Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Cortical Thickness and Behavior Abnormalities in Children Born Preterm

  • Leire Zubiaurre-Elorza,

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain, Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Barcelona, Spain

  • Sara Soria-Pastor,

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain, Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Barcelona, Spain, Clinical Institute of Neurosciences, Hospital Clínic Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain

  • Carme Junque ,

    cjunque@ub.edu

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain, Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Barcelona, Spain

  • Roser Sala-Llonch,

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain, Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Barcelona, Spain

  • Dolors Segarra,

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain, Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Barcelona, Spain

  • Nuria Bargallo,

    Affiliation Neuroradiology Section, Radiology Department, Centre de Diagnòstic per la Imatge (CDIC), Hospital Clínic, Barcelona, Spain

  • Alfons Macaya

    Affiliation Grup de Recerca en Neurologia Pediàtrica, Institut de Recerca Vall d’Hebron, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain

Abstract

Aim

To identify long-term effects of preterm birth and of periventricular leukomalacia (PVL) on cortical thickness (CTh). To study the relationship between CTh and cognitive-behavioral abnormalities.

Methods

We performed brain magnetic resonance imaging on 22 preterm children with PVL, 14 preterm children with no evidence of PVL and 22 full-term peers. T1-weighted images were analyzed with FreeSurfer software. All participants underwent cognitive and behavioral assessments by means of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) and the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL).

Results

We did not find global CTh differences between the groups. However, a thinner cortex was found in left postcentral, supramarginal, and caudal middle rostral gyri in preterm children with no evidence of PVL than in the full-term controls, while PVL preterm children showed thicker cortex in right pericalcarine and left rostral middle frontal areas than in preterm children with no evidence of PVL. In the PVL group, internalizing and externalizing scores correlated mainly with CTh in frontal areas. Attentional scores were found to be higher in PVL and correlated with CTh increments in right frontal areas.

Interpretation

The preterm group with no evidence of PVL, when compared with full-term children, showed evidence of a different pattern of regional thinning in the cortical gray matter. In turn, PVL preterm children exhibited atypical increases in CTh that may underlie their prevalent behavioral problems.

Introduction

Neonates born preterm have a high risk of developing long-term psychiatric and behavioral disorders [1][3]. The degree to which prematurity itself or associated cerebral lesions, i.e. the encephalopathy of prematurity, may account for this elevated risk, is currently unknown. Specifically, preterm birth has been related to attention/hyperactivity symptoms [4][6], a high prevalence of autism [7], and higher scores in other behavioral disorders [8][10].

Normal development in childhood is accompanied by an increase in cortical thickness (CTh) in almost all brain areas, whereas there is a progressive cortical thinning during adolescence [11]. Studies in neurodevelopmental disorders have reported both thicker and thinner cortical regions. For instance, cortical thickening has been described in autism [12], [13], whereas cortical thinning has been found in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [14].

Several studies have reported global and regional differences in brain tissue volumes in preterm children [15], [16] and PVL preterm cohorts [17][19]. However, there have been only two studies in adolescent subjects where the effect of preterm birth on CTh is investigated. Martinussen et al [20] found different areas of CTh abnormalities (both increments and decrements) at a mean age of 15 years in very low birth-weight and small for gestational age preterm subsamples. Moreover, the study of Nagy et al [21] found similar results in preterm adolescents after controlling for age at evaluation. Since CTh is subject to dynamic changes during normal development, it is conceivable that delayed or altered development in premature children with PVL might result in a distinct pattern of CTh. Conversely, previous studies in preterm samples have not characterized the effects of PVL on CTh.

The aim of the current study was to investigate the effect of prematurity and PVL on CTh in childhood and the putative impact of the latter on cognition and behavior. Our hypothesis is that preterm birth and brain injury such as PVL could have an impact on CTh and that these cortical abnormalities may be related to cognitive-behavioral outcomes at school age.

