Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Does Invasion Success Reflect Superior Cognitive Ability? A Case Study of Two Congeneric Lizard Species (Lampropholis, Scincidae)

  • Chalene N. Bezzina ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Chalene N. Bezzina, Joshua J. Amiel

    cbez6563@uni.sydney.edu.au

    Affiliation School of Biological Sciences A08, University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

  • Joshua J. Amiel ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Chalene N. Bezzina, Joshua J. Amiel

    Affiliation School of Biological Sciences A08, University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

  • Richard Shine

    Affiliation School of Biological Sciences A08, University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

Abstract

A species' intelligence may reliably predict its invasive potential. If this is true, then we might expect invasive species to be better at learning novel tasks than non-invasive congeners. To test this hypothesis, we exposed two sympatric species of Australian scincid lizards, Lampropholis delicata (invasive) and L. guichenoti (non-invasive) to standardized maze-learning tasks. Both species rapidly decreased the time they needed to find a food reward, but latencies were always higher for L. delicata than L. guichenoti. More detailed analysis showed that neither species actually learned the position of the food reward; they were as likely to turn the wrong way at the end of the study as at the beginning. Instead, their times decreased because they spent less time immobile in later trials; and L. guichenoti arrived at the reward sooner because they exhibited “freezing” (immobility) less than L. delicata. Hence, our data confirm that the species differ in their performance in this standardized test, but neither the decreasing time to find the reward, nor the interspecific disparity in those times, are reflective of cognitive abilities. Behavioural differences may well explain why one species is invasive and one is not, but those differences do not necessarily involve cognitive ability.

Introduction

Species invasions are one of the largest threats to native species worldwide, but our ability to predict invasion success remains weak. To become a successful invader, a species must pass through several discrete stages of the introduction process [1]. Rather than being just a random subset of taxa, invasive species are thought to have behavioural traits that improve their chances of advancing through each of these stages [2]. Some behavioural traits may enhance a species' invasiveness across all introductory stages, whereas other traits may facilitate one stage of the invasion process but impair success at another stage. For example, “bolder” individuals may be more likely to enter a transport vector and be shipped to a new location (transport stage) but might also have a greater risk of being detected at biosecurity checkpoints (introduction stage) [2], [3]. Recently, researchers have used a variety of species-level behavioural traits to predict species' invasiveness [2].

One trait that may reliably predict species invasiveness is intelligence. We generally consider an animal to be intelligent if it is able to 1) rapidly solve novel challenges that are ecologically relevant to that species, 2) solve a single relevant challenge using multiple strategies, and 3) solve several different types of relevant challenges [4]. Once an animal arrives in a new location (i.e. after transport and introduction), it must still overcome a variety of challenges. In order to successfully establish a new population, the new arrivals must identify and avoid novel predators, locate potential mates, obtain resources, and react appropriately to unfamiliar climatic regimes. Organisms that quickly modify their behaviours to meet these challenges are more likely to survive and reproduce in their new environment and therefore, we might expect intelligence to correlate positively with invasiveness [5], [6]. Indeed, across all four classes of terrestrial vertebrates, studies that use relative brain size as a proxy for intelligence have reported that large-brained species are more successful invaders than are small-brained species [5], [7], [8]. Nonetheless, brain size is only a rough guide to intelligence, and thus these studies do not provide any direct evidence that successful invaders are better at solving novel challenges than are unsuccessful invaders. To test the hypothesis that intelligence predicts the success of species introductions, we need to specifically measure and compare the learning ability of species that have established invasive populations, compared to related but non-invasive species.

The congeneric scincid lizards Lampropholis delicata and L. guichenoti provide an ideal model system with which to test this hypothesis. These species are both small (∼35–55 mm adult snout-vent length [SVL]), oviparous (average clutch size ∼3 eggs), ground-dwelling, generalist insectivores that are broadly sympatric in suburban habitats throughout southeastern Australia [3], [9], [10]. Yet, despite these similarities, only L. delicata has successfully established populations outside of its native range (e.g. Lord Howe Island, The Hawaiian Islands and New Zealand), whereas L. guichenoti has not [3 and references therein]. However, these species co-occur in each of the areas identified as source regions for L. delicata introductions. Likewise, both species have been intercepted during biosecurity checks of goods entering New Zealand [3], [11], [12], suggesting that both taxa have had introduction opportunities but that only L. delicata has capitalised on these opportunities to become invasive.

