Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

A Longitudinal Investigation of the Relationship between Posttraumatic Stress Symptoms and Posttraumatic Growth in a Cohort of Israeli Jews and Palestinians during Ongoing Violence

  • Brian J. Hall ,

    brianhall@umac.mo

    Affiliations Department of Psychology, The University of Macau, Macau (SAR), People’s Republic of China, Department of Health, Behavior and Society, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, Maryland, United States of America

  • Leia Y. Saltzman,

    Affiliation School of Social Work, Boston College, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Daphna Canetti,

    Affiliation School of Political Science, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel

  • Stevan E. Hobfoll

    Affiliation Department of Behavioral Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois, United States of America

Abstract

Objectives

Meta-analytic evidence based on cross-sectional investigations between posttraumatic growth (PTG) and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) demonstrates that the two concepts are positively related and that ethnic minorities report greater PTG. Few longitudinal studies have quantified this relationship so the evidence is limited regarding the potential benefit PTG may have on post-traumatic adjustment and whether differences between ethnic groups exist.

Methods

The current study attempts to fill a substantial gap in the literature by exploring the relationship between PTG and PTSD symptom clusters longitudinally using a nationally representative cohort of 1613 Israelis and Palestinian Citizens of Israel (PCI) interviewed via telephone on three measurement occasions during one year. Latent cross-lagged structural models estimated the relationship between PTG and each PTSD symptom cluster, derived from confirmatory factor analysis, representing latent and statistically invariant PTSD symptom factors, best representing PTSD for both ethnic groups.

Results

PTG was not associated with less PTSD symptom severity in any of the four PTSD clusters, for Jews and PCI. In contrast, PTSD symptom severity assessed earlier was related to later reported PTG in both groups.

Conclusions

This study demonstrates that PTSD symptoms contribute to greater reported PTG, but that PTG does not provide a salutatory benefit by reducing symptoms of PTSD.

Introduction

The Al Aqsa Intifada, a period of conflict in Israel lasting close to five years, produced an environment in which high numbers of civilians were repeatedly exposed to bombings and terror attacks. Bleich, Gelkopf, and Solomon [1] found that nearly half of a nationally representative sample of Israelis reported direct, or indirect (through family member or friend), exposure to terrorism. The high levels of exposure among the general population highlighted the clinical significance of understanding the nuances in the association between mental health outcomes and repeated, and long lasting, exposure to terrorism.

Studies have documented the association between exposure to political violence and terrorism and mental health problems [24]. Among the most investigated of these problems is posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Although it is important to evaluate the pathological responses to exposure, it is now well known that relatively few people exposed to terrorism develop PTSD [5], even in populations that face continued exposure and chronic conditions of deprivation [6]. A burgeoning effort has been undertaken to expand Tedeschi and Calhoun’s [7] theoretical work on possible positive adaptations that emerge as a result of coping with potentially traumatic events (PTE), a construct they named posttraumatic growth (PTG).

PTG is broadly defined as a “positive psychological change experienced as a result of the struggle with highly challenging life circumstances” and trauma [8]. PTG does not solely involve a return to pre-trauma levels of functioning (e.g. resiliency [9]), nor does it speak to the capacity to resist developing psychological disorders [10]. Rather, it is defined as achieving an enhanced level of functioning, sense of meaning or spirituality, and developing closer relationships with others that exceed pre-trauma functioning. In order for growth to occur, Tedeschi and Calhoun [7] posited that some level of distress would be necessary to bring about cognitive processes needed to facilitate growth. To what extent psychological distress is related to PTG overtime, and whether PTG may lead to reductions in psychological distress has not been widely explored in the literature

PTG and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

The relationship between PTG and psychological distress is one of the most widely debated issues in the PTG literature. In their early theoretical contributions, Tedeschi and Calhoun [7] suggested that an individual can report growth and psychological distress at the same time, and that these two dimensions are unrelated to each other. However, few studies support the viewpoint that PTG and psychological distress are unrelated [11, 12].

Cross-sectional studies have demonstrated inconsistent empirical findings regarding the relationship between PTG and PTSD symptom severity. Several studies suggest that PTG is positively associated with PTSD symptom severity such that greater PTG is related to greater PTSD symptom severity [10, 1315]. While others have demonstrated that PTG is associated negatively with PTSD symptom severity such that more PTG is related to less PTSD symptom severity [16, 17].

A recent meta-analysis of 42 studies (N = 11,469) provided greater clarity on this association and demonstrated a significant positive relationship between PTSD and PTG (r = .315) [18]. However, the cross-sectional nature of these studies limits the utility of these findings, and fails to solidify the temporal nature and quality (i.e., positive or negative) of the relationship between PTSD symptom severity and PTG. Longitudinal studies can elucidate these relationships further, and allow for testing of several potential temporal associations such that: 1) PTG leads to less PTSD symptom severity, 2) PTG leads to greater PTSD symptom severity, 3) PTSD symptom severity leads to greater PTG, 4) PTG and PTSD symptom severity are unrelated, and 5) PTSD symptom severity leads to less PTG, an association that has received less theoretical and empirical support.

