Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

“Attacks” or “Whistling”: Impact of Questionnaire Wording on Wheeze Prevalence Estimates

Abstract

Background

Estimates of prevalence of wheeze depend on questionnaires. However, wording of questions may vary between studies. We investigated effects of alternative wording on estimates of prevalence and severity of wheeze, and associations with risk factors.

Methods

White and South Asian children from a population-based cohort (UK) were randomly assigned to two groups and followed up at one, four and six years (1998, 2001, 2003). Parents were asked either if their child ever had “attacks of wheeze” (attack group, N=535), or “wheezing or whistling in the chest” (whistling group, N=2859). All other study aspects were identical, including questions about other respiratory symptoms.

Results

Prevalence of wheeze ever was lower in the attack group than in the whistling group for all surveys (32 vs. 40% in white children aged one year, p<0.001). Prevalence of other respiratory symptoms did not differ between groups. Wheeze tended to be more severe in the attack group. The strength of association with risk factors was comparable in the two groups.

Conclusions

The wording of questions on wheeze can affect estimates of prevalence, but has less impact on measured associations with risk factors. Question wording is a potential source of between-study-heterogeneity in meta-analyses.

Introduction

Questionnaires are the main tool used to estimate prevalence and time trends of wheezing in childhood [1, 2]. They are also widely used to study the natural history of wheeze, to determine risk factors of wheeze and to define wheeze phenotypes [38]. In most questionnaire surveys, parents are asked if the child has ever wheezed in life (wheeze ever), and if s/he has wheezed during the past 12 months (current wheeze). But questionnaires do not always use the same wording to assess wheeze. Some questionnaires used the phrase “attacks of wheeze” [912], while others (e.g., those used in the International Study on Asthma and Allergies in Childhood [ISAAC]) refer to wheeze as “wheezing or whistling in the chest” [13]. The first phrase describes wheezing as a discrete episode, while the second phrasing describes a broader range of symptom severity and patterns, and includes children with mild wheeze that does not present as attacks.

Given the breadth of the latter definition, one would expect higher prevalence estimates from surveys that use “wheezing or whistling in the chest” than from those that use “attacks of wheeze”. If phrasing affects prevalence estimates, then results from studies that formulate the question differently might not be comparable, and combining data across studies in meta-analyses may be misleading. To our knowledge, no previous study has tested this hypothesis and quantified the effects of differently worded questions on prevalence estimates of wheeze in preschool and young school children.

Our goal was to determine the effect of the wording of the question used to assess wheeze ever on prevalence estimates of wheeze, indicators of wheeze severity, and its associations with risk factors in a population-based respiratory cohort study.

Methods

Study design and study population

We analysed data from a population-based respiratory cohort from Leicestershire, UK, described in detail elsewhere [14]. In brief, the cohort consists of two sub-cohorts, each of which represents a random sample of all children born between 1993 and 1997 in the study area, stratified by ethnicity (white and South Asian). One sub-cohort included children born between May 1993 and April 1997, and was used to study time trends in prevalence of wheeze [15]. The key questions about respiratory symptoms were worded exactly the same in the questionnaires for these children as they had been in questionnaires used in a previous cohort of children, born eight years earlier [14]. The second sub-cohort included children, born between May 1996 and April 1997, who received a similar questionnaire. Most questions were the same as for the first group, but the wheeze ever question had the same wording as the ISAAC questionnaire [13].

Parents in both cohorts received respiratory questionnaires in 1998, 2001, 2003, 2006 and 2010. Parents in the second sub-cohort received an additional questionnaire in 1999. Apart from this, study methodology was largely identical for both sub-cohorts including: mailing dates, accompanying letter, questionnaire length and format, and wording of the large majority of questions.

For this study we used data from the 1998, 2001 and 2003 surveys. In order to ensure that the age distribution was comparable, while maximising the size of the two groups, we restricted the analysis to children born May 1st 1996—April 30th 1997 for both sub-cohorts.

Ethics statement

The Leicestershire Health Authority Research Ethics Committee (Leicester, UK) approved this study.

All information related to individuals from the cohort was made anonymous to investigators prior to analysis.

Questionnaires

The relevant questionnaire items for this study are published in the supporting information to this paper (S1 Fig).

The question related to wheeze ever differed between the sub-cohorts. The sub-cohort used for the study on time trends (referred to as ‘the attack group’) was asked the question: “Has your child ever had attacks of wheezing?” The other sub-cohort (referred to as ‘the whistling group’) was asked the question from the ISAAC questionnaire: “Has your child ever had wheezing or whistling in the chest at any time in the past?”