Methods

Participant Characteristics

thumbnail
Table 1. Characteristics of the samples: neonatal and demographic data.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042148.t001

The PVL group was recruited between June 2008 and May 2009 at the Hospital Universitari Vall d’Hebron (Barcelona, Spain) and consisted of 75 children (6–12 years of age) born preterm and with a history of PVL. Selection criteria included: gestational age (GA) <37 weeks, full intelligence quotient (FIQ) >70, neonatal diagnosis of PVL, and signs of PVL in the current MRI according to the criteria proposed by Flodmark et al [22]. The patients with PVL were recruited after being diagnosed with PVL in the neonatal period by means of ultrasound scans. This was defined as the presence of periventricular cysts of differing size and extension (PVL grades II–IV) or periventricular echodensities persisting over 14 days during the neonatal period. Neonatal peri/intraventricular hemorrhage grades III/IV or any other perinatal brain injury were exclusion criteria. From the 75 subjects, 21 were not available at the time of the study, 18 declined to participate, 11 were delivered at term, one did not show signs of PVL in the current MRI and two subjects had a FIQ <70. After applying these exclusion criteria, the sample comprised 22 PVL preterm participants. The clinical and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) findings for this sample have previously been published [19].

Fourteen non-PVL preterm children, at low risk for neurodevelopmental deficits, were selected from a cohort followed at the Hospital Clinic (Barcelona, Spain). To be considered a low-risk preterm the following criteria were applied: 1) GA = 30–34 weeks, 2) birth weight below 2500 g, 3) Apgar score at fifth minute >7, 4) absence of major neonatal morbidity, including severe respiratory distress syndrome, mechanical ventilation, necrotizing enterocolitis, neonatal sepsis or bronchopulmonary dysplasia, and 5) absence of cerebral pathology on neonatal cranial ultrasound [16]. For the current study, the subcohort of low-risk preterms (non-PVL preterm children) was selected on the basis of their neonatal characteristics (GA and birth weight), which were group-mean matched with the PVL preterm sample. In addition, 22 infants delivered at term at the same center were enrolled as a full-term control group who were mainly friends and classmates of the preterm children. Exclusion criteria for all the groups included an FIQ equal to or less than 70, any history of brain injury and contraindications for an MRI scan. Table 1 summarizes the demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample. The highest education level of the parents was recorded as low, intermediate or high as in a previous preterm study conducted in adulthood [23]. The Ethics Committee of the University of Barcelona approved this study, and written informed parental consent was obtained for each infant.

Neurodevelopmental Outcome Data

All participants were cognitively assessed with the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children - Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) [24]. Behavioral data were collected by means of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) [25]. The checklist is a parent-report measure of children’s behavior. It is divided into the following subscales: Aggressive Behavior, Anxious/Depressed, Attention Problems, Delinquent Rule-Breaking Behavior, Social Problems, Somatic Complaints, Thought Problems, and Withdrawn. Some of these subscales group into two higher order factors – internalizing (withdrawn, somatic complaints and anxious/depressed scales) and externalizing (delinquent and aggressive behavior scales) – whose sum provides the total score.

MRI Acquisition and Analysis

The three-dimensional MRI data sets were acquired at the Centre de Diagnòstic per la Imatge (Hospital Clinic, Barcelona, Spain). High-resolution T1-weighted images were acquired for all subjects on a 3-Tesla TIM TRIO scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). The following parameters were used: a MPRAGE sequence in sagittal orientation (TR/TE = 2300/2.98 ms; TI = 900 ms; 256×256 matrix, flip angle 9°, 1 mm3 isotropic voxel).

