Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Adolescents’ Use of Care for Behavioral and Emotional Problems: Types, Trends, and Determinants

  • Sijmen A. Reijneveld ,

    s.a.reijneveld@umcg.nl

    Affiliation University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Department of Health Sciences, Groningen, the Netherlands

  • P. Auke Wiegersma,

    Affiliation University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Department of Health Sciences, Groningen, the Netherlands

  • Johan Ormel,

    Affiliation University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Interdisciplinary Center Psychopathology and Emotion Regulation, Groningen, the Netherlands

  • Frank C. Verhulst,

    Affiliation Erasmus Medical Center, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Rotterdam, the Netherlands

  • Wilma A. M. Vollebergh,

    Affiliation Department of Social Sciences, University of Utrecht, Utrecht, the Netherlands

  • Danielle E. M. C. Jansen

    Affiliation University Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, Department of Health Sciences, Groningen, the Netherlands

Abstract

Objective

While adolescents use various types of care for behavioral and emotional problems, evidence on age trends and determinants per type is scarce. We aimed to assess use of care by adolescents because of behavioral and emotional problems, overall and by type, and its determinants, for ages 10–19 years.

Methods

We obtained longitudinal data on 2,230 adolescents during ages 10–19 from four measurements regarding use of general care and specialized care (youth social care and mental healthcare) in the preceding 6 months, the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) and Youth Self-Report, and child and family characteristics. We analyzed data by multilevel logistic regression.

Results

Overall rates of use increased from 20.1% at age 10/11 to 32.2% at age 19: general care was used most. At age 10/11 use was higher among boys, at age 19 among girls. Use of general care increased for both genders, whereas use of specialized care increased among girls but decreased among boys. This differential change was associated with CBCL externalizing and internalizing problems, school problems, family socioeconomic status, and parental divorce. Preceding CBCL problems predicted more use: most for mental health care and least for general care. Moreover, general care was used more frequently by low and medium socioeconomic status families, with odds ratios (95%-confidence intervals): 1.52 (1.23;1.88) and 1.40 (1.17;1.67); youth social care in case of parental divorce, 2.07 (1.36;3.17); and of special education, 2.66 (1.78;3.95); and mental healthcare in case of special education, 2.66 (1.60;4.51).

Discussion

Adolescents with behavioral and emotional problems use general care most frequently. Overall use increases with age. Determinants of use vary per type.

Introduction

Estimates of rates of behavioral and emotional problems among adolescents vary from 10 to 25%. These problems may lead to restrictions in their daily functioning and to severe long-term effects. [1][4] Ideally, all adolescents with such impairing problems should receive appropriate, evidence-based interventions. In reality, only a minority of them will receive care, [1], [5][8] that is, either general or specialist care. Specialist care involves either youth mental healthcare, or youth social care. [5], [6] In practice, various factors hamper proper use of adequate care.

Use of care has been shown to be more likely in case of more severe problems and associated impairments, [5][7], [9], [10] and to depend further on factors such as child age and gender, [5], [6] parental [5][7] and teacher perceptions of problems, [6], [11] and environmental factors, [5], [7], [11] like not living with two biological parents and educational difficulties of the child. [9] However, evidence concerning the influence of these determinants is inconclusive. This may be due to differences in the types of care that were studied, in the factors that were assessed, and in the ages of the adolescents involved. [5], [6] For example, Ford and co-authors found that in the age range 5–15 years socioeconomically disadvantaged children were more likely to receive youth social care, while boys were more likely to receive mental healthcare. [6] Use of any care for behavioral and emotional problems has also been shown to vary by age. [5], [6] This may be due to an age-specific effect of determinants. For example, a study of Ford and co-authors also shows that determinants of use of primary care varies by age, with only child behavioral problems playing a role at ages 5–10, whereas more factors contributed at ages 11–15. [6].

