Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Neandertal Humeri May Reflect Adaptation to Scraping Tasks, but Not Spear Thrusting

  • Colin N. Shaw ,

    cns30@cam.ac.uk

    Affiliations PAVE Research Group, Department of Archaeology & Anthropology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom, McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, Department of Archaeology and Anthropology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom

  • Cory L. Hofmann,

    Affiliation Neuromotion Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Michael D. Petraglia,

    Affiliation Research Laboratory for Archaeology and the History of Art, School of Archaeology, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom

  • Jay T. Stock,

    Affiliation PAVE Research Group, Department of Archaeology & Anthropology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom

  • Jinger S. Gottschall

    Affiliation Neuromotion Laboratory, Department of Kinesiology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania, United States of America

Neandertal Humeri May Reflect Adaptation to Scraping Tasks, but Not Spear Thrusting

  • Colin N. Shaw, 
  • Cory L. Hofmann, 
  • Michael D. Petraglia, 
  • Jay T. Stock, 
  • Jinger S. Gottschall
PLOS
x

Abstract

Unique compared with recent and prehistoric Homo sapiens, Neandertal humeri are characterised by a pronounced right-dominant bilateral strength asymmetry and an anteroposteriorly strengthened diaphyseal shape. Remodeling in response to asymmetric forces imposed during regular underhanded spear thrusting is the most influential explanatory hypothesis. The core tenet of the “Spear Thrusting Hypothesis”, that underhand thrusting requires greater muscle activity on the right side of the body compared to the left, remains untested. It is unclear whether alternative subsistence behaviours, such as hide processing, might better explain this morphology. To test this, electromyography was used to measure muscle activity at the primary movers of the humerus (pectoralis major (PM), anterior (AD) and posterior deltoid (PD)) during three distinct spear-thrusting tasks and four separate scraping tasks. Contrary to predictions, maximum muscle activity (MAX) and total muscle activity (TOT) were significantly higher (all values, p<.05) at the left (non-dominant) AD, PD and PM compared to the right side of the body during spear thrusting tasks. Thus, the muscle activity required during underhanded spearing tasks does not lend itself to explaining the pronounced right dominant strength asymmetry found in Neandertal humeri. In contrast, during the performance of all three unimanual scraping tasks, right side MAX and TOT were significantly greater at the AD (all values, p<.01) and PM (all values, p<.02) compared to the left. The consistency of the results provides evidence that scraping activities, such as hide preparation, may be a key behaviour in determining the unusual pattern of Neandertal arm morphology. Overall, these results yield important insight into the Neandertal behavioural repertoire that aided survival throughout Pleistocene Eurasia.

Introduction

For Neandertals living in the variable, and at times cold, climate of the Late Pleistocene high levels of energy expenditure and meat consumption were essential to survival [1][4]. While isotopic evidence suggests a life requiring the hunting or scavenging of large game [3], it remains unclear which habitual activities the fossilized skeletal remains of Neandertals and associated archaeological materials reflect. Numerous skeletal traits differentiate the postcranial skeleton of European and Levantine Neandertals from modern [5][7] and also prehistoric Homo sapiens [8][16]. Two particular characteristics have received considerable attention; pronounced humeral diaphysis strength asymmetry and anteroposteriorly strengthened humeral diaphyseal shape [5], [17][20]. In particular, humeral bilateral asymmetry for cross-sectional area, and torsional and average bending rigidity, appear exceptionally high in Neandertals (averaging 24–57%) compared to skeletal samples of modern Holocene H. sapiens (averaging 5–14%) [5]. While Upper Paleolithic H. sapiens also display relatively high levels of humeral bilateral asymmetry [14], the only living human groups with similar morphological characteristics are tennis players and cricketers (averaging 28–57% asymmetry for the same measurements) who habitually and asymmetrically load their upper limbs [5], [21].

An influential hypothesis by Churchill and colleagues [17], which has been cited broadly [22][32], argues that regular close-range hunting with thrusting spears produced the Neandertals pronounced right-dominant humeral strength asymmetry and highly anteroposteriorly strengthened diaphyses. The authors suggested that during underhand spear thrusting the dominant limb (positioned towards the back) generates the majority of the force, while the non-dominant limb (positioned towards the front) serves to guide and stabilize the spear [17]. Considerable experimental evidence [33][35] supports the idea that long bone diaphyses respond to increased forces by structurally augmenting their mass in the principal planes of deformation [36], [37]. Thus, asymmetric loading associated with spear thrusting was proposed to cause bilaterally asymmetric adaptation in the humeri. To test this, Schmitt and colleagues [38] measured strain distribution on the shaft of an instrumented ‘spear’ during thrusting tasks, and demonstrated that the area of the spear nearest the trailing (dominant) hand experienced a greater portion of the force upon impact and while forcefully holding the spear against the rigid target.