An automated cortical reconstruction of the T1-weighted images was performed by using FreeSurfer (version 4.3.1) software (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu). This method was used to create a three-dimensional cortical surface model of CTh using intensity and continuity information previously described in detail [26]. Processing of T1 high-resolution images included several procedures: removal of non-brain tissue [27], automated Talairach transformation, intensity normalization [28], tessellation of the gray matter/white matter boundary, automated topology correction, [29], [30] and surface deformation to detect gray matter/white matter and gray matter/cerebrospinal fluid boundaries [26]. Moreover, the cerebral cortex was divided into different regions according to gyral and sulcal structure information [31]. The resulting representation of cortical thickness was calculated as the distance between tissue boundaries (gray matter/white matter and gray matter/cerebrospinal fluid) [26].

Statistical Analysis

The data were tested for normality and homogeneity. To do this, a multivariate analysis of variance was carried out to test the differences in the quantitative variables (demographic, neuropsychological, and behavioral variables) among the three groups. The chi-square test of independence was performed to analyze differences in proportions when appropriate. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS v. 18.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL).

CTh maps were analyzed at the vertex-wise level separately for each hemisphere, using a general linear model approach as implemented by the FreeSurfer QDEC application. Before group analysis comparisons, individual CTh maps were registered to the standard template and smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 15 mm FWHM. We performed t-test comparisons to evaluate CTh differences due to (1) prematurity itself (non-PVL preterm children versus full-term group), (2) PVL per se (PVL preterm group versus non-PVL sample), and (3) PVL plus prematurity (PVL preterm group versus full-term children). Moreover, we performed a correlation between the CTh and CBCL main indexes, the attention subscale, and FIQ. We adjusted CTh for age at scan in order to remove any variance in CTh associated with the effects of normal development and thereby avoid false positives and increase statistical power. Maps were corrected for multiple comparisons (family-wise error; FWE) using the Monte Carlo Null-Z simulation with 10,000 permutations. Global mean thickness and regional thickness values comprised in the clusters that were statistically significant in the CTh maps were obtained and tested in SPSS with age at evaluation as a covariate.

Results

Neonatal, Demographic, Cognitive, and Behavioral Data

Neonatal data are detailed in Table 1. The three groups were comparable for the purpose of independently evaluating the effects of PVL or preterm birth.

Although general intelligence mean scores were within the normal range for PVL preterm (92.32±14.2), normal preterm (106.8±13.8), and term groups (121.91±15.4), there were differences in FIQ among the three samples (F = 21.01; P<.001). Behavioral scores (Table 2) showed differences in PVL preterm children compared to full-term controls only in the attentional scale.

Cortical Thickness Differences

Analysis of global CTh revealed no statistical differences between groups (left hemisphere: F = 0.71, P = .50; PVL preterm children: 2.91±0.15 mm; non-PVL preterm children: 2.87±0.08 mm; term children: 2.91±0.07 mm; and right hemisphere: F = 0.75, P = .50; PVL preterms: 2.86±0.15 mm; non-PVL preterms: 2.84±0.80 mm; term children: 2.88±0.07 mm).

Prematurity and PVL Effects on Regional Cortical Thickness

Results from the t-test comparisons between groups are detailed in Table 3 and Figure 1. When comparing PVL preterm children to non-PVL preterm children a thicker cortex was observed, whereas when the same group was compared to their term peers, both regional thicker and thinner cortices were found bilaterally. When comparing non-PVL preterm sample to full-term children, regional decrements in CTh were found in specific areas of the left hemisphere.

thumbnail
Table 3. Regional differences in cortical thickness between groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042148.t003

thumbnail
Figure 1. Maps of regional CTh comparisons between groups.