Could differences in cognitive ability explain the apparent disparity in the ability of the two species to become established after being introduced to a new location? Certainly, behavioural traits differ between these species. For example, Chapple et al. [3] found that L. delicata was more exploratory than L. guichenoti, plausibly allowing L. delicata to locate critical resources and mates in novel habitats (and thus, increasing L. delicata's likelihood of establishing invasive populations). However, more exploratory individuals would also encounter dangers (e.g. predators and environmental hazards) more frequently than less exploratory individuals, increasing their chances of injury and death [13]. Thus, exploratory behaviour alone seems unlikely to explain why L. delicata has been more successful than L. guichenoti at establishing populations in new locations.

Superior cognitive abilities (e.g. learning and memory) might have helped L. delicata to meet the challenges associated with translocation to a new habitat [5]. For example, L. delicata may remember the location of profitable resource patches and sensory cues associated with predators more rapidly than L. guichenoti, allowing L. delicata to maximize its chances of obtaining critical resources while reducing encounter rates with predators. Here, we test this hypothesis using a simple Y-maze with a food reward to explore whether or not L. delicata is able to solve a novel cognitive challenge more rapidly than does L. guichenoti. Because intelligence is thought to be advantageous during species introductions, we predicted a priori that the invasive L. delicata would significantly outperform its non-invasive congener L. guichenoti in the maze task. Such differences in cognitive ability may explain disparities in the capacity of these two species to establish populations in novel environments.

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

The University of Sydney Animal Ethics Committee approved all of the procedures described in this manuscript (approval #: L04/8-2010/3/5449). All animals were released upon completion of the study.

Collection and Housing

We collected 16 adult L. guichenoti (8 adult females and 8 adult males) and 16 adult L. delicata (8 adult females and 8 adult males) in suburban Sydney, New South Wales, Australia. Lizards were housed in individual plastic containers (200 mm×140 mm×70 mm) lined with paper towel. Each lizard was provided with a shelter (100 mm long×230 mm diameter) and ad libitum access to water. We withheld food from all lizards 48 hr prior to the first maze trial in order to standardize hunger levels.

Maze Task

As our novel cognitive challenge, we used a simple Y-maze with a food reward to assess learning rates in L. delicata and L. guichenoti. The use of simple T- and Y-mazes to test learning ability is a standard technique in studies of reptilian cognition [14]. Mazes were constructed from opaque U-channelled electrical conduit fitted with clear plastic tops (Tripac Distribution PTY LTD, Sydney, Australia). Two arms of each maze contained a wooden platform with a single plastic feeding well. The remaining arm in each maze was empty and designated as the starting location for all trials. There was also a central decision point used to determine turning errors. As lizards use visual cues during foraging [15], the two reward-containing arms of each maze were painted with different colours (blue and orange) and patterns (striped and solid) to provide local cues. Each colour-pattern combination was replicated and reversed to account for colour bias and side preference (figure 1). Also, the mazes remained in the same location within the room used for experimental testing, providing the lizards with the opportunity to navigate the mazes using positional cues external to the maze environment.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Y-mazes used to assess learning ability in L. delicata and L. guichenoti.

Each maze had three arms of equal length. Two maze arms were painted with contrasting colours (orange and blue) and patterns (stripes and solids) to provide visual cues. All colour-pattern combinations were replicated and reversed in our study (four mazes total). Two arms contained feeding wells (A and B) whereas the third arm was empty and designated as the starting position for each trial (C). There was also a central decision point (D) we used to determine turning errors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0086271.g001

Testing occurred over 15 days, with each lizard completing one learning trial per day. A learning trial consisted of a lizard locating a food reward (cricket, Acheta domestica) placed in one of the two feeding wells. For each lizard, the food reward was randomly assigned to a feeding well (left or right) prior to the start of the trial, and the location of the reward remained constant throughout the testing period. Trials began after lizards were introduced into the empty arm of the maze. We recorded the time it took for each lizard to locate the food reward (to a maximum of 30 minutes) and the direction the lizards turned after they first entered the decision point. Lizards that did not locate the food within 30 minutes were placed next to the correct feeding well and offered a cricket with forceps. All behavioural trials were run at 27°C [16] and recorded using overhead surveillance cameras (Aucom, Security, Bundoora, Australia).