Despite the evidence in support of the spear-thrusting hypothesis, other explanations for these unique humeral qualities remain viable. An unexplored suggestion made by Trinkaus and colleagues [5] is that Neandertal humeral morphology may reflect adaptation to functionally specific, and likely repetitive, tasks such as flint knapping or animal hide preparation. It is estimated that European Neandertals lived in cold conditions for many thousands of years [1], [39]. While complex physiological adaptations are probable [1], ‘clothing’ would have been necessary [39], [40]. The hides of large and small game animals would have allowed for the construction of ‘personal portable environments’ [41], providing some level of protection [42][44]. While the frequency of hide processing in Neandertal culture remains uncertain, one of the most ubiquitous tool types in Mousterian assemblages is the racloir, or side-scraper [45], [46], used to scrape sinew and hair from animal hides [47], [48]. Ethnographic evidence describes hide scraping among living groups as an arduous multi-phasic process requiring multiple hours per hide, spread over the period of a week [49][56]. Neandertals would have habitually knapped stone tools; nevertheless, for experienced individuals this task requires less time than hide scraping, and may impart less biomechanical strain [57], [58]. Although direct testing is required, a reasonable argument can be made that compared to hide scraping, knapping is less likely to elicit pronounced skeletal adaptations.

The spear-thrusting hypothesis is often cited as the paramount explanation for the unique humeral strength asymmetry and cross-sectional shape found in Neandertals. Arguably, further testing would help to assess whether this, or any other functional hypothesis, is the most appropriate explanation for the unique morphological characteristics of Neandertals. The forces generated through the activity of skeletal muscle (in combination with substrate reaction forces) define the functional environment believed to influence bone structure [59]. The present study takes an alternative approach to understanding how activity might have influenced bone structure by using electromyography (EMG) to quantify bi-lateral muscle activity at the chest (pectoralis major) and shoulder (anterior and posterior deltoid) during three distinct spear-thrusting tasks and four separate scraping tasks. This approach expands the behavioural model to include various subsistence-specific behaviours. We hypothesize that in comparison to scraping tasks, spear-thrusting tasks require greater right-side muscle activity than the left.

Results

Overall, muscle activity at the chest and shoulder reveals a dichotomous distinction between spear thrusting and single-arm scraping (Tables 1, 2, 3, Fig. 1). In contrast to predictions of bilateral muscle activity patterns required for underhand spear thrusting [17], [38], maximum muscle activity (MAX) and total muscle activity (TOT) was higher at the left (non-dominant) anterior deltoid (AD), posterior deltoid (PD) and pectoralis major (PM) compared to those same muscles on the right side of the body. Differences for MAX and TOT were significantly greater on the left side for the majority of these comparisons; this included PM during all three spearing activities (all values, p<.025), AD during strike hold (MAX p = .037 and TOT p = .0001), and PD during strike hold (MAX p = .049 and TOT p = .005).

thumbnail
Table 1. Bilateral anterior deltoid maximum (MAX) and total (TOT) muscle activity for all scraping and spearing tasks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040349.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Bilateral pectoralis major maximum (MAX) and total (TOT) muscle activity for all scraping and spearing tasks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040349.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. Bilateral posterior deltoid maximum (MAX) and total (TOT) muscle activity for all scraping and spearing tasks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040349.t003

thumbnail
Figure 1. Anterior Deltoid (A), Pectoralis Major (B) and Posterior Deltoid (C) bilateral muscle activity for spear thrusting and scraping tasks.

Total Muscle Activity (area under the curve) is presented as ‘area per unit time’, to ease interpretation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040349.g001

Earlier research had assumed that the underhand spear thrust involved similar mechanics to the stroking of a pool cue, and thus the dominant limb would generate the majority of the force while the non-dominant limb would serve primarily as a guide [17], [38]. Higher bending forces were predicted in the trailing arm as the proximally-inserted flexors (pectoralis major and anterior deltoid) pulled the proximal aspect of the humerus forward while the resistance offered by the object being speared exerted a posteriorly directed force on the distal humerus [38]. Spear thrusting involves a tensing of the body, a transfer of weight from the back to the front foot, slight medial rotation of the hips followed by the shoulders, and a simultaneous forward thrust of the arms. It appears that because the left hand is the closest point of contact upon impact and continued pushing against the target, the majority of the substrate reaction force is translated along the spear and is countered primarily by aspects of the musculoskeletal system on the left side of the body.