(A) PVL preterm versus non-PVL preterm (B) Non-PVL preterm and (C) PVL preterm versus full-term children. Colour bars represent statistically significant (P<.05) thinning (blue) or thickening (yellow). Scatter plots show mean CTh values for each group and for illustration purposes have not been corrected for covariate effects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042148.g001

Correlations between CTh, FIQ, and CBCL Scores

Regarding the general intelligence scores, a statistically significant negative correlation between FIQ and CTh was found only in the full-term group. Specifically, the maximum coordinates of the statistically significant clusters were located in the left superior frontal (cluster size  = 1372 mm2, cluster-level p value  = 0.0228, Talairach coordinates; x = −18.8, y = 59.4, z = 6.2) and right postcentral regions (cluster size  = 1458 mm2, cluster-level p value  = 0.020, Talairach coordinates; x = 52.9, y = −12.6, z = 44.2). However, the correlation did not achieve statistical significance either in the non-PVL preterm group or in the PVL preterm group.

For the behavior measures, we found statistically significant positive correlations only within the PVL preterm group. The specific regions were associated with the three main indexes of CBCL – the internalizing, externalizing, and total scores are shown in Table 4. Figure 2 displays internalizing and externalizing scores and their respective correlations with regional CTh. No correlations were found between CTh and CBCL main index scores for the full-term and non-PVL preterm children. In PVL preterm children, attentional scores correlated positively with CTh in right frontal areas (Table 5 and Fig. 3).

thumbnail
Table 4. PVL sample; positive correlations between CTh and behavioural scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042148.t004

thumbnail
Figure 2. Correlation analysis between regional CTh and (A) internalizing or (B) externalizing indexes in the PVL-preterm group.

The colour bar represents statistically significant (P<.05) thinning (blue) or thickening (yellow). Partial correlation statistics and scatter plots of mean CT in each area are displayed corrected for covariate effects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042148.g002

thumbnail
Table 5. Statistically significant correlations between CTh and attention problems variable in PVL-preterm and Preterm groups.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042148.t005

thumbnail
Figure 3. Map of correlation between attention subscale and CTh in PVL children.

The colour bar represents statistically significant (P<.05) thinning (blue) or thickening (yellow). Partial correlation statistics and scatter plots of mean CT in each area are displayed corrected for covariate effects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042148.g003

Discussion

In this MRI study we documented that school-aged children with a history of preterm birth, with or without PVL, show abnormal patterns of CTh when compared with their control peers. Even though we did not find differences between groups in global CTh, our findings provide evidence that prematurity per se is associated with regional cortical thinning, whereas PVL resulted in specific areas of CTh thickening. Attentional scores were statistically higher in PVL children and correlated positively with right frontal thickness. Correlations between CTh and behavioral indexes were only observed in PVL preterm children. Finally, only the full-term group showed a significant negative correlation between FIQ and CTh.

The fact that regionally thicker and thinner cortical regions were found, is in agreement with previous studies in adolescents that used comparisons with typically developing brains [20], [21]. Martinussen et al. [20] studied a group of adolescents with a history of very low birth-weight and identified thinner parieto-temporo-occipital areas, as well as thicker fronto-occipital areas. In our study, we also found thinner cortex in the non-PVL preterm group involving extensive frontal and parietal regions and small areas in the temporal and occipital cortex. The cortical thinning is more restricted in our sample, probably because we used the Monte Carlo multiple comparisons correction. A similar preterm sample was analyzed by Nagy et al. [21], who observed thinner cortices in the parietal and temporal areas but thicker cortices in the inferior temporal gyrus and left ventrolateral prefrontal regions. We speculate that these decrements in CTh found in non-PVL preterm children might translate into a delayed maturation of some cortical areas, since these regions are supposed to reach their maximal thickness in childhood.11 The current preterm sample with no evidence of PVL is a subgroup of a sample used in a previous gray matter volumetric study [16]. There is a correspondence between current CTh findings and previous voxel-based morphometry results, in the sense that parietal and temporal areas showed both thinning and smaller volumes. However, there are other areas that showed volumetric reductions and did not show reductions in CTh. These discrepancies are probably due to the different neuroimaging approaches used in the two studies (voxel-based approach vs. vertex-based approach) and the fact that cerebral volume is a composite brain measure of both CTh and surface area, which may show reductions in preterm children.