Only two published studies examined the relationship between PTG and PTSD over time [19, 20]. These studies supported the 3rd pathway described above, demonstrating that PTSD symptom severity is associated with greater PTG subsequently reported. However, one study did not have measurements of the main study variables available at each time point [19]. Much of the literature has focused on the overall PTSD construct without considering variation between PTG and symptom clusters of PTSD. Meta-analytic findings suggest that intrusive thoughts are most associated with PTG [25], so modeling reexperiencing symptoms separately from other PTSD symptoms utilizing a longitudinal study design may provide further support for a unique association. However, results from a longitudinal study examining the association between PTG and each PTSD symptom clusters among Katrina survivors [20], demonstrated consistent associations between each symptom cluster and PTG over time.

Multicultural Considerations in Posttraumatic Growth

The population in Israel is comprised of distinct ethno-cultural groups (i.e., Jews and Palestinian Citizens of Israel; PCI), each with their own unique ideology, religion, and ties to the land. It is therefore important to assess whether PTG and PTSD symptom severity are related in the same way for Jews and PCI. In order to study these relationships, a necessary precondition is to establish the factor structure of PTSD within these samples, and to formally assess measurement invariance between these groups. Only one known study has examined the factor structure of PTSD within an Israeli sample of adults, but did not distinguish between Jewish and PCI respondents [21] and only two have examined the factor structure of PTSD within the context of terrorism [21, 22].

The relationship between PTG and psychological distress may emerge differently for ethnic minority groups [4]. Furthermore, the latent concepts of PTG and PTSD may be influenced by culture, and therefore may differ across different ethnic groups [23, 24]. Minority group members in the United States report greater prevalence of PTG, and minority status is a significant moderator of the relationship between PTG and mental health [25]. A meta-analysis showed that when compared to samples with mostly White participants, samples with greater ethnic minority composition experienced less depression and reported greater well-being as their reported PTG increased. Furthermore, PTG was related to less global distress for these samples, and PTG was related to greater global distress for majority White samples. However, these results are not specific to minority group members, and studies are needed that model this association specifically for minorities. In the longitudinal study conducted among Katrina survivors described above [20], participants identifying as non-Hispanic Black were more likely to report PTG, but effect modification by ethnicity was not evaluated. Therefore, modeling minority status as a potential moderator between PTG and PTSD symptom severity, especially in longitudinal studies, is warranted.

The Present Study

Given only two known studies have examined the relationship between PTG and PTSD symptom severity generally [19, 20] and only one specifically within the context of ongoing exposure to terrorism [19], the present study aims to provide additional information about this association over time. The latent dimensions of PTSD are also evaluated with regard to this over-time association, which presents a more nuanced approach to understanding the association between PTSD and PTG among those living in environments where repeated exposure is likely. Therefore, the purpose of the present study is to evaluate the relationships between PTSD symptom severity and PTG within a longitudinal framework. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used to first establish best fitting latent models for PTSD and PTG to ensure that the measurement of each latent construct was appropriate for use in the Jewish and PCI populations. Furthermore, ethnicity was examined as a potential moderator of the temporal relationship between PTG and PTSD symptom severity. In light of previous longitudinal research, we predicted that symptom severity across each PTSD symptom cluster would be associated with increased PTG over time [19, 20].

Method

Procedure and Participants. A nationally representative sample of 1613 Israelis was randomly surveyed. Comprehensive landline telephone lists (98% of all numbers in Israel) from the Israeli telephone company (Bezeq) database were used. Stratification by region ensured population representation for Jews and PCI. Phone interviews were conducted on three measurement occasions (August 17-September 8, 2004; February 22-March 13, 2005; July 31-October 9, 2005) by a survey institute in Israel using a structured questionnaire [26]. All scales were translated and back translated by language experts and were completed by participants in 30–40 minutes. Initial contact was made by a Hebrew speaker; Arabic and Russian speakers were available if individuals did not speak Hebrew and callbacks were arranged within 24 hours if a Russian or Arabic speaker was not immediately available. Fifteen attempts were made to contact an adult at each telephone number. At the onset of the interview, verbal informed consent was obtained due to the anonymous nature of the telephone survey. Consent was recorded by the survey institute and linked to the survey respondent ID. This study and verbal consent method was approved by the institutional review boards of Kent State University, University of Haifa, and Rush University Medical Center and conducted in compliance with institutional review board protocols. The response rate for eligible participants was 57% [15, 27]. This rate compared favorably with other studies conducted on adults following the September 11, terror attacks within the United States [28]. Participation rates between 30% and 70% are only weakly associated with survey bias, and any potential bias in sampling is addressed by examining the representativeness of the obtained sample [29]. There were no statistical differences between the current sample and the 2003 Israeli Census in terms of sex, ethnicity, age, and education. Of the 1613 (1136 Jews; 477 PCI) participants in the first wave of data collection, 840 (52% overall; 54% Jew; 48% PCI) were retained for 6-month follow-up. The majority of these individuals (716 people, 85% overall; 94% Jews; 62% PCI) were retained at 12-month follow-up.

Measures

Demographic information was obtained regarding participants’ age, sex, income, and educational attainment.

Exposure to terrorism.