During the performance of all three unimanual scraping tasks, right side MAX and TOT was significantly greater at the AD (all values, p<.01) and PM (all values, p<.02) compared to the left (Tables 1, 2, 3, Fig. 1). In keeping with this trend, muscle activity was greater at the right PD compared to the left, but not significantly so. In contrast, performance of the fourth scraping activity, two-handed vertical pull-down, revealed the opposite trend: TOT at PM (p = .017) and MAX and TOT at PD (p = .035 and p = .012, respectively) were significantly greater on the left side compared to the right. Although the movements involved in the push task appear biomechanically opposite to the pull and hack tasks, shoulder and chest muscle activity was similarly asymmetric and right dominant in all cases. This suggests, as one might expect, that right-handed hide preparation engenders muscular activity that loads the humeri more unilaterally towards the right side. Contrary to this, although bilateral differences were found in some cases, during two handed vertical pull-down, muscle activity on the right and left sides were more comparable than during any other scraping or spearing task.

Discussion

The results demonstrate that during spear thrusting tasks, patterns of activity among muscles that originate at the shoulder and chest and insert onto the humerus are significantly greater on the left side of the body compared to the right. In general terms, muscular activity translates to forces imposed upon the diaphyses of long bones and can stimulate remodeling. The results suggest that if the regular performance of spear thrusting tasks caused an asymmetric increase in humeral rigidity, the left humerus would become more rigid than the right; the opposite pattern to what is found in the Neandertal fossil record. Overall, these results do not support a fundamental tenet of the spear-thrusting hypothesis; that underhand thrusting requires greater muscle activity on the right side of the body compared to the left [17], [38]. This finding contrasts with the conclusions of Schmitt et al. [38], which measured bending strain on the spear itself. It remains unclear which of these two methodological approaches (Schmitt et al. [38] vs. the present study) most accurately reflects spearing-induced osteogenesis in the arms. A study that replicates both methods is required to clarify any lack of concordance. While muscle activity per se is not an expression of force production, muscle activity is directly related to the production of force. Because the deltoid and pectoralis insert onto the humerus, measurements of muscle activity provide a description of the loads imposed upon the humerus. Patterns of muscle activity recorded during three separate unimanual scraping tasks revealed significantly greater right side (scraping, dominant hand) muscle activity compared to the left. This finding supports the idea that if scraping tasks were a regular construct of Neandertal life, as is the case in modern human groups that process hides [49], [52], these activities may be a viable alternative for explaining the pronounced bilateral humeral diaphysis strength asymmetry found in Neandertals.

These analyses raise a number of important questions that require further testing. First, do the patterns of asymmetric muscle activity measured during spearing and scraping tasks accurately reflect patterns of bone strain, and ultimately bone adaptation? Second, while it is plausible that the magnitude of bone strain resulting from intense spear thrusting could cause adaptation in humeral structure, is the same true for a less-intense, yet highly repetitive, activity such as scraping? For practical and ethical reasons, the first of these questions is difficult to directly test in humans (for a rare example see [60]). Nevertheless, while strain magnitude may be a primary influence [61] experimental work involving animal models suggests that the number of strain cycles (repetitions) also plays a critical role in the mechanism by which bone responds to mechanical strain [62][64]. Thus, if scraping tasks were adequately strenuous and performed regularly this activity may stimulate adaptive bone remodeling. Although we cannot be sure of the precise hide preparation techniques used by Neandertals, ethnographic accounts can provide useful models. Hide-processing tasks performed by the Inuit are described as arduous, highly technical, and time-consuming [49], with experienced individuals needing more than 8 hours spread over several days to make a dried skin suitable for clothing. Ethnographic reports of the Wolayta and Oromo of Ethiopia agree with this timeline and estimate that six and ten hours, respectively, are required to process a single cattle hide [55], [56]. Among the Tahltan of Northern British Columbia each family processes ∼30 new hides every 12 months, effectively engaging a single individual for over half the year [52]. Although hide processing methods vary globally and throughout time, wet-hide defleshing and multiple bouts of dry-hide scraping are always involved [49][54]. For example, for each hide, the Inuit perform three separate scraping tasks totaling more than six hours of strenuous labour [49]. Among the Aleut of Alaska and the Tchotchke of Siberia, most hide-processing tasks are performed by females [65], [66]. Humeral bilateral strength asymmetry appears much lower in Neandertal females compared to males [17], [67], and may reflect a sexual division of labour. However, due to the dearth of paired humeri attributable to female Neandertals, the attribution of differences in sex-specific bi-manual task specialization between Neandertals and H. sapiens remains speculative. Additionally, it has been suggested that anterior tooth wear in Neandertals may reflect the use of the mouth as a ‘third hand’ during hide scraping and food processing [68], [69], nevertheless, there is variable evidence as to how important this action may have been [70].

