waitt C, Tiddeman BP, Feinberg DR, Perrett DI, et al. Symmetry is related to sexual dimorphism in faces: data across culture and species. PLoS ONE. 2008; 3: e2106. pmid:18461131
  122. 122. Feinberg DR, Jones BC, DeBruine LM, Moore FR, Law Smith MJ, Cornwell RE, et al. The voice and face of a woman: One ornament that signals quality? Evol Hum Behav. 2005; 26: 398–408.
  123. 123. Saxton TK, Burriss RP, Murray AK, Rowland HM, Roberts SC. Face, body and speech cues independently predict judgments of attractiveness. J Evol Psychol. 2009; 7: 23–35.
  124. 124. Van Dongen S. Fluctuating asymmetry and masculinity/femininity in humans: A meta-analysis. Arch Sex Behav. 2012; 41: 1453–1460. pmid:22437551
  125. 125. Campbell A. Female competition: Causes, constraints, content, and context. J Sex Res. 2004; 41: 16–26. pmid:15216421
  126. 126. Wood JW. Dynamics of human reproduction: Biology, biometry, demography. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter; 1994.
  127. 127. Wilson JM, Manning J. Fluctuating asymmetry and age in children: evolutionary implications for the control of developmental stability. J Hum Evol. 1996; 30: 529–537.
  128. 128. Hope D, Bates T, Penke L, Gow AJ, Starr JM, Deary IJ. Symmetry of the face in old age reflects childhood social status. Econ Hum Biol. 2013; 11: 236–244. pmid:21820367
  129. 129. Furnham A, Reeves E. The relative influence of facial neoteny and waist-to- hip ratio on judgements of female attractiveness and fecundity. Psychol Health Med. 2006; 11: 129–141. pmid:17129903