Participants’ level of exposure was assessed using several items designed to gauge the specific nature of their experiences ranging from the death of a family member or friend, to being in the target location within 48 hours prior to the terror attack. One summed score was created for each measurement occasion that represented the total number of exposures to terrorism, the total number of exposures within three months prior the start of the study (measured at baseline), or since the last measurement occasion for 6-month and 12-month follow-up.

PTSD.

PTSD symptom severity was measured using the posttraumatic stress disorder symptom scale, interview format (PSS-I) [30]. This scale was validated for use in telephone surveys with Jewish and Palestinian populations in Israel [31]. Participants reported on the severity of PTSD symptoms occurring for at least one month relating to experiences involving a terrorist attack. The PSS-I contains 17 items that assess PTSD symptom criteria based on the DSM-IV-TR [32], including items representing reexperiencing, avoidance, and hyperarousal symptom clusters. Items were answered on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 not at all to 3 extremely. Cronbach alpha was. 85 or higher at each study wave.

Posttraumatic growth.

Four items, rated from 0 not at all to 3 extremely from the COR-Evaluation [33] were used to assess posttraumatic growth. This brief scale captures growth in the three domains; self-perception, interpersonal relationships, and philosophy of life; and demonstrated adequate psychometric properties (α = .82) in studies previously conducted in Israel [26]. This brief scale has also directly compared to Tedeschi and Calhoun’s [34] Posttraumatic Growth Inventory (PTGI) and was found to be highly correlated in a college student population (r = .85) [17].

Statistical analysis

Confirmatory factor analyses.

Factor analyses were conducted on the baseline sample to evaluate measurement models for PTG and PTSD to establish whether these constructs were invariant between ethnic groups. The configural, metric and temporal invariance of PTG and PTSD was also evaluated in multigroup longitudinal measurement models. Establishing metric invariance between groups is a necessary precondition for testing multigroup structural models. If model parameters are non-invariant, the structural pathways cannot be compared, as equivalent constructs do not exist in each group.

Fully latent cross-lagged panel analyses.

Panel analyses were conducted to evaluate 1) the temporal relationship between PTG and PTSD symptoms and 2) if the relationship over time is consistent among Jewish and PCI groups. Structural models examined the association between PTG and each symptom cluster of PTSD separately due to non-convergence resulting from the statistical complexity of the models including simultaneous regressions with fully latent PTSD.

The cross-lagged model paths were evaluated in a multistep nested models procedure starting with the saturated model (i.e., autoregressive paths, cross-lagged paths, and correlated like-item residuals free to vary between groups). Four additional models were specified, each constraining one pathway at equivalence for Jews and PCI. These models were used to evaluate whether a cross-lagged pathway was equal in terms of significance and strength between Jews and PCI.

Analyses were conducted using Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) for missing data (covariance coverage range 25% to 100%—current study), MLR estimation (i.e., robust methods) was used for non-normally distributed data, using Mplus version 5.1. Four goodness-of-fit indices were used to evaluate the adequacy of the CFA and structural models: the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker Lewis Index (TLI), the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), and the root mean square of approximation (RMSEA). Values equal to, or greater than,. 90 for the CFI and TLI, and values lower than. 08 for the RMSEA and SRMR, were considered indicators of good model fit [35]. Nested CFA and cross-lagged models were compared using Sattora-Bentler χ2 difference test (S-B Δχ2) incorporating the scaling correction factor for non-normal data provided by Mplus, with a p value set at. 05.

Results

Descriptive and Bivariate Analyses

Table 1 presents the demographic composition of the sample for Jews (N = 1136) and PCI (N = 477) and presents the means, standard deviations, and percentages of study variables.

thumbnail
Table 1. Means, standard deviations of study variables and demographic composition of the sample.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124782.t001

Measurement Model for PTSD

The four factor model of PTSD with correlated latent factors provided adequate fit for Jews (χ2(112, N = 1136) = 369.217, CFI = .93, TLI = .91, RMSEA = .045 (.040 -. 050) SRMR = .039) and PCI (χ2(112, N = 477) = 238.120, CFI = .91, TLI = .88, RMSEA = .049 (.040 -. 057) SRMR = .05). Factor loadings for each of the four latent factors were significant, and above. 44 for Jews and PCI, save for numbing symptoms for the PCI sample. Modification indices called for the residual error for numbing items (i.e., feeling of detachment or estrangement from others and restricted range of affect) to be freely estimated. This pathway was introduced in the models given the considerable conceptual overlap of these items and the improvement in model fit.

Measurement Model for PTG

A single factor model of PTG fit the data well for Jews χ2(2, N = 1136) = 10.08, CFI = .99, TLI = .96, RMSEA = .06 (.03 -. 09) SRMR = .02, and for PCI χ2(2, N = 477) = 10.56, CFI = .98, TLI = .92, RMSEA = .09 (.04 -. 15), SRMR = .03. Factor loadings for the four PTG items for Jews and PCI respectively were. 57/.51 for “Feeling that my life has meaning,”. 78/.72 for “Intimacy with one or more family members,”. 75/.75 for “Closer relations with my friends,” and. 62/,72 for “More confidence in my ability to do things.”

Cross-Lagged Panel Analyses

Reexperiencing symptoms and PTG.