Stone tool types coupled with analyses of use-wear patterns can provide insight into the subsistence activities of prehistoric groups [71][73]. Scrapers are the most common artefact type found in Neandertal tool assemblages of Western Europe and the Near East. The most common scraper types within Mousterian toolkits are the simple, but distinctive, retouched tool forms called racloirs, or side-scrapers [45], [74]. Microscopic analysis of Mousterian scrapers support the view that use is mainly confined to retouched edges, although there is little support for an association between scraper types and particular functions [48], [75]. The function of scrapers has, in fact, been shown to be variable, and includes the scraping of plants, hide, antler or bone to scraping, cutting and slicing of wood and meat or skin [48], [76]. Hide-working is typically identified as a significant scraper function, recently exemplified by the analysis of scrapers from the Mousterian site of Payre (Ardeche, France) which show polish and striations from scraping, as well as skin and hair fragments [47].

Alternatively, one might hypothesize that Neandertals spear thrusted using a left-handed grip (right hand forward), which would generate greater right side muscle activity and therefore a greater load on the right humerus. All participants in the present study were right handed, were given no instructions regarding hand placement on the spear, and all performed the tasks using the same grip: left hand towards the front of the spear with the thumb facing forward, right hand positioned at the back (for corroboration see [38]). Thus, for Neandertals to employ a ‘left-handed’ grip on the spear they would almost certainly have been left-handed. Globally, ∼90% of all humans can be classified as right-handed [77], [78], a trend that appears consistent for at least ten thousand years [79], and perhaps as far back as 500K [68], [80] or even 2 million years [81], [82]. If the rate and distribution of handedness in Homo sapiens is indicative of handedness distribution in Neandertals it is quite unlikely that all Neandertals in the fossil record (where matching right and left humeri are available) were left-handed and employed a left-handed spearing grip.

Finally, the results from the present study do not directly inform the hypothesis that spear thrusting is responsible for the pronounced diaphyseal shape (anteroposterior strengthening) in both the right and left humeri of Neandertals [38]. Alternative hypotheses, however, do stem from the finding of somewhat comparable muscle activity in the right and left shoulder and chest during two-handed ‘pull down’ scraping tasks. Previous research has argued that the habitual performance of repetitive bimanual tasks, such as the use of digging sticks by Later Stone Age South African women, can stimulate pronounced bilateral A-P strengthening at the humeral diaphysis [83]. Overall, it is not inconceivable that 1) the pronounced humeral strength asymmetry characteristic of Neandertals resulted from repetitive unimanual scraping tasks, and 2) that the corresponding A-P strengthened shape of the Neandertal humeral diaphysis resulted from the habitual performance of a separate scraping task(s) that was symmetric, bimanual, and loaded the humerus in the A-P plane.

In conclusion, the consistency of the results provides evidence that scraping activities, such as hide preparation, may be a key behaviour in determining the unusual pattern of Neandertal upper limb morphology. The muscle activity required to perform various underhanded spearing tasks does not lend itself to explaining the pronounced right dominant strength asymmetry found in Neandertal humeri. Future studies of spear thrusting mechanics might evaluate the action at specific joints by comparing the timing of muscle activation at the shoulder flexors, elbow flexors, and rotator cuff using EMG and motion analysis. Although it still remains to be demonstrated conclusively, if both unimanual and bimanual hide scraping tasks were performed habitually, the muscle activity associated with these tasks may be a parsimonious explanation for the Neandertal’s unique humeral morphology. Overall, these results yield important insight into the Neandertal behavioural repertoire that aided survival throughout Pleistocene Eurasia.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Thirteen right-handed men (age: 28 ± 6.4 years, mass: 78.0 ± 10.4 kg, height: 180.8 ± 8.9 cm) with no known musculoskeletal disorders volunteered in this experiment. Participants were prescreened to exclude any predominantly left handed individuals; handedness was determined by a brief survey adapted from Oldfield [84]. The Institutional Review Board of The Pennsylvania State University approved this study’s protocol, and all participants provided informed written consent prior to participation. The participant pictured in Figures S1, S2, S4, S5 and S7 has given written informed consent, as outlined in the PLoS consent form, to publication of their photograph.