Both groups were asked identical questions asking about current wheeze and other respiratory symptoms related to the past 12 months, including night cough, chronic rhinitis and ear infections. All these items came from the ISAAC questionnaire [13], except for the question about ear infections. Questions about wheeze severity were also worded identically in both groups, namely activity disturbance, and sleep disturbance due to wheeze, as well as wheeze without colds. The same wording was also used in both groups for most risk factors assessed by questionnaire.

Statistical analyses

First, we assessed prevalence of wheeze ever, current wheeze, night cough, chronic rhinitis and any ear infection at ages one, four and six years in both groups. We used logistic regression and estimated odds ratios (ORs) to compare the groups. The questions about current wheeze and the other respiratory symptoms, which were worded identically, served as control questions, i.e. any prevalence differences between the sub-cohorts for these questions cannot be due to their wording. We estimated ORs with and without further adjustment for a range of potential risk factors, which may have been distributed unequally by chance between the groups: sex, exact age, breast feeding, nursery care, number of siblings, pre- and postnatal exposure to environmental tobacco smoke (ETS), parental asthma and parental hay fever, Townsend score (an area-based deprivation measure) [16] and parental education.

Second, we assessed prevalence of indicators of wheeze severity, namely activity disturbance and sleep disturbance due to wheeze, and having wheeze without a cold at ages one, four and six years.

Third, we investigated if the strength of association between wheeze ever and risk factors differed between groups. For this, we included interaction terms between group membership and the above-mentioned risk factors in multivariable logistic regressions. We used Stata 12.1 (Stata Corporation, Austin, Texas) to analyse the data.

White and South Asian children were analysed separately because these ethnic groups have previously been shown to have different symptoms and triggers of wheeze, lung function, parental understanding of wheeze and wheeze-related health service use [1721].

Results

Study population

A total of 5300 children received a questionnaire in 1998, and were born between May 1st 1996 and April 30th 1997. Of these, 4068 were white children and 1232 were South Asians (S2 Fig). The questionnaire that asked about attacks of wheeze was given to 1077 children. The questionnaire that asked about whistling in the chest was given to 4223 children. Response rates in 1998 were 78.5% (845/1077) for the attack group and 79.1% (3401/4223) for the whistling group. In 2001, response rates were 61.2% (633/1035) for the attack group and 64.7% (2632/4068) for the whistling group. In 2003, response rates were 50.0% (522/1054) for the attack group and 51.4% (2100/4082) for the whistling group. Less than 1.2% of questions about wheeze ever were missing for white children, and less than 2.4% of questions about wheeze ever were missing for South Asian children in both groups and for all survey years.

The attack group and the whistling group were similar in sex, age, prenatal and perinatal factors, environmental exposures, socioeconomic factors and parental history of atopic disorders (white children Table 1, South Asian children Table A in S1 File).

thumbnail
Table 1. Characteristics of the study population at age one year (white children).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131618.t001

Prevalence of wheeze and other respiratory symptoms

At age one year, prevalence of wheeze ever among white children was 31.8% in the attack group and 39.6% in the whistling group (p<0.001, Table 2). The absolute difference in prevalence was 7.8%, which corresponds to a 19.7% lower prevalence in the attack group than in the whistling group. The absolute difference in prevalence of wheeze ever at age four was 9.7%, and at age six it was 9.0%. Prevalence of current wheeze and other respiratory symptoms, which was determined by questions with identical wording, was comparable between the attack group and the whistling group at ages one, four and six (Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Prevalence of wheeze and other respiratory symptoms (white children).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131618.t002

Adjusting for risk factors for wheeze did not affect our findings at any age (Fig 1). At the age of one year, when we compared white children in the whistling group and the attack group, unadjusted OR for wheeze ever was 1.41 (95% CI 1.16, 1.72) and adjusted OR was 1.42 (1.12, 1.78). For current wheeze, unadjusted OR was 1.18 (0.97, 1.43) and adjusted OR was 1.22 (0.97, 1.54). Other respiratory questions were also similar between groups, with adjusted OR of 1.07 (0.82, 1.38) for night cough, 1.02 (0.81, 1.29) for chronic rhinitis, and 0.98 (0.79, 1.22) for any ear infection.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Crude and adjusted odds ratios for respiratory symptoms in white children.