As can be seen in Table 2, configural, temporal and metric invariance was maintained across the two study groups, which allowed for an evaluation of the cross-lagged models. The path from T1 PTG to T2 reexperiencing was not significant for Jews (β = -0.01) or PCI (β = -0.04). The path from T1 reexperiencing to T2 PTG was significant for Jews (β = .15, p = .004) and PCI (β = .17, p = .04). The path from T2 PTG to T3 reexperiencing symptoms was not significant for Jews (β = -0.04) or PCI (β = 0.07). Finally, the path from T2 reexperiencing to T3 PTG was significant for Jews (β = 0.26, p <. 001) but not for PCI (β = 0.08). Other than this final pathway, no differences were noted between the Jewish and PCI samples. This fully saturated model is depicted by Fig 1.

thumbnail
Table 2. Multigroup Cross-lagged Analysis of Reexperiencing Symptoms.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124782.t002

thumbnail
Fig 1. Standardized structural equation modeling results of the saturated fully cross-lagged model demonstrating the temporal relationship between reexperiencing symptoms and posttraumatic growth.

All bold cross-lagged model paths are significant for one or both groups in the multigroup models. Dashed lines indicate non-significant path coefficients for both groups. * p <. 05. ** p <. 01. Values to the left of the diagonal line represent standardized path coefficients for Jews, and values to the right represent standardized path coefficients for PCI. PTG = posttraumatic growth. PTSD—B = posttraumatic stress disorder reexperiencing symptoms.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124782.g001

Avoidance symptoms and PTG.

As shown in Table 3, configural, temporal and metric invariance was maintained across the two study groups, which allowed for an evaluation of the cross-lagged models. The path from T1 PTG to T2 avoidance symptoms were non-significant Jews (β = 0.05) and PCI (β = 0.10). The path from T1 avoidance to T2 PTG was significant for Jews (β = .13, p = .04) but not for PCI (β = .02). The path from T2 PTG to T3 avoidance symptoms was not significant for Jews (β = -0.01) or PCI (β = .06). In the final model, the path from T2 avoidance symptoms to T3 PTG was significant for Jews (β = 0.23, p = .002) but not for PCI (β = .16). Despite differences in the significance of model paths, when paths were constrained at equality for both groups, no significant differences were noted. This fully saturated model is depicted by Fig 2.

thumbnail
Table 3. Multigroup Cross-lagged Analysis of Behavioral Avoidance Symptoms.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124782.t003

thumbnail
Fig 2. Standardized structural equation modeling results of the saturated fully cross-lagged model demonstrating the temporal relationship between avoidance symptoms and posttraumatic growth.

All bold cross-lagged model paths are significant for one or both groups in the multigroup models. Dashed lines indicate non-significant path coefficients for both groups. * p <. 05. ** p <. 01. Values to the left of the diagonal line represent standardized coefficients for Jews, and values to the right represent standardized path coefficients for PCI. PTG = posttraumatic growth. PTSD—A = posttraumatic stress disorder avoidance symptoms.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124782.g002

Emotional numbing symptoms and PTG.

As shown in Table 4, configural, temporal and metric invariance was maintained across the two study groups, which allowed for an evaluation of the cross-lagged models. The path from T1 PTG to T2 numbing symptoms Jews was (β = -0.10, p = .07) and PCI (β = -0.04, p = .67). T1 numbing to T2 PTG Jews was (β = .07, p = .25) and PCI (β = .29, p = .001). The path was significantly different between these groups.

thumbnail
Table 4. Multigroup Cross-lagged Analysis of Emotional Numbing Symptoms.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124782.t004

The path from T2 PTG to T3 numbing symptoms for Jews was (β = -.04, p = .59) and PCI (β = .15, p = .27). In the final model, the path from T2 numbing symptoms to T3 PTG for Jews was (β = 0.04, p = .50) and PCI (β = .17, p = .19). This fully saturated model is depicted by Fig 3.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Standardized structural equation modeling results of the saturated fully cross-lagged model demonstrating the temporal relationship between numbing symptoms and posttraumatic growth.

All bold cross-lagged model paths are significant for one or both groups in the multigroup models. Dashed lines indicate non-significant path coefficients for both groups. * p <. 05. ** p <. 01. Values to the left of the diagonal line represent standardized coefficients for Jews, and values to the right represent standardized path coefficients for PCI. PTG = posttraumatic growth. PTSD—N = posttraumatic stress disorder numbing symptoms.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124782.g003

Hyperarousal symptoms and PTG.

As shown in Table 5, configural and temporal invariance was found between the two groups. However, metric invariance was not was upheld and therefore the multigroup cross-lagged model could not be estimated. In order to evaluate the structural relationship between hyperarousal and PTG, cross-lagged models were specified separately for Jews and PCI.

thumbnail
Table 5. Multigroup results for cross-lagged analysis of Hyperarousal Symptoms.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124782.t005

Hyperarousal and PTG: Jews.

In order to test whether the individual cross-lagged paths meaningfully contributed to the model, structural pathways were deleted starting with the T2 variables regressed on T1 variables, and then for the T3 variables regressed on T2 variables.