A theoretical model may help to structure the role of various factors in the seeking and obtaining of care for behavioral and emotional problems. An useful and widely used model is Andersen’s socio-behavioral model of care utilization. [7], [11], [12] This model assumes that use of care is subject to factors that can be either predisposing, enabling, or expressing need. Predisposing factors refer to everything that might prompt seeking and using care, e.g. educational level. Enabling factors involve the means by which services might be accessed, e.g. the competences to achieve aims. Need factors in this study include health problems and the degree to which they require care, e.g. the severity of the health problem concerned. This model may be applied to specific services or to the entire care system. [7] Need factors should probably be included in any prediction of use of care for behavioral and emotional problems, given the wealth of data on the association between perceived behavioral and emotional problems and use of care. [5][7], [9] Determinants of use are likely to vary by type of care, as use of care is the result of various care seeking processes. [13] This has first to do with the severity of the problems. Typically, less severe problems can be expected to be handled in primary care, according to Goldberg and Huxley’s model of filters of care which presumes a number of filters to be passed before a person reaches more complex and intensive types of care. [5], [13] Second, mental health care traditionally aims at children with mental problems, i.e. regarding behavior or emotion. In contrast, youth social care traditionally aims at supporting the social and economic context of youth. [5], [7], [11] Because of these different aims, behavioral and emotional problems can be expected to be associated most strongly with use of mental health care, whereas factors like parental divorce and poverty can be expected to be associated with youth social care. However, circumstances leading to use of care may be interconnected as well. For instance, social circumstances like parental divorce and unemployment, [9] or living in a deprived area, [14] may lead to behavioral and emotional problems. Evidence on the factors associated with use of various types of care is very scarce.

The aim of this study is first to assess by gender and age the use of care by adolescents in the age range 10–19 years because of behavioral and emotional problems. A second aim is to assess the factors determining this use overall and by type of care, along with the degree to which these factors explain changes in use by age. Andersen’s socio-behavioral model of health care utilization will be used as the theoretical basis for this study.

Methods

Sample and Procedure

The study was performed as part of the TRacking Adolescents’ Individual Lives Survey (TRAILS) study, a large prospective population study of Dutch adolescents, designed to examine and explain the progress of mental health and social development from pre-adolescence into adulthood. [15]Enrolment for TRAILS started in 2001. Children were randomly selected from the Population Registers of five municipalities in the north of the Netherlands and were included if they were aged 10–11 and attended a school that was willing and able to participate: N = 2,935 children. Of these, 2,230 provided informed consent to participate from both parent and child (76.0%; T1). Non-respondents more frequently were boy, or single child, or had a low-educated parent, but they did not differ regarding psychopathology. [4], [7], [10] The present study involves data from the first four measurement waves of TRAILS (T1–T4), running from 2001 until 2010. Mean age at T1 was 11.1 years; standard deviation 0.55. At T2, T3, and T4 mean ages were 13.6, 16.2, and 19.1 years, respectively.

During the first and third measurement wave, well-trained interviewers visited one of the parents or their guardian at their homes to administer an interview covering the child’s developmental history and somatic health, parental psychopathology, and care utilization. The parent or guardian was also asked to fill out a questionnaire at each wave, as was the adolescent. [16] The design of each wave of the study was separately approved by the Dutch National Medical Ethics Committee (www.ccmo.nl), including the written informed consent by both child and parents.

Measurements

The data concerned service use, and predisposing, enabling, and need factors, based on Andersen’s model; for each factor, the wave at which it was measured is indicated as “(T1),” etc.

Service use was measured as parent-reported use of any type of professional care because of behavior or emotional issues of the child in the past six months (T1–4). In the Dutch care system, children and adolescents with behavioral and emotional problems can contact either preventive child healthcare, their family physician or the office for youth care. If more specialized care is needed, these can refer either to youth mental health care or to youth social care. Youth mental health care comprises care provided by child psychologists, and child and adolescent psychiatrists. Youth social care comprises care provided by youth and social workers and includes child protection. All types of care requiring referral have been included under the heading “specialized care”. Based on this structure of the care system, care was categorized as general care (preventive child healthcare, family physician, home care, etc.), youth social care (youth social work, youth protection, etc.), and youth mental healthcare (child psychologist, psychiatrist, etc.). The latter two were also denoted as “specialized care.” [4].