Cues and Navigation

Lizards are able to sense prey using tongue-flicking to sample chemical cues [17]. We ensured that cricket scent was present in both food wells, to eliminate scent as a long-distance direct cue to the location of the food reward (i.e. before the lizard entered one arm of the maze rather than another). We were not interested in which learning mechanisms (e.g. visual discrimination versus spatial memory) lizards used to locate their food rewards. This is because the ability to efficiently learn a novel behavioural task is likely to benefit translocated species, regardless of the mechanism they use to accomplish the task. For example, a lizard that is able to reliably locate a thermally optimal basking site may benefit by increasing the amount of time it spends at its preferred body temperature while reducing the amount of time it spends searching for basking sites in thermally sub-optimal microhabitats [18] this is true regardless of the mechanism the lizard uses to locate the site. Therefore, we provided lizards with several different types of cues (described above) that they could use to navigate the maze and locate the food reward.

Analyses

In a maze, we recognise that an animal is capable of learning if it 1) decreases its time to locate a reward, and 2) progressively takes a more direct route to the reward over successive trials. Therefore, we used two criteria for learning: a decrease in latency to the reward across the 15 trials, and an increase in the probability of taking the most direct route to the reward across the 15 trials (described in more detail below). Because we took repeated measurements on the same individuals over time (i.e. the assumption of independence between observations was not met) and we were interested in the average responses of both species in the maze rather than subject-specific responses, we used generalized estimating equations (GEE) to determine whether or not L. delicata and L. guichenoti were capable of maze learning and whether or not the two species differed in learning rate [19].

Model 1—We were interested in whether or not both species decreased the amount of time it took them to locate the food reward across the 15 trials. We used GEE with a Gamma error distribution (log link function) and an autoregressive AR(1) working correlation matrix, to assess the relationship between mean latency to the reward (outcome variable), species and trial number (explanatory variables).

We were also interested in whether or not L. delicata and L. guichenoti behaved differently in the maze. When using latency to reach a reward as an outcome variable, consistent interspecific differences in behaviour can influence individual performance scores. For example, neophobia may cause individuals to remain motionless for long periods of time in early trials, when the maze environment is unfamiliar [20]. A more exploratory species (such as L. delicata) may move through a maze more readily and locate the reward faster than a less exploratory species (such as L. guichenoti). Such behavioural differences might lead us to infer interspecific disparities in learning abilities that do not actually exist (type I error). To control for differences in species' behaviour in the maze, we calculated the amount of time a lizard spent immobile in each trial, and included this measurement as an explanatory variable in the above GEE model.

Model 2—We were also interested in whether or not L. delicata and L. guichenoti progressively took a more direct route to the reward across the 15 trials. We assessed whether lizards turned towards the reward (i.e. took the most direct route; scored as 1) or away from the reward (i.e. deviated from the most direct route; scored as 0) when they first entered the decision point. If a lizard is learning the location of the food reward, then its probability of turning towards the reward should increase over the 15 trials. We used GEE with a Binomial error distribution (logit link function), and an AR(1) working correlation matrix to assess the relationship between direction of first turn (outcome variable), species and trial number (explanatory variables).

For both models, we chose to use AR(1) working correlation matrices because in this model the output variable depends linearly on its own previous values [21]. Similarly, we expect that a lizard's performance in the Y-maze is a function of its previous maze experience and that lizard performance will improve as the number of maze trials increases. We used a robust variance estimator, which reduces the risk of confounding effects if the empirical working correlation matrix deviates from the theoretically assumed one. Gender has the potential to influence maze learning in other taxa [22], so we initially included sex as a variable in both models. However, sex was not a significant predictor of maze performance (P>0.05 in all cases), so we omitted it from both models. For models 1 and 2, we included a species×trial interaction term. This interaction did not significantly predict lizard performance so we omitted it from both models. Corrected quasilikelihood under the independence model criterion (QICC) verified that the best models included only main effects. Our data did not include any missing cases. For all statistical analyses, we used SPSS v.21 and an alpha level of P = 0.05.