First, in Model 2, fixing the path from T1 PTG to T2 hyperarousal symptoms to zero yielded a non-significant result, S-B Δχ2(1, N = 1136) = 0.99. This indicated that this path did not contribute to the model, and that PTG did not predict later hyperarousal symptoms for Jews. The non-significant model path from the fully saturated model was (β = -0.05, p = .26).

In model 3, the paths from T1 hyperarousal to T2 PTG were fixed at zero. There was a significant difference between the saturated and constrained models, S-B Δχ2(1, N = 1136) = 7.22 p <. 001. This indicated that this path contributed to the model. The significant model path from the fully saturated model was (β = 0.13, p = .004), which demonstrated a small effect size relationship between T1 hyperarousal predicting T2 PTG for Jews.

In model 4, the paths from T2 PTG to T3 hyperarousal was fixed at zero. There were no significant differences between the saturated and constrained models, S-B Δχ2(1, N = 1136) = -0.11, p >. 05. This indicated that this path did not contribute to the model, and that T2 PTG did not predict T3 hyperarousal for Jews. The non-significant model path from the fully saturated model was (β = 0.04, p = .57).

In model 5, the path from T2 hyperarousal to T3 PTG was fixed at zero. Results revealed a significant difference between the saturated and constrained models, S-B Δχ2(1, N = 1136) = 10.18, p <. 001. This indicated that this path contributed to the model. The significant model path from the fully saturated model was (β = 0.14, p = .03), which indicated that T2 hyperarousal predicted T3 PTG for Jews. This fully saturated model is depicted by Fig 4.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Standardized structural equation modeling results of the saturated fully cross-lagged model demonstrating the temporal relationship between hyperarousal symptoms and posttraumatic growth for the Jewish Sample.

All bold cross-lagged model paths are significant for the models. Dashed lines indicate non-significant path coefficients. * p <. 05. ** p <. 01. PTG = posttraumatic growth. PTSD—H = posttraumatic stress disorder hyperarousal symptoms.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124782.g004

Hyperarousal and PTG: PCIs.

The first and most saturated cross-lagged model allowed all cross-lagged structural paths to be freely estimated. Covariances between latent variables at each measurement occasion were also freely estimated. This initial model fit the data well, χ2(297, N = 477) = 471.51, CFI = .90, TLI = .89, RMSEA = .029 (.029 -. 040), SRMR = .08, BIC 23736.90. This fully saturated model is depicted by Fig 5.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Standardized structural equation modeling results of the saturated fully cross-lagged model demonstrating the temporal relationship between hyperarousal symptoms and posttraumatic growth for the PCI Sample.

All bold cross-lagged model paths are significant for the models. Dashed lines indicate non-significant path coefficients. Asterisks indicate significance values for covariances and autoregressive paths. * p <. 05. ** p <. 01. PTG = posttraumatic growth. PTSD—H = posttraumatic stress disorder hyperarousal symptoms.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124782.g005

In order to test whether the individual cross-lagged paths meaningfully contributed to the model, structural pathways were deleted starting with the T2 variables regressed on T1 variables, and then for the T3 variables regressed on T2 variables. First, in Model 2, fixing the path from T1 PTG to T2 hyperarousal symptoms to zero yielded a non-significant result, S-B Δχ2(1, N = 477) = 1.65, p >. 05. This indicated that this path did not contribute to the model, and that PTG did not predict later hyperarousal symptoms for PCI. The non-significant model path from the fully saturated model was (β = 0.12, p = .15).

In model 3, the paths from T1 hyperarousal to T2 PTG were fixed at zero. Results failed to reveal a significant difference between the saturated and constrained models, S-B Δχ2(1, N = 477) = 3.39. This indicated that this path did not contribute to the model, and that T1 hyperarousal did not predict T2 PTG for PCI. The non-significant model path from the fully saturated model was (β = 0.15, p = .05).

In model 4, the paths from T2 PTG to T3 hyperarousal was fixed at zero. Results revealed no significant differences between the saturated and constrained models, S-B Δχ2(1, N = 477) = -.03. This indicated that this path did not contribute to the model, and that T2 PTG did not predict T3 numbing for PCI. The non-significant model path from the fully saturated model was (β = 0.02, p = .83). In model 5, the path from T2 numbing to T3 PTG was fixed at zero. Results revealed no significant differences between the saturated and constrained models, S-B Δχ2(1, N = 477) = 2.64. This indicated that this path did not contribute to the model, and that T2 hyperarousal did not predict T3 PTG for PCI. The non-significant model path from the fully saturated model was (β = 0.24, p = .07).

Discussion

Utilizing data gathered from a representative sample of Israeli Jews and PCI, the results supported a generally cross-culturally sound representation of PTSD represented by a four-factor correlated model of reexperiencing, avoidance, emotional numbing, and hyperarousal [36]. Importantly, each of these factors, save for hyperarousal, were equivalently measured between Jews and PCI. PTG was not associated with less PTSD symptom severity in any of the four PTSD clusters, for both Jews and PCI. In contrast, PTSD symptom severity was generally predictive of PTG such that PTSD assessed earlier was related to later reported PTG in both groups, calling into question the salutogenic utility of the PTG construct.