Predisposing factors refer to everything that might predispose an adolescent to seek and use a specific service. In this study, the predisposing factors include age (T1–4), gender (T1), degree of urbanization (T1), family structure (T1–2), ethnicity (T1), educational level of adolescent (T1–2) and parents (T1), and socioeconomic status of the parent(s) (T1).

Urbanization was assessed by the number of residential addresses per 3.14 square kilometers (i.e., by drawing a circle with a radius of one kilometer from each point). [7], [8] Following the guidelines of Statistics Netherlands, it was dichotomized as less than 1000 for rural and 1000 and more for urban (http://www.rivm.nl/vtv/object_map/o2617n21780.html).

Family structure was measured as not having continuously lived with two parents from birth, denoted as “one biological parent.” If yes, this was due to divorce in 90% of cases (remainder almost always single mothers by choice). Of those with one biological parent, one-third had a stepparent at T1.

Educational level of the adolescent was measured by parent report in two ways. The first one concerned progress in primary education at T1, measured as: 1) regular primary education, 2) special primary education, 3) having repeated one or two grades, and 4) having skipped a grade. The second one concerned school level at T2, being higher secondary school, lower secondary or vocational school, or primary school or special education. For standard pupils, the transition from primary to secondary school occurs around age 12 in the Dutch system.

Parental education consisted of five levels: 1) elementary education, 2) lower tracks of secondary education, 3) higher tracks of secondary education, 4) senior vocational education, and 5) university education.

Parental occupational level was measured based on the International Standard Classification for Occupations, [15] as low, medium, or high.

Finally, family socioeconomic status was computed as the aggregate of parental occupational level, parental educational level, and family income: Cronbach alpha: 0.84. This was categorized as low (lowest 25%), medium (next 50%) and high (highest 25%). [17].

Enabling factors are related to the means by which adolescents might access mental healthcare. The enabling factors considered in this study were perceived social support of children from parents and peers, and perceived self-competence (all T1). “Perceived social support” was measured as perceived affection from, and behavioral confirmation toward both parents and classmates, respectively. [18] Perceived self-competence was measured by the Self-Perception Profile for Children (SPPC) questionnaire, which measures self-reported competencies in youths. Previous research has shown it to be highly valid and reliable in the Dutch setting; [19] Cronbach’s alphas in this study varied from 0.58 (SPPC Sports) to 0.81 (SPPC Appearance).

Need factors concerned how adolescents viewed their own mental health and how their parents perceived need. These views were assessed using the Youth Self-Report (YSR) and the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) with a time-frame of the past 6 months, for waves 1–3. The YSR and CBCL are highly reliable and valid measurements of behavioral and emotional problems over the preceding six months. [20] They are filled out by parents and adolescents or pre-adolescents, respectively, but in other respects contain similar items. We used age-standardized scores on two broad-band dimensions: internalizing (anxious/depressed, withdrawn/depressed, and somatic complaints) and externalizing problems (aggressive behavior and rule-breaking behavior). Cronbach’s alphas for the first wave of this study were: YSR internalizing –0.87; YSR externalizing –0.85; CBCL internalizing –0.85; CBCL externalizing - 0.90.