No previous study has investigated PVL influences on CTh by including an appropriate control preterm cohort with no focal white matter abnormalities. Our PVL preterm sample showed thinner CTh in the temporal lobe, involving the medial and middle regions, when compared to full-term controls. This thinning is in keeping with a previous volumetric study using voxel-based morphometry [19], but some of the regions that showed volume decrements in that study, such as the postcentral, inferior parietal, middle frontal, and inferior temporal regions, did not show CTh reductions. Since gray matter volume depends on both CTh and surface area as explained above, and based on previous experimental observations [32], we speculate that PVL targeting of the neural precursor population at the periventricular germinal matrix may result in an undermining of the horizontal expansion of the developing cortex without an impairment of CTh. It has been proposed that volume is more closely related to surface area than cortical thickness [33]. On the other hand, thicker occipital lobe was seen bilaterally. This finding concurs with that of a study in adolescents, where subjects with and without PVL were analyzed together [20]. In fact, when we tried to isolate the effect of PVL by comparing preterms with and without PVL, we found that the presence of white matter lesions was associated with increased regional CTh in the pericalcarine and rostral middle frontal areas. Thicker cortices may reflect an atypical cerebral development with changes in the normal development curves. Thicker cortical regions in the same age range is a common finding in some neurodevelopmental disorders, such as autism [12], [13], and in absence epilepsy [34]. CTh is thought to be dependent on the processes of gray matter dendritic arborization and pruning [35] and the degree of myelinization at the gray/white matter interface [36]. Thus, disproportionate pruning, reduced neuronal/glial complement or immature development in specific cortical regions might be present in non-PVL preterm children, whereas an abnormal or delayed pruning program might occur in some PVL cortical areas. Further longitudinal studies with larger samples may help to clarify these CTh developmental trajectories in PVL preterm samples.

Intelligence scores have been correlated with gray matter volume in children born preterm without [15], [16] or with PVL [37]. In a recent study, a combination of total white matter volume together with the cross-sectional area of the corpus callosum explained most of the FIQ variance in preterm adolescents, irrespective of the extent of white matter injury [38]. In the present study, although the FIQ was significantly lower in the two samples of preterm children, we found a negative correlation only between FIQ and CTh in the full-term sample. Thus, higher FIQs were associated with cortical thinning. These findings are in line with previous reports that have also found this negative correlation in normal samples of children and adolescents [39], [40]. It has been suggested that cortical thinning reflects the process of synaptic pruning leading to improved efficiency of cerebral connectivity.

In addition to cognitive deficits, preterm samples also display behavioral problems compared to their term peers [41]. In our study, the main indexes for behavior did not differ between groups, as we and others have previously described [16], [42]. However, the attention subscale was found to be altered in PVL preterm children compared to full-term sample. Attention problems are a common finding in preterm children [2], [43]. Decrements in caudate nucleus volume [44], as well as in fractional anisotropy in the internal/external capsule and inferior/middle fascicles [45], have been related to attention deficits in preterm adolescents. Interestingly, we found that PVL preterm children showed the more pronounced thicker regions of the right hemisphere that are essential parts of the attentional network [46]. Indeed, frontal cortex and fronto-striatal structural and functional abnormalities are a common finding in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [47].

Regarding behavioral problems, we found that CTh in frontal areas correlated with internalizing and externalizing indexes. Internalizing problems imply higher scores in withdrawn, somatic complaints, and anxious/depressed scales, whereas externalizing problems reflect delinquent and aggressive behavior domains. We found that the higher the CTh values in the frontal areas, the more serious were the behavioral problems detected in childhood. In this respect, it is worth noting that the frontal cortical regions are involved in: (1) executive abilities (i.e. maintenance and redirection of attention, mental flexibility, shift in behavioral settings, etc.) dependent on the dorsolateral prefrontal circuit; (2) production of appropriate behaviors in response to social demands, which relate to the orbitofrontal circuit, and (3) generation of motivated behavior, which is controlled by the anterior cingulate gyrus [48]. In a recent study in healthy children, higher scores on the CBCL aggressive subscale were related to thinner anterior cingulate cortex [49]. However, our results showed a positive correlation between externalizing scores and CTh in cingulate cortex. As postulated for intelligence scores, the regional cortical thickness increments found in PVL preterm children, presumably due to defective synaptic pruning, may account for their worse scores in behavioral domains.