The Best Fitting Model of PTSD and PTG for Jews and PCIs

The results demonstrated that PTSD evidenced adequate cross-cultural generalizability. Three of the four symptom clusters for PTSD were measured equivalently between the Jewish and PCI samples. The symptoms of reexperiencing, avoidance, and emotional numbing remained unchanged over time, and the strength of the factor loadings was invariant across groups. A different picture emerged for the hyperarousal symptom cluster. Although this construct demonstrated temporal stability, the strength of the associations among the factor loadings was heterogeneous across groups. Although the models were not entirely equivalent in terms of the strength of the parameter estimates across groups, the four-factor model was the best fitting among those that were tested.

The PTG construct was equivalently measured between the Jewish and PCI samples. For each group, the four-items loaded onto one latent factor, and this model fit the data reasonably well for both groups. This investigation, although limited by the few items in the scale, provides evidence that the PTG construct was measured adequately within a sample that has experienced ongoing exposure or threat of exposure to terrorism. It also provides evidence that the construct evidenced generalizability between samples and this allowed multigroup analysis to proceed.

Evaluating the Relationship between PTSD and PTG

The results of the cross-lagged models for PTSD and PTG demonstrated a clear and relatively consistent finding: PTSD symptom cluster severity was associated with greater concurrent and subsequent PTG. At no point was self-reported PTG related to less PTSD symptom cluster severity either cross-sectionally or prospectively. These findings are consistent with previous studies measuring the association between PTSD and PTG [10, 1315, 1820] and add to the growing number of studies suggesting that PTG and psychopathology are positively related to one another within terrorism contexts [10, 14, 15, 18, 27].

Specific associations between PTSD symptom clusters and PTG revealed that reexperiencing, avoidance, and hyperarousal symptoms were consistently associated with greater PTG between both measurement periods for the Jewish sample, but between T1 and T2 and between T2 and T3, and at neither time point, respectively, for the PCI. The association between reexperiencing and hyperarousal and PTG supports theory suggesting rumination or cognitive processing is needed for growth to occur [7]. Avoidance symptoms may be related to denial or represent an illusory side of PTG posited by researchers who claim that reported PTG can be truly related to personal growth and also to self-deception [37, 38]. Numbing symptoms were not consistently associated with PTG. This comports well with the theory that cognitive processing is needed for PTG to occur [7].

The effect sizes for the cross-sectional associations when significant were medium [39], and stronger than the cross-lagged effects. This is not surprising but notable, and suggests that cross-sectional studies may exaggerate the relationship between PTSD and PTG. Although this study does not speak directly to causality, these findings are highly suggestive that PTG does not lead to positive mental health outcomes.

Ethnicity as a Moderator of the Relationship between PTG and PTSD

Within the context of the current study, ethnic minority status, as defined as being a PCI rather than a Jewish Israeli, did not lead to a different general pattern of association between PTG and any of the PTSD symptom clusters. However, the association between PTG and the PTSD symptom clusters was not as strong for the PCI sample compared to the Jewish sample. When significant associations were noted, they only existed between T1 and T2, and not from T2 to T3. The only association that was significant in the PCI sample, and not in the Jewish sample, was between emotional numbing and PTG. For PCI, numbing at T1 was related to greater PTG at T2. Overall, the PCI sample did not evidence a reduction in the severity of any PTSD symptom cluster as a result of self-reported PTG. This finding is consistent with previous research in Israel [10, 14, 15, 27], but contradictory to studies conducted within the United States [25].

Although PTG demonstrated temporal metric invariance between the two samples, PTG was not stable over time for PCI. The autoregressive pathway for PTG between T2 and T3 was not significant in the models. This indicated a lack of stability (i.e., test retest reliability) for this construct between the later follow-up periods. It may be possible that as this group gained temporal distance from the Al Aqsa Intifada period, the PTG that they reported began to weaken.

Strengths and Limitations of the Current Study

This study has several notable strengths. The longitudinal design tested the temporal relationships between PTSD symptom clusters and PTG and this overcomes a serious limitation of the majority of studies testing these associations. A second strength was to evaluate the factor structure of PTSD and PTG and to demonstrate the equivalence of these constructs between different cultural groups. Additionally, the large sample size allowed for ample statistical power to examine the multigroup models reliably. Further, this study extends the literature by examining these processes in a sample that has experienced ongoing and repeated psychological trauma. Taken together, this study contributes significantly to our understanding of how PTSD and PTG are related, the cross-cultural generalizability of PTSD, and the role of ethnic minority status in this relationship.

Despite its strengths, this study also had several limitations. The study design, although longitudinal, cannot discern strict casual relatedness among the study variables since the effects of unmeasured influences cannot be ruled out. In particular, participants’ subjective report of the severity and centrality of their traumatic event exposure, a well documented predictor of mental health outcomes [4042], was not measured. It may be possible that variation exists in the degree to which terrorism exposure is experienced by our participants as traumatic and may explain variations in the extent to which PTG was expressed [8].

Furthermore, this study captures one discrete period of time characterized by the pressure of ongoing threat of further attacks. As a result, PTG in this context may be different than growth reported following single-incident trauma exposure [10]. In particular, this environment may provide little opportunity of entering a resolution phase where individuals are free from psychopathology, at which point their PTSD symptoms would likely be unrelated to PTG [8].