Analysis

We first imputed missing data on the CBCL and YSR based on the multivariate normal model, [21], [22] as implemented in the NORM software. This procedure minimizes the loss of statistical power, provides correctly estimated standard errors, and preserves the characteristics of the data set as a whole. [23] Adolescents with missing values in categorical independent variables were retained in the analyses by creating separate dummies per variable. Next, we computed descriptives of the background of the adolescents concerned, overall and by gender. Third, we tested whether rates of the use of various services differed by age, using chi-square tests. Fourth, we assessed which factors determined use of various services and whether these differed by age, using logistic regression analyses. We first did this for blocks of determinants, predisposing and enabling factors (i.e., sociodemographic ones) and need factors (levels of behavioral and emotional problems), and then for all determinants combined. Regarding need factors, we examined the associations with current needs, that is, current CBCL and YSR scores (for use in waves 1–3), and with previous needs, that is, CBCL and YSR scores during the preceding assessment (for use in waves 2–4). Finally, we examined whether the effect of level of problems on trends in use of services differed for internalizing as compared to externalizing problems and whether these effects varied by gender, again using logistic regression.

We performed the first until third step using IBM SPSS 20.0 (/www01.ibm.com/software/analytics/spss/). The fourth and fifth steps were done with multilevel techniques in MLwiN 2.22 (www.cmm.bristol.ac.uk/MLwiN), with the highest level being the adolescent with his/her personal and family characteristics, and the lower level being each measurement wave that was available for that adolescent. In that way, we could account for the fact that various measurements for the same adolescent were correlated. [3], [7], [24] Moreover, if an outcome was not available for one wave, the adolescent still contributed to the modeling for the other waves.

In the multilevel models, the probability of the response of the measurement at the i-th wave in the j-th individual was modelled as follows, following various previous report, e.g. [15], [25], [15], [26]:where α represents the constant term;

β1 … βp represent the regression coefficients of the wave-specific explanatory variables x1 … xp;

γ1 and γ2 represent the regression coefficients of the individual explanatory variables z1 and z2;

eij represents the wave-level residuals, and e’j represents the individual-level residuals. These residuals are also denoted as random variables, with a zero expectation, and σij2 and σi2 as respective variances. Random variation at wave level was assumed to be approximately binomially distributed, based on the dichotomized outcomes. [27] We did not assess covariance terms between wave- and individual-level. Models were fitted using the most accurate procedure available, i.e. a predictive quasi-likelihood procedure in combination with a second order Taylor expansion series. [28]

Results

Table 1 provides descriptives of the background of the adolescents concerned. At baseline (ages 10/11), they had a predominantly Dutch-born ethnic background (89%), most lived in a two-parent family (76%), and in a rural or semi-rural area (72%), and most had a regular progress at primary school (75%).

thumbnail
Table 1. Background characteristics of the adolescents who reported on use of care at T1, by gender*.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093526.t001

Table 2 shows that at all ages, use of general care was higher than of specialized care. Overall rates of use increased from 20.1% at age 10/11 to 32.2%. at age 19. At age 10/11 use was higher among boys than among girls, but towards age 19, it had more than doubled among girls while being rather stable among boys. This gender-differential increase implies that rates at age 10/11 years are higher for boys, but at age 19 much higher for girls. Looking at types of care, the increases for girls occurred in both general care and specialized care, whereas for boys an increase only occurred for use of general care. Use of specialized (youth social and mental health) care in boys decreased. Moreover, quite a few adolescents used several types of care within the assessment period, all prevalences of use per age-category adding to over 100%.

thumbnail
Table 2. Use of various types of care because of behavioral and emotional problems in the past six months, by gender and measurement wave (mean age per wave in years): numbers and percentages of users, p-values for gender differences, and p-values for trend across waves.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093526.t002

Multilevel logistic regression showed that age trends in overall use indeed differ by gender, as shown by statistically significant interactions of gender with wave (Table 3). Adjusted for gender and wave, several predisposing enabling and need factors were associated with overall higher use of any care. Mutual adjustment between factors decreased the strength of most of these associations, as shown by generally lower odds ratios, OR (Table 3, most right columns). In these models, we separately adjusted for CBCL and YSR scores in either the concurrent or the preceding wave. OR for the former were mostly somewhat higher than for the latter, though they were always in the same direction; the latter are not shown. Moreover, associations with higher use were stronger for the parent-reported CBCL scales than for the adolescent-reported YSR scales. Associations of need factors with higher use of care decreased relatively strongly after mutual adjustment, indicating their relatively high correlations.