The odds ratios compare the whistling group to the attack group (adjusted for sex, exact age, breast feeding, nursery care, number of siblings, pre- and postnatal exposure to environmental tobacco smoke (ETS), parental asthma and parental hay fever, Townsend score (an area-based deprivation measure) and parental education). The error bars denote 95% confidence intervals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131618.g001

South Asian children reported less wheeze ever than white children (Table B in S1 File). But, as in white children, prevalence of wheeze ever also differed between the attack group and the whistling group. There were absolute differences of 6.0% (age one year), 7.5% (age four years) and 11.0% (age six years), while prevalence of current wheeze and other respiratory symptoms were more similar at all ages between the two groups. ORs, with and without adjusting for confounders, were comparable (S3 Fig).

Severity of wheeze

Among white children aged one with wheeze, some indicators of severity of wheeze were more common in the attack group than in the whistling group (activity disturbance [16.6 vs. 9.7%] and sleep disturbance [64.2 vs 54.3%]). A severity difference was still seen at age four, but not at age six (Table 3). In South Asians, reported severity of wheeze varied little between the groups (Table C in S1 File).

thumbnail
Table 3. Prevalence of indicators of wheeze severity (white children).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131618.t003

Association with risk factors

There was little evidence that the strength of association between wheeze ever and potential risk factors differed between groups for white or for South Asian children: p-values for interaction tests were >0.1, except for parental asthma and hay fever in children aged one, and sex in children aged six years (whites only) (Table 4 and Tables D and E in S1 File). Adjusted results for the associations of the investigated risk factors with wheeze ever were comparable to unadjusted ones (data not shown).

thumbnail
Table 4. Association of different risk factors with wheeze ever in white children aged one year (unadjusted).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131618.t004

Discussion

Prevalence of wheeze ever in preschool and young school children was lower when parents were asked if their child had “attacks of wheezing” than when they were asked if their child had suffered from “wheezing or whistling in the chest”. The absolute difference in prevalence ranged from 8–10% in different age groups of white children. Among those who reported wheeze, severity of wheeze tended to be higher in the attack group than in the whistling group, particularly in one to two year-olds. In contrast, the strength of the association of wheeze ever with risk factors was less affected by the wording.

To our knowledge, this is the first study to systematically evaluate the way wording used to assess wheeze ever affects estimates of wheeze prevalence in preschool and young school children. In a Norwegian study, adults aged 15–70 years filled in two different respiratory questionnaires at an interval of about two hours. The authors found that prevalence of chest wheeze varied from 18.7% to 24.5%, depending on the wording of the question [22]. Ekerljung et al distributed booklets, each containing two respiratory questionnaires, to 16–75 year-old subjects in Sweden [23]. They observed that different wording of questions related to respiratory symptoms resulted in a difference in prevalence estimates of up to 8.8%, and that differences in question layout resulted in a difference up to 6.6%. Our results agree with findings of Patel et al, who reviewed global variations in wheeze prevalence in schoolchildren [24] and found that ISAAC studies reported significantly higher prevalence than non-ISAAC studies. However, their report did not account for regional variation of wheeze prevalence within countries [2527], and the studies they compared were different in many methodological aspects, which made it difficult to attribute their findings specifically to wording.

Our study included two fully comparable groups that differed only in the wording of the questionnaire they received. All children were randomly sampled at the same time, from the same sampling frame, and were simultaneously surveyed using otherwise identical methods. Thus our study setting was comparable to a randomized trial. Questions on current wheeze and other respiratory symptoms like night cough, rhinitis and ear infections were identically worded and could therefore be used as control questions. As expected, their prevalence was similar in both groups, and confirmed the comparability of the two groups regarding general respiratory morbidity.

We included a substantial number of South Asians in the study, a significant ethnic minority group in the UK. Findings were similar in both ethnic groups, suggesting that our results might generalize to non-white ethnic groups.

We based our study on English language questionnaires administered to preschool and young school children in the UK. We do not yet know how wording affects prevalence of wheeze in other languages or countries, or in older children.

Our results suggest that parents are more reluctant to confirm wheeze ever’ when the question includes the word “attack”. Observed differences persisted throughout early childhood. When wheeze prevalence estimates are compared, or when data are pooled in meta-analyses, it is therefore important to report whether or not questions were identical, and to consider the wording of questions to be a potential source of heterogeneity. Furthermore, the wording of questions should be carefully considered when children are classified into wheezing phenotypes. Reported severity of wheeze tended to be higher in children with “attacks” of wheeze. This supports our initial assumption that fewer children would be reported to have wheezing “attacks”, and would, on average, report more severe symptoms than children reported to have “wheezing or whistling in the chest”.