The limited number of items included in the PTG measure restricted our ability to assess the relationship between PTSD and PTG. Although the items used in this study to measure PTG were found to correlate highly with the PTGI [34], the full dimensionality of the construct was not captured by these four items. As a result, conclusions about the relationship between PTSD and PTG can be made for only three of the five domains of PTG commonly studied. This study is unable to speak to the potential relationship between PTSD and growth in the areas of spirituality and new life possibilities. Furthermore this study did not account for a potential non-linear relationship (quadratic) relationship between PTSD symptom severity and PTG [18].

Conclusions

Research has documented that the majority of people who experience a major traumatic event, such as terrorism, are resilient [9]. However, the concept of growth reaches beyond resiliency and provides hope for an alternative outcome to PTSD and psychological suffering following trauma. As a result it is essential to understand the relationship between growth and distress over time, and to determine if the relationship varies between majority and minority status populations. This paper contributes to the existing literature by demonstrating that PTSD symptom severity, across symptom clusters and for two ethic groups, contributed to increased self-reported PTG, but that at no time did PTG lead to reduced symptom severity.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Alyson Zalta, PhD for comments she provided on an earlier manuscript draft. We thank Kalon Sou, MSc, for his assistance in the preparation of the manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: BJH DC SEH. Performed the experiments: BJH DC SEH. Analyzed the data: BJH. Wrote the paper: BJH LS DC SEH.