thumbnail
Table 3. Determinants of use of care, adjusted for gender and wave, and additionally for all shown factors: odds ratios (OR), and 95% confidence intervals (CI) from multilevel logistic regression.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093526.t003

The adjustment for predisposing, enabling, and need factors had some influence on the associations of use of any care with gender, measurement wave, and their interaction. In particular, the OR for waves increased somewhat, whereas the OR for the interaction of gender and wave decreased. Thus, after this adjustment, increases by wave in use of any service by age were more similar for boys and girls. The effect of need did not vary by gender.

In its top rows Table 4 shows the results as presented in Table 3, but now also per type of care. Interactions of gender and wave were found for all types of care, except for youth social care at wave 4. Model 2 in Table 4 shows that adjustment for all factors increased ORs somewhat for wave but decreased them for the gender-wave interaction. This again indicates that these factors do not account for the overall increase in use at increasing ages, but that they account for some of the differential increase in use of care between girls and boys with increasing age. The latter effect was particularly large for use of youth social care.

thumbnail
Table 4. Determinants of use of care in three models including: 1) only gender and wave; 2) all factors; and 3) factors derived from stepwise forward selection.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0093526.t004

The bottom part of Table 4 shows the results of a stepwise forward selection of determining factors according to the Andersen model, again for any care and the three types of care, with need for care always included. Only a limited number of factors were associated with higher use, both for any care and for the three types of care, separately. These particularly concerned the parent-reported need factors (i.e., CBCL scores) and poor progress during primary education (all types except for general care), lower family SES (general care), and recent parental divorce (youth social care). This small set of factors accounts for most of the differences in odds ratios between the gender/wave and the fully adjusted models. The effect of need factors did not vary by gender except for that of adolescent-reported internalizing problems; that effect was slightly larger for boys (OR 1.03; 95% confidence interval 1.00 to 1.07).

Discussion

We found that in the age range 10–19 years, the use of care services because of behavioral and emotional problems was rather high but mostly involved general care. At age 10/11 use was much higher for boys, but during adolescence use more than doubled for girls, whereas it was rather stable for boys. This led to a much higher use by girls at age 19, with the differential increase being due to specialized care, in particular, youth mental healthcare. Part of this differential change can be accounted for by a limited set of factors: in particular, needs as measured by parent-reported behavioral and emotional problems of the adolescent. In addition, some predisposing factors contribute to this differential change such as family SES for general care, parental divorce, and special education for youth social care, and special education for youth mental healthcare. These factors were all associated with higher use.

Our finding of increasing use by age confirms the available evidence but decidedly shows that the increase strongly differs by gender and by type of care. This especially holds for use of specialized care, which even decreased slightly among boys as they grew older. This differentiation may also explain the heterogeneity in findings up until now, where studies either focused on young [28] or older adolescents, and/or did not adjust for age, gender, and their interaction when assessing the role of other predictors. [6], [10] Re-analyses of these data with inclusion of an age/gender interaction might be of interest.

Needs, measured by CBCL and YSR, were the strongest predictor of higher use of care, with associations being stronger for concurrent than for past needs, and stronger for parent-reported than for adolescent-reported needs. The associations were considerable: adjusted for age/gender, a one standard deviation change in CBCL score was associated with 1.55 higher odds for use of any care. The dominance of parent-report may be interpreted by the rather strong impact parents still have on the help-seeking of their child at these ages and confirms previous findings regarding this. [6] The only exception concerns adolescent-reported internalizing problems which, after mutual adjustment, were still associated with use of youth mental healthcare. An explanation may be that in particular emotional problems affect the adolescent’s well-being and thus stimulate them to seek care. Mental healthcare is then probably more likely to be administered than youth care, since the latter targets the adolescent’s social environment to a relatively greater degree.