The main limitation of our study is its small sample size, which limits the statistical power. However, our study has some specific strengths. First, we selected appropriate control groups to analyze the independent impact of prematurity and PVL on CTh at school age. Second, we considered the characteristic trajectories of CTh regional growth curves occurring in the normal developing brain. Most brain regions attain the maximum peak of increment during childhood, followed by a decrement in adolescence and a subsequent stability in adulthood [11]. Consequently, all our analyses of global and regional CTh were controlled by age at examination. Finally, it is worth highlighting that CBCL is a parent-report form and might therefore be somewhat subjective.

This study provides evidence of the different patterns of CTh in childhood between a preterm cohort with no evidence of focal white matter abnormalities and a preterm cohort with brain injury such as PVL. In PVL preterm children, behavioral abnormalities are associated with thicker frontal areas, and attentional deficits are associated with increased CTh in several regions of the right hemisphere. Further research is needed to (1) determine developing CTh curves in PVL from the neonatal period onwards, (2) investigate whether the CTh differences observed in childhood persist into adolescence, and (3) establish whether they have a long-term impact on the cognitive and behavioral domains.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: LZE CJ DS AM. Performed the experiments: LZE RSL SSP. Analyzed the data: LZE RSL NB. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: LZE SSP RSL NB. Wrote the paper: LZE SSP CJ RSL DS NB AM.