References

  1. 1. Bleich A, Gelkopf M, Solomon Z. Exposure to terrorism, stress-related mental health symptoms and coping behaviors among a nationally representative sample in Israel. Journal of American Medical Association. 2003; 290: 612–620.
  2. 2. Heath EM, Hall BJ, Canetti D, Hobfoll SE. Exposure to political violence, psychological distress, resource loss, and benefit findings as predictors of domestic violence among Palestinians. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy. 2013; 5: 366–376.
  3. 3. Johnson RJ, Caneti D, Palmieri PA, Galea S, Varley J, Hobfoll SE. A prospective study of risk and resilience factors associated with posttraumatic stress symptoms and depression symptoms among Jews and Arabs exposed to repeated acts of terrorism in Israel. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy. 2009; 1: 291–311.
  4. 4. Tracy M, Hobfoll SE, Canetti-Nisim D, Galea S. Predictors of depressive symptoms among Israeli Jews and Arabs during the Al Aqsa intifada: A population based cohort study. Annals of Epidemiology. 2008; 18: 447–457 pmid:18261923
  5. 5. Bonanno GA, Westphal M, Mancini AD. Resilience to Loss and Potential Trauma. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2010; 7: 511–535.
  6. 6. Hobfoll SE, Mancini AD, Hall BJ, Canetti D, Bonanno GA. The limits of resilience: Distress following chronic political violence among Palestinians. Social Science and Medicine. 2011; 72:1400–1408. pmid:21440348
  7. 7. Tedeschi RG, Calhoun LG. Trauma and Transformation: growing in the aftermath of Suffering. Thousand Oaks, California: Sage; 1995.
  8. 8. Tedeschi RG, Calhoun LG. Target Article: "Posttraumatic Growth: Conceptual Foundations and Empirical Evidence." Psychological Inquiry. 2004; 15: 1–18.
  9. 9. Bonanno GA. Resilience in the face of potential trauma. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2005; 14: 135–138.
  10. 10. Hobfoll SE, Palmieri PA, Johnson RJ, Canetti-Nisim D, Hall BJ, Galea S. Trajectories of Resilience, Resistance and Distress during Ongoing Terrorism: The Case of Jews and Arabs in Israel. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2009; 77: 138–148. pmid:19170460
  11. 11. Cordova MJ, Giese-Davis J, Golant M, Kronenwetter C, Chang V, Spiegel D. Breast cancer as trauma: Posttraumatic stress and posttraumatic growth. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings. 2007; 14: 308–319.
  12. 12. Smith SK, Samsa G, Ganz PA, Zimmerman S. Is there a relationship between posttraumatic stress and growth after a lymphoma diagnosis? Psycho-Oncology. 2014; 23: 315–321 pmid:24123368
  13. 13. Dekel R, Nuttman-Shwartz O. Posttraumatic stress and growth: The contribution of cognitive appraisal and sense of belonging to the country. Health and Social Work. 2009; 34: 87–96. pmid:19425338
  14. 14. Hall BJ, Hobfoll SE, Canetti D, Johnson RJ, Palmieri PA, Galea S. Exploring the association between posttraumatic growth and PTSD: A national study of Jews and Arabs during the 2006 Israeli-Hezbollah War. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2010; 198: 180–186. pmid:20215994
  15. 15. Hobfoll SE, Canetti-Nisim D, Johnson RJ, Palmieri PA, Varley JD, Galea S. The association of exposure, risk, and resiliency factors with PTSD among Jews and Arabs exposed to repeated acts of terrorism in Israel. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2008; 21: 9–21. pmid:18302179
  16. 16. Ai AL, Cascio T, Santangelo LK, Evans-Campbell T. Hope, Meaning, and Growth Following the September 11, 2001, Terrorist Attacks. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 2005; 20: 523–548. pmid:15788553
  17. 17. Hall BJ, Hobfoll SE, Palmieri PA, Canetti-Nisim D, Shapira O, Galea S. The Psychological Impact of Impending Forced Settler Disengagement in Gaza: Trauma and Posttraumatic Growth. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2008; 21: 22–29. pmid:18302180
  18. 18. Shakespeare-Finch J, Lurie-Beck J. A meta-analytic clarification of the relationship between posttraumatic growth and symptoms of posttraumatic distress disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2014; 28, 2, 223–9. pmid:24291397
  19. 19. Dekel S, Ein-Dor T, Solomon Z. Posttraumatic growth and posttraumatic distress: A longitudinal study. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy. 2012; 4: 94–101.
  20. 20. Lowe SR, Manove EE, Rhodes JE. Posttraumatic stress and posttraumatic growth among low income mothers who survived Hurricane Katrina. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2013; 81: 877–889. pmid:23750467
  21. 21. King DW, Orazem RJ, Lauterbach D, King LA, Hebenstreit CL, Shalev AY. Factor structure of posttraumatic stress disorder as measured by the Impact of Event Scale—Revised: Stability across cultures and time. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy. 2009; 1:173–187.
  22. 22. Palmieri PA, Weathers FW, Difede J, King DW. Confirmatory factor analysis of the PTSD Checklist and the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale in disaster workers exposed to the World Trade Center Ground Zero. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007; 116: 329–341. pmid:17516765
  23. 23. Hinton DE, Lewis-Fernandez R. The cross-cultural validity of posttraumatic stress disorder: Implications for DSM-5. Depression and Anxiety. 2011; 28: 783–801. pmid:21910185
  24. 24. Splevins K, Cohen K, Bowley J, Joseph S. Theories of posttraumatic growth: Cross cultural perspectives. Journal of Loss and Trauma. 2010; 15: 259–277.
  25. 25. Helgeson VS, Reynolds KA, Tomich PL. A meta-analytic review of benefit finding and growth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2006; 74: 797–816. pmid:17032085
  26. 26. Hobfoll SE, Canetti-Nisim D, Johnson RJ. Exposure to terrorism, stress-related mental health symptoms, and defensive coping among Jews and Arabs in Israel. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2006; 74: 207–218. pmid:16649865
  27. 27. Hall BJ, Hobfoll SE, Canetti-Nisim D, Johnson RJ, Galea S. The defensive nature of benefit finding during ongoing terrorism: An examination of a national sample of Israeli Jews. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2009; 28: 993–1021. pmid:22058603
  28. 28. Galea S, Ahern J, Resnick H, Kilpatrick D, Bucuvalas M, Gold J, et al. Psychological sequelae of the September 11 terrorist attacks in New York City. New England Journal of Medicine. 2002; 346: 982–987. pmid:11919308
  29. 29. Galea S, Tracy M. Participation rates in epidemiologic studies. Annals of Epidemiology. 2007; 17: 643–653. pmid:17553702
  30. 30. Foa EB, Riggs DS, Dancu CV, Rothbaum BO. Reliability and validity of a brief instrument for assessing post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1993; 6: 459–473.
  31. 31. Hobfoll SE, Canetti D, Hall BJ, Brom D, Palmieri PA, Johnson RJ, et al. Are community studies of trauma's impact exaggerated? A Study among Jews and Palestinians. Psychological Assessment. 2011; 23: 599–605. pmid:21381832
  32. 32. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Press; 2000
  33. 33. Hobfoll SE, Lilly RS. Resource conservation as a strategy for community psychology. Journal of Community Psychology. 1993; 21: 128–148.
  34. 34. Tedeschi RG, Calhoun LG. The Posttraumatic Growth Inventory: measuring the positive legacy of trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 1996; 9: 455–471. pmid:8827649
  35. 35. Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin; 1990; 107: 238–246. pmid:2320703
  36. 36. King DW, Leskin GA, King LA, Weathers FW. Confirmatory factor analysis of the clinician-administered PTSD Scale: Evidence for the dimensionality of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Assessment. 1998; 10: 90–96.
  37. 37. Hobfoll SE, Hall BJ, Canetti-Nisim D, Galea S, Johnson RJ, Palmieri PA. Refining our understanding of traumatic growth in the face of terrorism: Moving from meaning cognitions to doing what is meaningful. Applied Psychology: An International Review. 2007; 56: 345–366.
  38. 38. Zoellner T, Maercker A. Posttraumatic growth in clinical psychology—A critical review and introduction of a two component model. Clinical Psychology Review. 2006; 26: 626–653. pmid:16515831
  39. 39. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York, New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.
  40. 40. Boals A, Steward JM, Schuettler D. Advancing our understanding of posttraumatic growth by considering event centrality. Journal of Loss and Trauma. 2010; 15: 518–533.
  41. 41. Lancaster SL, Kloep M, Rodriguez BF, Weston R. Event centrality, posttraumatic cognitions, and the experience of posttraumatic growth. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment, & Trauma. 2013; 22: 379–393.
  42. 42. Schuettler D, Boals A. The path to posttraumatic growth versus posttraumatic stress disorder: Contributions of event centrality and coping. Journal of Loss and Trauma. 2011; 16: 180–194.