We found rather similar associations between parent-reported need factors and use of youth mental health care and of youth social care. One might expect somewhat stronger associations for youth mental health care which specifically targets at mental problems. The fact that this is not the case may be explained in several ways. First, social and economic problems such as parent divorce and unemployment have been shown to lead to adolescent mental health problems. [5][7], [9] This would imply that the association of parent-reported behavioral and emotional problems with youth social care is in fact due to the role of underlying social and economic problems. Some support for this explanation is provided by our finding that - in addition to behavioral and emotional problems - parental divorce was also independently associated with use of youth social care but not with that of youth mental health care. A second explanation may be that youth social care and youth mental health care simply serve partially overlapping groups of adolescents. A third explanation might be that need factors are always more important than predisposing and enabling factors regarding use of care in case of behavioral and emotional problems, independent of the type of care. Further research is needed to disentangle these explanations.

Interestingly, the effect of needs hardly varied by gender, the only exception being that the effect of adolescent-reported needs on use of youth social care was relatively larger for boys. Thus the increase of the severity of problems in girls may explain their increasing use of care as they grow older, but the effect of the problems that they themselves and their parents perceive is, per unit increase, no stronger than it is for boys. Regarding youth social care, this effect is even a bit weaker, which may also be interpreted as that the increase in emotional problems for girls, is not fully translated into use of care. This topic certainly deserves further study.

Needs were found to be associated with higher use of all types of care, but the further set of factors predicting higher use was small, concerned only predisposing factors, and varied somewhat by type of care. Lower family SES predicted higher use of general care but not of youth social care. In particular the latter contrasts with findings of previous studies that showed use of youth social care to be more likely in the case of socioeconomic disadvantage of the families involved. [14] One explanation might be that disadvantage is mediated by needs and by poor progress at school. Further predictors for use differ relatively little between youth social care and youth mental healthcare. The only real exception was parental divorce, which may e.g. lead to a need for temporary shelter of youth that is mostly provided by youth social care. This may be interpreted as another indication of a partial overlap of these two types of care in targeting the same groups of adolescents as far as it concerns care provided for behavioral and emotional problems. Differences may, however, be larger for other reasons of encounter, such as child protection in cases of youth social care.

It should be noted that a rather small set of factors explained a substantial part of the age/gender differences in use of care, with the strongest factor being the needs of adolescents as reported by their parents and further factors all concerning predisposing ones. Enabling factors, relating to the means that adolescents have to access care, did not contribute. Explanations for this might be that in most cases the parents decide what help will be sought for their child, or that access to care is good anyhow and thus does not require additional competencies. Moreover, it should be noted that we did not assess appropriateness of care, but only actual use of care. Determinants as found might thus theoretically have led to improper use.

Finally, the increase in use at older ages in girls was hardly explained by any factor. In contrast, in particular for boys differences became somewhat bigger after adjustment for both all factors and for only the needs and predisposing factors. The question remains how to explain this age-trend. Is it due to older adolescents and their parents being more effective in their seeking of care, and in passing the various filters of the care system as outlined by Goldberg and Huxley? [5], [7], [11] Or is it a period-effect instead of an age-effect, as in the Netherlands overall rates of use increased somewhat during the decade covered by this cohort? [13] Evidently, this issue requires further study.