References

  1. 1. Stewart AL, Rifkin L, Amess PN, Kirkbride V, Townsend JP, et al. (1999) Brain structure and neurocognitive and behavioural function in adolescents who were born very preterm. Lancet 353: 1653–1657.
  2. 2. Bhutta AT, Cleves MA, Casey PH, Cradock MM, Anand KJ (2002) Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: A meta-analysis. JAMA 288: 728–737.
  3. 3. Reijneveld SA, de Kleine MJ, van Baar AL, Kollee LA, Verhaak CM, et al. (2006) Behavioural and emotional problems in very preterm and very low birthweight infants at age 5 years. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 91: F423–428.
  4. 4. Anderson PJ, De Luca CR, Hutchinson E, Spencerñ-Smith MM, Roberts G, et al. (2010) Attention Problems in a Representative Sample of Extremely Preterm/Extremely Low Birth Weight Children. Dev Neuropsychol 36(1): 57–73.
  5. 5. Shum D, Neulinger K, O’Callaghan M, Mohay H (2008) Attentional problems in children born very preterm or with extremely low birth weight at 7–9 years. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 23(1): 103–12.
  6. 6. Samara M, Marlow N, Wolke D (2008) for the EPICure Study group (2008) Pervasive Behavior Problems at 6 Years of Age in a Total-Population Sample of Children Born at <25 Weeks of Gestation. Pediatrics 122(3): 562–573.
  7. 7. Limperopoulus C, Bassan H, Sullivan NR, Soul JS, Robertson RL, et al. (2008) Positive Screening for Autism in Ex-preterm Infants: Prevalence and Risk Factors. Pediatrics 121(4): 758–765.
  8. 8. Stjernqvist K, Svenningsen NW (1999) Ten-year follow-up of children born before 29 gestational weeks: health, cognitive development, behaviour and school achievement. Acta Paediatr 88(5): 557–62.
  9. 9. Anderson P (2003) Doyle LW; Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group (2003) Neurobehavioral outcomes of school-age children born extremely low birth weight or very preterm in the 1990s. JAMA 289(24): 3264–3272.
  10. 10. Gray RF, Indurkhya A, McCormick MC (2004) Prevalence, stability, and predictors of clinically significant behavior problems in low birth weight children at 3, 5, and 8 years of age. Pediatrics 114(3): 736–743.
  11. 11. Shaw P, Kabani NJ, Lerch JP, Eckstrand K, Lenroot R, et al. (2008) Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J Neurosci 28: 3586–3594.
  12. 12. Raznahan A, Toro R, Daly E, Robertson D, Murphy C, et al. (2010) Cortical anatomy in autism spectrum disorder: An in vivo MRI study on the effect of age. Cereb Cortex 20: 1332–1340.
  13. 13. Mak-Fan KM, Taylor MJ, Roberts W, Lerch JP (2012) Measures of cortical grey matter structure and development in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 42(3): 419–427.
  14. 14. Shaw P, Eckstrand K, Sharp W, Blumenthal J, Lerch JP, et al. (2007) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104: 19649–19654.
  15. 15. Peterson BS, Vohr B, Staib LH, Cannistraci CJ, Dolberg A, et al. (2000) Regional brain volume abnormalities and long-term cognitive outcome in preterm infants. JAMA 284: 1939–1947.
  16. 16. Soria-Pastor S, Padilla N, Zubiaurre-Elorza L, Ibarretxe-Bilbao N, Botet F, et al. (2009) Decreased regional brain volume and cognitive impairment in preterm children at low risk. Pediatrics 124: e1161–1170.
  17. 17. Inder TE, Huppi PS, Warfield S, Kikinis R, Zientara GP, et al. (1999) Periventricular white matter injury in the premature infant is followed by reduced cerebral cortical gray matter volume at term. Ann Neurol 46: 755–760.
  18. 18. Tzarouchi LC, Astrakas LG, Zikou A, Xydis V, Kosta P, et al. (2009) Periventricular leukomalacia in preterm children: Assessment of grey and white matter and cerebrospinal fluid changes by MRI. Pediatr Radiol 39: 1327–1332.
  19. 19. Zubiaurre-Elorza L, Soria-Pastor S, Junque C, Segarra D, Bargallo N, et al. (2011) Gray matter volume decrements in preterm children with periventricular leukomalacia. Pediatr Res 69: 554–560.
  20. 20. Martinussen M, Fischl B, Larsson HB, Skranes J, Kulseng S, et al. (2005) Cerebral cortex thickness in 15-year-old adolescents with low birth weight measured by an automated MRI-based method. Brain 128: 2588–2596.
  21. 21. Nagy Z, Lagercrantz H, Hutton C (2011) Effects of preterm birth on cortical thickness measured in adolescence. Cereb Cortex 21: 300–306.
  22. 22. Flodmark O, Roland EH, Hill A, Whitfield MF (1987) Periventricular leukomalacia: radiologic diagnosis. Radiology 162: 119–124.
  23. 23. Weisglas-Kuperus N, Hille EE, Duivenvoorden HH, Finken MJ, Wit JM, et al. (2009) Intelligence of very preterm or very low birth weight infants in young adulthood. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 94: F196–200.