The strength of associations of wheeze ever with different risk factors was comparable between groups among white and South Asians children, at any age. Results went in the same direction and were of comparable effect size. Exceptions were parental asthma for white children aged one year, and sex of children aged six years, though these might have occurred by chance, given multiple testing. This suggests that wording is less likely to affect studies that investigate associations of wheeze with risk factors.

In summary, we found that the way the question on wheeze is worded can considerably affect estimates of prevalence of wheeze in young children. Wording had less impact on estimated strength of association with risk factors. The wording of the questionnaire must be considered as a potential source of between-study-heterogeneity in meta-analyses.

Supporting Information

S1 Fig. Original questions about wheeze and other respiratory symptoms.

The questions were sent to the attack group and the whistling group in 1998, 2001 and 2003 in Leicestershire, UK.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131618.s001

(TIFF)

S2 Fig. Response rate of study population for questionnaire surveys in 1998, 2001, and 2003.

At each survey, the attack group received the question, “Has your child ever had attacks of wheezing?” At each survey, the whistling group received the question. “Has your child ever had wheezing or whistling in the chest at any time in the past?”

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131618.s002

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Crude and adjusted odds ratios for respiratory symptoms in South Asian children.

The odds ratios compare the whistling group to the attack group (adjusted for sex, exact age, breast feeding, nursery care, number of siblings, pre- and postnatal exposure to environmental tobacco smoke (ETS), parental asthma and parental hay fever, Townsend score (an area-based deprivation measure) and parental education). The error bars denote 95% confidence intervals.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131618.s003

(TIF)

S1 File.

Table A in S1 File. Characteristics of the study population at age 1 year (South Asians). Table B in S1 File. Prevalence of wheeze and other respiratory symptoms (South Asian children). Table C in S1 File. Prevalence of indicators of wheeze severity (South Asian children). Table D in S1 File. Association of different risk factors with wheeze ever in white children aged 4 and 6 years (unadjusted). Table E in S1 File. Association of different risk factors with wheeze ever in South Asian children aged 1, 4 and 6 years (unadjusted)

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131618.s004

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank the parents of the Leicestershire respiratory cohort children for completing the questionnaires. We thank Parvinder Singh for her contribution to the statistical analyses and we thank Kali Tal for her editorial assistance.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: AMP BDS CEK. Performed the experiments: CSB CEK. Analyzed the data: AMP BDS. Wrote the paper: AMP BDS CSB CEK.