Electromyography

Two 2.22 cm square silver-silver chloride electrodes (Vermed Inc. Bellows Falls, VT) were placed on the right and left anterior deltoid (AD), posterior deltoid (PD), infraspinatus (IS), and pectoralis major (PM) muscles. A ground electrode was placed on the left acromion process. All sites were abraded and wiped with alcohol prior to electrode placement. Electrode placement (Fig. S1) was determined based on anatomical landmarks and verified through a series of functional tests [85]. All data were collected at 1000 Hz with an amplifier hardware system (Bortec Octopus, Calgary, Alberta, Canada) using Evart software (Version 3.21, Motion Analysis Corp., Santa Rosa, CA). EMG provides a measure of the neural excitation of a muscle, which is correlated with the production of muscle force. This study reports measures of both maximum muscle activity (MAX), a measure of the maximum integrated muscle activity (where activity is the electrical potential across the region of the muscle), and total muscle activity (TOT), a measurement of the area under the curve of the linear envelope.

Experimental Protocol

Participants performed three different spear thrusting tasks and four different scraping tasks. The order of all tasks was randomized, and each activity was performed to the beat of a 120 Hz metronome to standardize timing across participants. Every fourth beat (i.e., every two seconds) produced a unique tone allowing participants to synchronize their movements in multiples of 0.5 seconds or 2.0 seconds, as per the desired protocol. Participants performed several practice trials of each task to ensure their comfort with the frequency and effort necessary for each task (further detail below). Three 12-second trials of each task were recorded. All participants first performed a baseline task for the purpose of normalizing the EMG data. This baseline task required the participants to stand in an anatomically neutral position holding a 2.25 kg plate in each hand. Participants were instructed to then flex both the shoulder and elbow in the sagittal plane to 90°. The participants then began a sequence of externally and internally rotating the shoulder 90° in 2 second intervals, for a total of 18-seconds. Elbow flexion was maintained at 90 degrees of flexion throughout.

Spearing Tasks

Spearing protocols were adapted from Schmitt and colleagues [38]. Each spearing task was performed with a wooden dowel (length  = 214 cm, diameter  = 3.5 cm) with a rounded end. Participants were instructed to perform each spearing tasks using an underhanded grip, at a self-selected distance from the target. The ‘target’ was composed of five 61×61 cm pieces of carpet (thickness  = 3.5 cm), placed in front of a fitness mat (thickness  = 60 cm). The target was located approximately 100 cm above the ground (Fig. S2). Although not explicitly instructed, all participants chose to operate the spear leading with the non-dominant limb (see Discussion). Additionally, while thrusting the spear, participants were instructed to exert maximal effort, striking the mat with as much force as possible. Participants were required to keep their feet stationary on the ground, but no further limitations were imposed upon the participants body position.

Three different spearing tasks were performed:

Single strike.

A single thrust, followed by a rapid withdrawal, contacting the target in 2-second intervals.

Repeated strike.

Three consecutive rapid strikes performed at an interval of 0.5 seconds, followed by a 2.5 second rest period.

Strike hold.

Single strike with continued isometric drive of spear into the target for 2 seconds, followed by a 2 second withdrawal resting period.

Scraping Tasks

All scraping tasks were performed on a 61 cm×61 cm piece of carpet, with the fibers facing out. Carpet was used as a substitute for large ungulate hide; the regularity of the carpet’s material properties ensured that the scraping surface was consistent for each participant. It was explained to the participants that the softer carpet fibers represented material that needed to be removed (e.g., sinew, adipose tissue) during the scraping process. Participants were instructed to remove carpet fibers within a relatively narrow strip (5 cm×30 cm) during each 12-second trial, and to select an angle and amount pressure so that the under layer of the carpet was not damaged. Thus, the intensity (downward pressure applied) was relatively consistent amongst the participants. The four scraping tasks performed were chosen to simulate specific movements used by modern hide processing groups using non-mechanised tools. Arguably, these tasks are variations of tasks also performed in prehistory. These include:

Hack.