  24. 24. Wechsler D (2007) Escala de inteligencia para niños-IV WISC-IV. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
  25. 25. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA (2001) Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth & Families.
  26. 26. Fischl B, Dale AM (2000) Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97: 11050–11055.
  27. 27. Segonne F, Dale AM, Busa E, Glessner M, Salat D, et al. (2004) A hybrid approach to the skull stripping problem in MRI. Neuroimage 22: 1060–1075.
  28. 28. Sled JG, Zijdenbos AP, Evans AC (1998) A nonparametric method for automatic correction of intensity nonuniformity in MRI data. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 17: 87–97.
  29. 29. Fischl B, Liu A, Dale AM (2001) Automated manifold surgery: Constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortex. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 20: 70–80.
  30. 30. Segonne F, Pacheco J, Fischl B (2007) Geometrically accurate topology-correction of cortical surfaces using nonseparating loops. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 26: 518–529.
  31. 31. Desikan RS, Segonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, et al. (2006) An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31: 968–980.
  32. 32. Chenn A, Walsh CA (2002) Regulation of cerebral cortical size by control of cell cycle exit in neural precursors. Science 297: 365–369.
  33. 33. Winkler AM, Kochunov P, Blangero J, Almasy L, Zilles K, et al. (2010) Cortical thickness or grey matter volume? The importance of selecting the phenotype for imaging genetics studies. Neuroimage 53: 1135–1146.
  34. 34. Tosun D, Siddarth P, Toga AW, Hermann B, Caplan R (2011) Effects of childhood absence epilepsy on associations between regional cortical morphometry and aging and cognitive abilities. Hum Brain Mapp 32: 580–591.
  35. 35. Huttenlocher PR (1990) Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia 28: 517–527.
  36. 36. Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Leonard CM, Welcome SE, Kan E, et al. (2004) Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. J Neurosci 24: 8223–8231.
  37. 37. Zubiaurre-Elorza L, Soria-Pastor S, Junque C, Fernandez-Espejo D, Segarra D, et al. (2012) Thalamic changes in a preterm sample with periventricular leukomalacia: correlation with white matter integrity and cognitive outcome at school age. Pediatr Res 71(4 Pt 1): 354–360.
  38. 38. Northam GB, Liegeois F, Chong WK, Wyatt JS, Baldeweg T (2011) Total brain white matter is a major determinant of IQ in adolescents born preterm. Ann Neurol 69: 702–711.
  39. 39. Shaw P, Greenstein D, Lerch J, Clasen L, Lenroot R, et al. (2006) Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature 440: 676–679.
  40. 40. Luders E, Thompson PM, Narr KL, Zamanyan A, Chou YY, et al. (2011) The link between callosal thickness and intelligence in healthy children and adolescents. Neuroimage 54: 1823–1830.
  41. 41. Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, Oosterlaan J (2009) Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics 124: 717–728.
  42. 42. Gurka MJ, LoCasale-Crouch J, Blackman JA (2010) Long-term cognition, achievement, socioemotional, and behavioral development of healthy late-preterm infants. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 164: 525–532.
  43. 43. Kulseng S, Jennekens-Schinkel A, Naess P, Romundstad P, Indredavik M, et al. (2006) Very-low-birthweight and term small-for-gestational-age adolescents: Attention revisited. Acta Paediatr 95: 224–230.
  44. 44. Nosarti C, Allin MP, Frangou S, Rifkin L, Murray RM (2005) Hyperactivity in adolescents born very preterm is associated with decreased caudate volume. Biol Psychiatry 57: 661–666.
  45. 45. Skranes J, Vangberg TR, Kulseng S, Indredavik MS, Evensen KA, et al. (2007) Clinical findings and white matter abnormalities seen on diffusion tensor imaging in adolescents with very low birth weight. Brain 130: 654–666.
  46. 46. Raz A, Buhle J (2006) Typologies of attentional networks. Nat Rev Neurosci 7: 367–379.
  47. 47. Cubillo A, Halari R, Smith A, Taylor E, Rubia K (2011) A review of fronto-striatal and fronto-cortical brain abnormalities in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and new evidence for dysfunction in adults with ADHD during motivation and attention. 48(2): 194–215.
  48. 48. Tekin S, Cummings JL (2002) Frontal-subcortical neuronal circuits and clinical neuropsychiatry: An update. J Psychosom Res 53: 647–654.
  49. 49. Ducharme S, Hudziak JJ, Botteron KN, Ganjavi H, Lepage C, et al. (2011) Right anterior cingulate cortical thickness and bilateral striatal volume correlate with child behavior checklist aggressive behavior scores in healthy children. Biol Psychiatry 70: 283–290.