References

  1. 1. Pearce N, Ait-Khaled N, Beasley R, Mallol J, Keil U, Mitchell E, et al. Worldwide trends in the prevalence of asthma symptoms: phase III of the International Study of Asthma and Allergies in Childhood (ISAAC). Thorax. 2007;62(9):758–66. pmid:17504817
  2. 2. Rusconi F, Spycher B, Kuehni C. Epidemiology and phenotypes of bronchial asthma and wheezing disorders. ERS Handbook: Paediatric respiratory medicine European Respiratory Society; 2013. p. 293–7.
  3. 3. Brooke AM, Lambert PC, Burton PR, Clarke C, Luyt DK, Simpson H. The natural history of respiratory symptoms in preschool children. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 1995;152(6 Pt 1):1872–8. pmid:8520749
  4. 4. Neuman A, Hohmann C, Orsini N, Pershagen G, Eller E, Kjaer HF, et al. Maternal smoking in pregnancy and asthma in preschool children: a pooled analysis of eight birth cohorts. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2012;186(10):1037–43. pmid:22952297
  5. 5. Pescatore AM, Dogaru CM, Duembgen L, Silverman M, Gaillard EA, Spycher BD, et al. A simple asthma prediction tool for preschool children with wheeze or cough. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2014;133(1):111–8e13. pmid:23891353
  6. 6. Savenije OE, Granell R, Caudri D, Koppelman GH, Smit HA, Wijga A, et al. Comparison of childhood wheezing phenotypes in 2 birth cohorts: ALSPAC and PIAMA. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2011;127(6):1505–12e14. pmid:21411131
  7. 7. Sonnenschein-van der Voort AM, Arends LR, de Jongste JC, Annesi-Maesano I, Arshad SH, Barros H, et al. Preterm birth, infant weight gain, and childhood asthma risk: A meta-analysis of 147,000 European children. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2014.
  8. 8. Spycher BD, Silverman M, Brooke AM, Minder CE, Kuehni CE. Distinguishing phenotypes of childhood wheeze and cough using latent class analysis. The European respiratory journal: official journal of the European Society for Clinical Respiratory Physiology. 2008;31(5):974–81.
  9. 9. Anderson HR, Butland BK, Strachan DP. Trends in prevalence and severity of childhood asthma. BMJ. 1994;308(6944):1600–4. pmid:8025426
  10. 10. Luyt DK, Burton PR, Simpson H. Epidemiological study of wheeze, doctor diagnosed asthma, and cough in preschool children in Leicestershire. BMJ. 1993;306(6889):1386–90. pmid:8518607
  11. 11. Powell CV, Nolan TM, Carlin JB, Bennett CM, Johnson PD. Respiratory symptoms and duration of residence in immigrant teenagers living in Melbourne, Australia. Arch Dis Child. 1999;81(2):159–62. pmid:10490527
  12. 12. Venn A, Lewis S, Cooper M, Hill J, Britton J. Increasing prevalence of wheeze and asthma in Nottingham primary schoolchildren 1988–1995. The European respiratory journal: official journal of the European Society for Clinical Respiratory Physiology. 1998;11(6):1324–8.
  13. 13. Asher MI, Keil U, Anderson HR, Beasley R, Crane J, Martinez F, et al. International Study of Asthma and Allergies in Childhood (ISAAC): rationale and methods. The European respiratory journal: official journal of the European Society for Clinical Respiratory Physiology. 1995;8(3):483–91.
  14. 14. Kuehni CE, Brooke AM, Strippoli MP, Spycher BD, Davis A, Silverman M. Cohort profile: the Leicester respiratory cohorts. Int J Epidemiol. 2007;36(5):977–85. pmid:17911154
  15. 15. Kuehni CE, Davis A, Brooke AM, Silverman M. Are all wheezing disorders in very young (preschool) children increasing in prevalence? Lancet. 2001;357(9271):1821–5. pmid:11410189
  16. 16. Townsend P, Philimore P, Beattie A. Health and deprivation: inequality and the North. Bristol: Croom Helm; 1988.
  17. 17. Kuehni CE, Strippoli MP, Low N, Brooke AM, Silverman M. Wheeze and asthma prevalence and related health-service use in white and south Asian pre-schoolchildren in the United Kingdom. Clinical and experimental allergy: journal of the British Society for Allergy and Clinical Immunology. 2007;37(12):1738–46.
  18. 18. Kuehni CE, Strippoli MP, Silverman M. Food intolerance and wheezing in young South Asian and white children: prevalence and clinical significance. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2006;118(2):528–30. pmid:16890786
  19. 19. Michel G, Silverman M, Strippoli MP, Zwahlen M, Brooke AM, Grigg J, et al. Parental understanding of wheeze and its impact on asthma prevalence estimates. The European respiratory journal: official journal of the European Society for Clinical Respiratory Physiology. 2006;28(6):1124–30.
  20. 20. Netuveli G, Hurwitz B, Levy M, Fletcher M, Barnes G, Durham SR, et al. Ethnic variations in UK asthma frequency, morbidity, and health-service use: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet. 2005;365(9456):312–7. pmid:15664226
  21. 21. Strippoli MP, Kuehni CE, Dogaru CM, Spycher BD, McNally T, Silverman M, et al. Etiology of ethnic differences in childhood spirometry. Pediatrics. 2013;131(6):e1842–9. pmid:23713103
  22. 22. Brogger JC, Bakke PS, Gulsvik A. Comparison of respiratory symptoms questionnaires. Int J Tuberc Lung Dis. 2000;4(1):83–90. pmid:10654649
  23. 23. Ekerljung L, Ronmark E, Lotvall J, Wennergren G, Toren K, Lundback B. Questionnaire layout and wording influence prevalence and risk estimates of respiratory symptoms in a population cohort. The clinical respiratory journal. 2013;7(1):53–63. pmid:22243692
  24. 24. Patel SP, Jarvelin MR, Little MP. Systematic review of worldwide variations of the prevalence of wheezing symptoms in children. Environ Health. 2008;7:57. pmid:19000318
  25. 25. Variations in the prevalence of respiratory symptoms, self-reported asthma attacks, and use of asthma medication in the European Community Respiratory Health Survey (ECRHS). The European respiratory journal: official journal of the European Society for Clinical Respiratory Physiology. 1996;9(4):687–95.
  26. 26. Duran-Tauleria E, Rona RJ. Geographical and socioeconomic variation in the prevalence of asthma symptoms in English and Scottish children. Thorax. 1999;54(6):476–81. pmid:10334999
  27. 27. Krstic G. Asthma prevalence associated with geographical latitude and regional insolation in the United States of America and Australia. PLoS One. 2011;6(4):e18492. pmid:21494627