Results

Model 1—Latency to the Reward

In our initial model, there was a significant effect of trial number (P<0.01) on latency to the reward, with the amount of time it took to locate the reward decreasing across the 15 trials in both species (figure 2). The lack of a significant species×trial interaction suggests that L. delicata and L. guichenoti decreased their mean latency times at the same rate. A significant species effect on latency to the reward (P<0.01) reflects the fact that L. guichenoti had lower mean latency times across all 15 trials than did L. delicata (table 1; figure 2). However, the two species also differed in the amount of time that they spent immobile (P<0.01), with L. delicata spending more time immobile than L. guichenoti across all 15 trials (figure 3a). Inclusion of “time spent immobile” in our model eliminated the significant difference between the two species in terms of latency to the reward (P = 0.17). That is, the reason that L. delicata took longer than L. guichenoti to reach the reward was simply because it spent a longer proportion of the trial immobile (table 2; figure 3b).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Time taken to reach food reward.

Mean latency times (in seconds) for L. delicata (solid line) and L. guichenoti (broken line) to reach a food reward across 15 trials in a Y-maze. Error bars represent standard errors for each species in each trial.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0086271.g002

thumbnail
Figure 3.

Panel A: Time spent immobile. Mean amount of time (in seconds) L. delicata and L. guichenoti spent immobile in each maze trial. Panel B: Latency times. Mean latency times (in seconds) for L. delicata and L. guichenoti corrected for the amount of time spent immobile in each trial. Solid lines represent L. delicata and broken lines represent L. guichenoti. Error bars represent standard errors for each species in each trial.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0086271.g003

thumbnail
Table 1. Species differences in mean latency to the reward across 15 maze trials.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0086271.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Species differences in time spent immobile across 15 maze trials.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0086271.t002

Model 2—Direction of First Turn

We did not find a significant effect of trial number (P = 0.99) or species (P = 0.054) on direction of first turn, suggesting that neither L. delicata nor L. guichenoti increased their probability of turning in the “correct” direction (i.e. toward the reward) as the trials progressed (table 3; figure 4).

thumbnail
Figure 4. Turning probability.

Probability of L. delicata (solid line) and L. guichenoti (broken line) turning towards the food reward after entering the decision point of a maze in each trial. Error bars represent standard errors for each species in each trial.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0086271.g004

thumbnail
Table 3. Species differences in first turn direction across 15 maze trials.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0086271.t003

Discussion

Our two indicators of maze learning ability were 1) a decrease in the amount of time it takes to locate the food reward, and 2) a progressively more direct route to the food reward over successive trials. Both L. delicata and L. guichenoti decreased their mean latency to the reward across 15 trials in our Y-mazes (figure 2), meeting our first criterion for maze learning. Moreover, we did not find a species×trial interaction, suggesting that both species decreased the time it took them to solve the maze at the same rate. Lampropholis guichenoti had lower mean latency times across all 15 trials (figure 2), suggesting that they consistently outperformed L. delicata; however, once we considered the time both species spent immobile (i.e. compared latency times when the lizards were actually moving), there was no significant interspecific difference in mean latencies (figure 3b).

The interspecific differences we recorded in maze behaviour (i.e. time spent immobile) may be due to habitat preferences. Not only are L. delicata more commonly found in vegetated rather than open areas [3], but Chapple et al. [3] reported that they spend more time hiding than L. guichenoti when provided with the opportunity to seek shelter in a laboratory experiment. Thus, L. delicata may be less “comfortable” in the open setting of the maze than are L. guichenoti. Modification of the maze environment might allow a more accurate measure of L. delicata's ability to complete the task. We predict that inclusion of refuges or covered ledges within the maze would reduce the time that L. delicata spends immobile, and increase the species' overall performance - perhaps to the point that this species performs as well as does its bolder congener, L. guichenoti. If animals that prefer open habitats perform better in mazes, this result could have important implications for the use of mazes in intraspecific as well as interspecific comparisons of cognitive ability. Juvenile reptiles often spend more time in covered habitats than do conspecific adults [23]. Also, pregnant female reptiles often prefer more sheltered habitats than do non-pregnant females or males [24]. This ecological heterogeneity means that variables such as age, sex and reproductive status may influence the performance of individuals in a maze task. In any cognitive test, contextual variables can influence the performance of individuals [25]. Maze designs that take into account a species' preference for sheltered or open habitats may drastically improve the performance of animals in a maze and reduce the magnitude of type I errors in comparative cognition studies.