The hack was performed with the dominant hand while grasping a chisel (FatMax 1” width, Stanley Black & Decker Inc., New Britain, CT) wrapped in athletic tape (Fig. S3). Participants stood in an athletic stance facing the target, and maintained their balance with the non-dominant arm. The target carpet was positioned near shoulder height, and was fixed to a wooden ramp, which was angled away from the participant at 15°. The participant then performed a repeated downward striking motion in the sagittal plane contacting the target every 0.5 seconds (Fig. S4).

Vertical pull down.

This task was performed with both hands, while standing. A mini-crowbar (Wonder Bar, Stanley Black & Decker Inc., New Britain, CT) was used to simulate an end-scraper (Fig. S3). Participants were instructed to hold the bottom of the simulated scraper with the dominant hand and to apply additional pressure as needed to remove the carpet fibers with the non-dominant hand placed on top of the tool (Fig. S5). The range of motion began at the participants shoulder and ended at the mid-abdomen. The start of the task began with the tool placed on the carpeted target near the level of the participants shoulder; this was followed by a downward scrape in 2-second intervals.

Push and Pull: These tasks were performed with the participant in a kneeling bent-over position, using a replica Mousterian side-scraper (Fig. S6). Depending on which task was being performed, the participant began by either scraping away from the body (push) or scraping toward the body (pull). Each scraping action involved moving the tool in the sagittal plane, and was performed throughout a 2 second interval. For these tasks, the participant was instructed to support their body with the non-dominant limb by placing it on the scraping surface (Fig. S7).

Data Analysis

All EMG data were first normalized to the mean of the baseline task. The normalized data were high-pass filtered (20 Hz), full-wave rectified, and the resulting linear envelope was smoothed with a low pass filter (2 Hz) using MATLAB (Version R2009a, The Mathworks Inc, Natick, MA).

EMG Signal Identification and Isolation

In order to isolate the EMG data during the period of action for each repetition of each task, activity burst time windows were defined in a unique manner for each task. For all spearing trials, the processed EMG data for the right infraspinatus was utilized for defining activity burst time windows in the following ways:

Single strike.

The peak value for each local maximum was determined. Following this, the times at which 20% of this maximum occurred (one before and one after the peak) were found. A range of 0.5 seconds before and after each of these values was considered the outer limit of the activity burst window (Fig. S8, top).

Repeated Strike

A primary window surrounding the three characteristic muscle activity peaks was considered to start and end 0.1 seconds before and after the first and third peaks, respectively. This primary window was divided by three, and these were considered the time window for each of the three individual bouts (Fig. S8, middle).

Strike hold.

The peak value for the period of activity was determined. Following this, the times at which 40% of the maximum (before and after the peak) were found. The window of activity was considered to start 0.225 seconds before the first instance and end 0.225 seconds after the second instance of 40% of the maximum. These values were all self-selected in an attempt to capture all muscle activity, and minimize the presence of periods of rest (i.e., little to no muscle activity), within the activity burst time window. Once these time windows were determined for the right infraspinatus, they were applied across all eight muscles and were visually inspected to ensure that all relevant data had been isolated (Fig. S8, bottom).

Scraping tasks were processed in a different fashion, as generally there were no significant periods of rest between bouts of activity. A peak detection algorithm was used to find all local maxima and minima for each trial of activity (Fig. S9). For each scraping task, an appropriate muscle was selected, based on visual inspection, for peak detection (hack – Right anterior deltoid; vertical pull down – Left posterior deltoid; push & pull – Right posterior deltoid). Activity burst time windows were defined as the time from one local minimum to the adjacent minimum. Again, these windows were applied to all eight muscles and visually inspected for accuracy. If a time window did not fit a pre-determined range, as decided based on metronome timing, these data were removed from future analyses; e.g., if a hack activity burst time window was found to be 1.1 seconds, this was removed, as it is likely that a minimum was missed by the peak detector (as per the hack protocol, activity time bursts should be close to 0.5 seconds). This type of erroneous data could also potentially be due to incorrect participant timing during trials. This exclusion ensured that homologous data windows were compared.