Strengths and Limitations

Our study has substantial strengths, in particular, its large sample, high response rate, longitudinal nature, and assessment of a wide range of types of care. Moreover, by using multilevel techniques in longitudinal data, we were able to control for individual factors that might otherwise yield spurious variations by age. However, some limitations should be noted as well. We could not assess all determinants at all ages, which might yield some overestimation of the relative effect of needs. In addition, differences by age as measured may also reflect changes in the care system over time. During the study period, 2001–2010, the design of the Dutch care system was relatively stable but use increased. [29] Second, we only assessed use, not its intensity. If such a differentiation were to be used, associations might have been stronger, but this would certainly require additional study. Moreover, use of care was measured based on reports, albeit parent-report and not self-report. Use of parent-report may have reinforced its validity, but may also have led to some underestimation of the prevalence rates at the fourth wave, age 19. At that age, not all parents may be fully aware of the use of care by their child. Because of the same reason, we did not assess parent-reported child behavioral and emotional problems at that age, causing the analyses on the importance of current need to cover only ages 10–16. A final limitation is that needs were measured as perceived needs, i.e. report on the CBCL and YSR, which may deviate from clinically assessed needs.

Implications

General care was used much more frequently than specialized mental healthcare for adolescent behavioral and emotional problems. Maintaining and reinforcing expertise regarding this topic in primary and general medical care should therefore be prioritized. This type of care is much more accessible for troubled youth and might also be more cost-effective than specialized services.

A reinforcement of this expertise might also provide some counterbalance against the combined use of several types of care in a short period and the rather strong increase of use of specialized care at increasing ages by female adolescents. An explanation for that increase may be that general care does not adequately meet the specific needs of this group, or that it concerns improper use. This merits further study. Given the rather strong expansion of care for young people’s emotional and behavioral problems, ways of coping with these problems and adequately targeting those in greatest need deserve our utmost attention.

Acknowledgments

Data Sharing: TRAILS data of the T1, T2 and T3 measurement waves are deposited in the Data Archiving and Networked Services of the Royal Dutch Academy of Sciences (DANS-KNAW) and can be accessed at www.dans.knaw.nl. The T4 measurement wave will become available in this archive in the first half of 2014.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: SAR. Performed the experiments: JO FCV. Analyzed the data: SAR DEMCJ. Wrote the paper: SAR. Commenting on study protcol: PAW JO FCV WAMV DEMCJ. Commenting on results of analyses: PAW JO FCV WAMV DEMCJ. Discussing and editing of manuscript and approving final version: PAW JO FCV WAMV DEMCJ.