Our next goal was to determine whether or not L. delicata and L. guichenoti decreased their mean latency times by taking a more direct route to the food reward. Neither species increased their probability of turning in the correct direction toward the reward across the 15 trials, and we did not find a species effect on direction of the first turn (figure 4). This result suggests that neither L. delicata nor L. guichenoti progressively took a shorter route to the food reward across the 15 trials and thus, our second criterion for maze learning was not met. If a species steadily decreases its time to locate a reward within a maze without taking a more direct route, it suggests that the species has habituated to the maze environment and simply searches the maze more rapidly over successive trials [20]. In keeping with this interpretation, our data show a rapid decrease in the amount of time spent immobile in successive trials (figure 3a). Based on these data, neither L. delicata or L. guichenoti were capable of learning the position of a food reward within a Y-maze. Instead, both species appeared to locate the reward by performing increasingly rapid serial searches of the maze environment. However, our results do not preclude the possibility that L. delicata and L. guichenoti are capable of learning the location of a food reward within a Y-maze. Given a greater number of trials, L. delicata and L. guichenoti may have learned to reliably locate the reward.

Species that can rapidly solve novel ecological challenges are predicted to have an advantage during introduction events [6], [8]. Although neither L. delicata nor L. guichenoti learned the location of a food source within 15 trials, this result does not mean that intelligence is irrelevant to establishment success. We only tested learning ability in a single behavioural context, which is unlikely to provide an accurate representation of species' intelligence [4], [26]. Further cognitive testing using multiple experimental frameworks may reveal that L. delicata is capable of solving a variety of ecologically relevant challenges more efficiently than can L. guichenoti. Such experiments would give a more comprehensive comparison of cognition between these two species, and a more robust test of the hypothesis that intelligence facilitates invasion success.

Another factor that may influence learning ability and therefore, invasive potential, is age. As animals age, cognitive functions such as learning and memory deteriorate [27]. Indeed, using the same Y-mazes and a similar experimental design, we found that hatchling three-lined skinks (Bassiana duperreyi) decreased their time to locate a food reward and took a more direct route to the reward across 15 trials, providing strong evidence that B. duperreyi learned the location of the food reward [14]. It would be interesting to see if hatchling L. delicata and L. guichenoti display a similar capacity for learning as do hatchling B. duperreyi. Interspecific disparities in learning ability may also be more apparent in younger age classes. These hypotheses could be easily tested using hatchling L. delicata and L. guichenoti, and the same experimental design we used in the present study. If greater learning ability does correlate positively with establishment success, then the age of individuals at the time of introduction may be a strong predictor of invasive potential.

Finally, learning ability might not have a significant influence on establishment success at all. Rather, alternative pre-existing behavioural traits (such as aggressiveness, and habitat preference and flexibility) may predict establishment success more accurately. For example, Chapple et al. [3] suggest that relative to L. guichenoti, L. delicata's exploratory nature and propensity to hide increases its propagule pressure during introductions, which in turn increases the probability that L. delicata will successfully establish in new environments. If other behavioural traits can predict establishment success more effectively than intelligence, does this not invalidate previous studies, which have suggested that relatively large brains provide translocated individuals with a selective advantage [6][8]? Not necessarily. A larger brain may have numerous functional consequences, including non-cognitive advantages in sensory and motor functions. Any such attribute plausibly could enhance invasion success [7]. Therefore, the putative role of intelligence as a predictor of invasion success remains unclear. In order to understand this relationship, we will need to explore cognitive disparities among successful and unsuccessful invaders in a variety of different contexts.

Acknowledgments

For statistical advice, we thank Mats Olsson in the School of Biological Sciences at the University of Sydney.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: CNB JJA RS. Performed the experiments: CNB. Analyzed the data: JJA CNB RS. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: RS JJA. Wrote the paper: CNB JJA RS.