Once the activity bursts were defined for each muscle and condition, the peak (maximum muscle activity; MAX) and area under the curve (total muscle activity; TOT) was calculated. MAX and TOT were used for intra-participant comparisons of muscle activity (i.e., left versus right). Since all EMG traces were normalized to the baseline task, values of MAX are unitless (reported as mV/mV). As a result, units for TOT are reported in seconds (mV*s/mV). Since EMG was normalized to a standard baseline task, opposed to maximum voluntary contraction, the magnitudes of MAX and TOT represent the ratio of the muscles activity of the task to the baseline. MAX and TOT are simply used for comparison between right and left values of the same muscle during the same activity. Averages for both MAX and TOT variables were computed from all activity bursts over the three total trials for each task.

Statistical Analyses

Student’s paired t-tests were performed to compare right vs. left maximum muscle area (MAX) and total muscle area under the curve (TOT). All statistical comparisons were performed using PASW (Version 18.0.0, IBM Corp., Armonk, NY). Tests were considered statistically significant at p ≤ 0.05.

Supporting Information

Figure S1.

Electrode placement. Left to right: left posterior deltoid, left infraspinatus, right infraspinatus, right posterior deltoid, right anterior deltoid, right pectoralis major, left pectoralis major, left anterior deltoid.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040349.s001

(TIF)

Figure S2.

Experimental set up for spearing tasks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040349.s002

(TIF)

Figure S3.

Chisel (top) and mini-crowbar utilized in the hack and vertical pull down scraping tasks, respectively.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040349.s003

(TIF)

Figure S4.

Experimental setup for the hack task.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040349.s004

(TIF)

Figure S5.

Experimental setup for the vertical pull down scraping task.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040349.s005

(TIF)

Figure S6.

Replica side scrapers used for the push and pull scraping tasks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040349.s006

(TIF)

Figure S7.

Experimental setup for the push and pull scraping tasks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040349.s007

(TIF)

Figure S8.

Activity window definition; spearing tasks. Processed EMG patterns shown in black, with the final activity time windows shown in green.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040349.s008

(TIF)

Figure S9.

Activity window definition; scraping tasks. Processed EMG patterns in black. Blue lines indicate local minima, determined from peak detection code, whereas green lines illustrate the activity bursts.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040349.s009

(TIF)

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Tim Ryan, Neil Sharkey, John Challis and Nori Okita, Sylvie Beyries, Jill Oakes and Sarah Beam (Bata Shoe Museum) for their support and helpful suggestions during the course of this project, to Stefano Benazzi, Brigitte Holt, Asier Gomez-Olivencia, Steven Churchill and Eric Trinkaus for commenting on versions of this manuscript, to all participants, without whom this research would have remained theoretical, and to Bruce Bradley and John Lord for very generously knapping and donating the replica Mousterian side-scrapers used in this study. CNS wishes to also acknowledge the benevolence of the Cushing Anthropological Research Fund for its ongoing financial support.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: CS CH MP JS JG. Performed the experiments: CS CH JG. Analyzed the data: CS CH JG. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: CS CH JG. Wrote the paper: CS CH MP JS JG.