References

  1. 1. Kieling C, Baker-Henningham H, Belfer M, Conti G, Ertem I, et al. (2011) Child and adolescent mental health worldwide: Evidence for action. Lancet 378: 1515–1525.
  2. 2. Smith JP, Smith GC (2010) Long-term economic costs of psychological problems during childhood. Soc Sci Med 71: 110–115.
  3. 3. Jaspers M, de Winter AF, Huisman M, Verhulst FC, Ormel J, et al. (2012) Trajectories of psychosocial problems in adolescents predicted by findings from early well-child assessments. J Adolesc Health 51: 475–483.
  4. 4. Ormel J, Oldehinkel AJ, Sijtsema J, van Oort F, Raven D, et al. (2012) The TRacking adolescents’ individual lives survey (TRAILS): Design, current status, and selected findings. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 51: 1020–1036.
  5. 5. Sayal K (2006) Annotation: Pathways to care for children with mental health problems. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 47: 649–659.
  6. 6. Ford T, Hamilton H, Meltzer H, Goodman R (2008) Predictors of service use for mental health problems among British schoolchildren. Child and Adolescent Mental Health 13: 32–40.
  7. 7. Jansen DE, Wiegersma P, Ormel J, Verhulst FC, Vollebergh WA, et al. (2013) Need for mental health care in adolescents and its determinants: The TRAILS study. Eur J Public Health 23: 236–241.
  8. 8. Jorg F, Ormel J, Reijneveld SA, Jansen DE, Verhulst FC, et al. (2012) Puzzling findings in studying the outcome of “real world” adolescent mental health services: The TRAILS study. PLoS One 7: e44704.
  9. 9. Tick NT, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC (2008) Ten-year increase in service use in the Dutch population. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 17: 373–380.
  10. 10. Amone-P’olak K, Ormel J, Oldehinkel AJ, Reijneveld SA, Verhulst FC, et al. (2010) Socioeconomic position predicts specialty mental health service use independent of clinical severity: The TRAILS study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 49: 647–655.
  11. 11. Zwaanswijk M, van der Ende J, Verhaak PF, Bensing JM, Verhulst FC (2005) Help-seeking for child psychopathology: Pathways to informal and professional services in the Netherlands. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 44: 1292–1300.
  12. 12. Andersen RM (1995) Revisiting the behavioral model and access to medical care: Does it matter? J Health Soc Behav 36: 1–10.
  13. 13. Goldberg DP, Huxley P (1992) Common mental disorders: A bio-social model. London: Routledge.
  14. 14. Harland P, Reijneveld SA, Brugman E, Verloove-Vanhorick SP, Verhulst FC (2002) Family factors and life events as risk factors for behavioural and emotional problems in children. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 11: 176–184.
  15. 15. Reijneveld SA, Veenstra R, de Winter AF, Verhulst FC, Ormel J, et al. (2010) Area deprivation affects behavioral problems of young adolescents in mixed urban and rural areas: The TRAILS study. J Adolesc Health 46: 189–196.
  16. 16. de Winter AF, Oldehinkel AJ, Veenstra R, Brunnekreef JA, Verhulst FC, et al. (2005) Evaluation of non-response bias in mental health determinants and outcomes in a large sample of pre-adolescents. Eur J Epidemiol 20: 173–181.
  17. 17. Ganzeboom HBG, Treiman DJ (2003) Three internationally standardised measures for comparative research on occupational status. In: Hoffmeyer-Zlotnick JHP, Wolf C, editors. Advances in cross-national comparison. A European working book for demographic and socio-economic variables. New York: Kluwer. 159–193.
  18. 18. Veenstra R, Lindenberg S, Oldehinkel AJ, De Winter AF, Ormel J (2006) Temperament, environment, and antisocial behavior in a population sample of preadolescent boys and girls. International Journal of Behavioral Development 30: 422–432.
  19. 19. Ormel J, Lindenberg S, Steverink N, Vonkorff M (1997) Quality of life and social production functions: A framework for understanding health effects. Soc Sci Med 45: 1051–1063.
  20. 20. Muris P, Meesters C, Fijen P (2003) The self-perception profile for children: Further evidence for its factor structure, reliability, and validity. Personality and Individual Differences 35: 1791–1802.
  21. 21. Verhulst FC, van der Ende J (2013) Handleiding ASEBA. vragenlijsten voor leeftijden 6 tot en met 18 jaar [Manual ASEBA. questionnaires for ages 6 till 18 years]. Rotterdam: ASEBA Netherlands.
  22. 22. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA (2001) Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington (VT): University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & families.
  23. 23. Rubin DB (1987) Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. New York: Wiley.
  24. 24. Graham JW (2009) Missing data analysis: Making it work in the real world. Annu Rev Psychol 60: 577–605.
  25. 25. Snijders TAB, Bosker RJ (2011) Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. London: Sage.
  26. 26. Reijneveld SA (1998) The impact of individual and area characteristics on urban socioeconomic differences in health and smoking. Int J Epidemiol 27: 33–40.
  27. 27. Reijneveld SA, Brugman E, Verhulst FC, Verloove-Vanhorick SP (2004) Identification and management of psychosocial problems among toddlers in Dutch preventive child health care. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 158: 811–817.
  28. 28. Goldstein H (1995) Multilevel statistical models. London: Edward Arnold.
  29. 29. Sadiraj K, Ras M, Putman L, Jonker JJ (2013) Does youth care continue to grow? the demand on services. Realisation 201–2011, and prognosis 2011–2017 [in Dutch]. The Hague: Social and Cultural Planning Office.
  30. 30. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA (2007) Multicultural supplement to the manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington (VT): University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & families.