References

  1. 1. Blackburn TM, Pyšek P, Bacher S, Carlton JT, Duncan RP, et al. (2011) A proposed unified framework for biological invasions. Trends Ecol Evol 26: 333–339.
  2. 2. Chapple DG, Simmonds SM, Wong B (2012) Can behavioural and personality traits influence the success of unintentional species introductions? Trends Ecol Evol 27: 57–64.
  3. 3. Chapple DG, Simmonds SM, Wong B (2011) Know when to run, know when to hide: can behavioural differences explain the divergent invasion success of two sympatric lizards? Ecol Evol 1: 278–289.
  4. 4. Roth G, Dicke U (2005) Evolution of the brain and intelligence. Trends Cogn Sci 9: 250–257.
  5. 5. Sol D, Duncan RP, Blackburn TM, Cassey P, Lefebvre L (2005) Big brains, enhanced cognition, and response of birds to novel environments. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102: 5460–5465.
  6. 6. Sol D, Székely T, Liker A, Lefebvre L (2007) Big-brained birds survive better in nature. Proc R Soc B 274: 763–769.
  7. 7. Amiel JJ, Tingley R, Shine R (2011) Smart moves: effects of relative brain size on establishment success of invasive amphibians and reptiles. PLoS ONE 6: e18277.
  8. 8. Sol D, Bacher S, Reader SM, Lefebvre L (2008) Brain size predicts the success of mammal species introduced into novel environments. Am Nat 172: S63–S71.
  9. 9. Chapple D, Hoskin C, Chapple S, Thompson M (2011) Phylogeographic divergence in the widespread delicate skink (Lampropholis delicata) corresponds to dry habitat barriers in eastern Australia. BMC Evol Biol 11: 191.
  10. 10. Chapple DG, Chapple SN, Thompson MB (2011) Biogeographic barriers in south-eastern Australia drive phylogeographic divergence in the garden skink, Lampropholis guichenoti. J Biogeogr 38: 1761–1775.
  11. 11. Gill B, Bejakovtch D, Whitaker A (2001) Records of foreign reptiles and amphibians accidentally imported to New Zealand. NZ J Zool 28: 351–359.
  12. 12. Kraus F (2009) Alien reptiles and amphibians: a scientific compendium and analysis. Springer Science+Business Media B.V.
  13. 13. Sih A, Bell A, Johnson JC (2004) Behavioural syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends Ecol Evol 19: 372–378.
  14. 14. Amiel JJ, Lindström T, Shine R (2013) Egg incubation effects generate positive correlations between size, speed and learning ability in young lizards. Anim Cogn published online first (DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-013-0665-4).
  15. 15. Leal M, Powell BJ (2012) Behavioural flexibility and problem-solving in a tropical lizard. Biol Lett 8: 28–30.
  16. 16. Cromie GL, Chapple DG (2012) Impact of tail loss on the behaviour and locomotor performance of two sympatric Lampropholis skink species. PLoS ONE 7: e34732.
  17. 17. Cooper W (1994) Chemical discrimination by tongue-flicking in lizards: A review with hypotheses on its origin and its ecological and phylogenetic relationships. J Chem Ecol 20: 439–487.
  18. 18. Torr GA, Shine R (1993) An experimental analysis of thermally-dependent behavior patterns in the scincid lizard Lampropholis guichenoti. Copeia 1993 3: 850–854.
  19. 19. Zeger SL, Liang K-Y, Albert PS (1988) Models for longitudinal data: a generalized estimating equation approach. Biometrics 44: 1049–1060.
  20. 20. Day LB, Crews D, Wilczynski W (1999) Spatial and reversal learning in congeneric lizards with different foraging strategies. Anim Behav 57: 393–407.
  21. 21. Ballinger GA (2004) Using generalized estimating equations for longitudinal data analysis. Organ Res Meth 7: 127–150.
  22. 22. Roof RL, Stein DG (1999) Gender differences in Morris water maze performance depend on task parameters. Physiol Behav 68: 81–86.
  23. 23. Blomberg S, Shine R (2004) Practical census techniques for animal populations. In: Sutherland W, editor. Reptiles. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  24. 24. Graves BM, Duvall D (1993) Reproduction, rookery use, and thermoregulation in free-ranging, pregnant Crotalus v. viridis. J Herpetol 27: 33–41.
  25. 25. Macphail EM, Barlow HB (1985) Vertebrate intellegence: the null hypothesis [and discussion]. Phil Trans R Soc B 308: 37–51.
  26. 26. Auersperg AM, von Bayern AM, Gajdon GK, Huber L, Kacelnik A (2011) Flexibility in problem solving and tool use of kea and New Caledonian crows in a multi access box paradigm. PLoS ONE 6: e20231.
  27. 27. Liu R, Liu IY, Bi X, Thompson RF, Doctrow SR, et al. (2003) Reversal of age-related learning deficits and brain oxidative stress in mice with superoxide dismutase/catalase mimetics. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100: 8526–8531.