References

  1. 1. Steegman TA, Cerny FJ, Holliday TW (2002) Neandertal Cold Adaptation: Physiological and Energetic Factors. American Journal of Human Biology 14: 566–583.TA SteegmanFJ CernyTW Holliday2002Neandertal Cold Adaptation: Physiological and Energetic Factors.American Journal of Human Biology14566583
  2. 2. Sorensen MV, Leonard WR (2001) Neandertal energetics and foraging efficiency. Journal of Human Evolution 40: 483–495.MV SorensenWR Leonard2001Neandertal energetics and foraging efficiency.Journal of Human Evolution40483495
  3. 3. Richards M, Pettitt P, Trinkaus E, Smith F, Paunovic M, et al. (2000) Neandertal diet at Vindija and Neandertal predation: The evidence from stable istopes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 97: 7663–7666.M. RichardsP. PettittE. TrinkausF. SmithM. Paunovic2000Neandertal diet at Vindija and Neandertal predation: The evidence from stable istopes.Proceedings of the National Academy of Science9776637666
  4. 4. Snodgrass J, Leonard W (2009) Neandertal energetics revisited: insights into population dynamics and life history evolution. PaleoAnthropology. pp. 220–237.J. SnodgrassW. Leonard2009Neandertal energetics revisited: insights into population dynamics and life history evolution.PaleoAnthropology220237
  5. 5. Trinkaus E, Churchill SE, Ruff CB (1994) Postcranial Robusticity in Homo. II: Humeral Bilateral Asymmetry and Bone Plasticity. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 93: 1–34.E. TrinkausSE ChurchillCB Ruff1994Postcranial Robusticity in Homo. II: Humeral Bilateral Asymmetry and Bone Plasticity.American Journal of Physical Anthropology93134
  6. 6. Trinkaus E, Churchill SE, Villemeur I, Riley KG, Heller JA, et al. (1991) Robusticity versus Shape: The Functional Interpretation of Neandertal Appendicular Morphology. Journal of the Anthropological Society of Nippon 99: 257–278.E. TrinkausSE ChurchillI. VillemeurKG RileyJA Heller1991Robusticity versus Shape: The Functional Interpretation of Neandertal Appendicular Morphology.Journal of the Anthropological Society of Nippon99257278
  7. 7. Churchill S, Trinkaus E (1990) Neandertal scapular glenoid morphology. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 83: 147–160.S. ChurchillE. Trinkaus1990Neandertal scapular glenoid morphology.American Journal of Physical Anthropology83147160
  8. 8. Trinkaus E (1997) Appendicular robusticity and the paleobiology of modern human emergence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 94: 13367–13373.E. Trinkaus1997Appendicular robusticity and the paleobiology of modern human emergence.Proceedings of the National Academy of Science941336713373
  9. 9. Trinkaus E, Churchill SE, Ruff C, Vandermeersch B (1999) Long bone shaft robusticity and body proportions of the Saint-Cesaire 1 Chatelperronian Neanderthal. Journal of Archaeological Science 26: 753–773.E. TrinkausSE ChurchillC. RuffB. Vandermeersch1999Long bone shaft robusticity and body proportions of the Saint-Cesaire 1 Chatelperronian Neanderthal.Journal of Archaeological Science26753773
  10. 10. Trinkaus E, Ruff C, Churchill SE (1998) Upper limb versus lower limb loading patterns among Near Eastern Middle Paleolithic hominids. In: Akazawa T, Aoki K, Bar-Yosef O, editors. pp. 392–404.E. TrinkausC. RuffSE Churchill1998Upper limb versus lower limb loading patterns among Near Eastern Middle Paleolithic hominids.T. AkazawaK. AokiO. Bar-Yosef392404editors.Neandertals and modern humans in West Asia.New York: Plenum Press. New York: Plenum Press.
  11. 11. Churchill SE (1994) Human Upper Body Evolution in the Eurasian Later Pleistocene. PhD Dissertation: University of New Mexico. SE Churchill1994Human Upper Body Evolution in the Eurasian Later Pleistocene.PhD Dissertation: University of New Mexico
  12. 12. Trinkaus E, Churchill SE (1999) Diaphyseal cross-sectional geometry of near eastern Middle Palaeolithic humans: the humerus. Journal of Archaeological Sciences 26: 173–184.E. TrinkausSE Churchill1999Diaphyseal cross-sectional geometry of near eastern Middle Palaeolithic humans: the humerus.Journal of Archaeological Sciences26173184
  13. 13. Trinkaus E (2005) The upper limb remains. In: Trinkaus E, Svoboda J, editors. pp. 327–372.E. Trinkaus2005The upper limb remains.E. TrinkausJ. Svoboda327372editors.Early Modern Human Evolution in Central Europe: The people of Dolni Vnullstonice and Pavlov.New York: Oxford University Press. New York: Oxford University Press.
  14. 14. Trinkaus E (2010) The Tianyuan 1 upper limb remains. In: Shang H, Trinkaus E, editors. pp. 96–131.E. Trinkaus2010The Tianyuan 1 upper limb remains.H. ShangE. Trinkaus96131editors.The early modern human from Tianyuan Cave, China.College Station: Texas A&M University Anthropology Series. College Station: Texas A&M University Anthropology Series.
  15. 15. Trinkaus E (2008) Behavioral implications of the Muierii 1 early modern human scapula. Annuaire Roumain d’Anthropologie 45: 27–41.E. Trinkaus2008Behavioral implications of the Muierii 1 early modern human scapula.Annuaire Roumain d’Anthropologie452741
  16. 16. Trinkaus E (2008) Kiik-Koba 2 and Neandertal axillary border ontogeny. Anthropological Science 116: 231–236.E. Trinkaus2008Kiik-Koba 2 and Neandertal axillary border ontogeny.Anthropological Science116231236
  17. 17. Churchill S, Weaver AH, Niewoehner WA (1996) Late Pleistocene human technological and subsistence behavior: functional interpretations of upper limb morphology. Quaternaria Nova 6: 413–447.S. ChurchillAH WeaverWA Niewoehner1996Late Pleistocene human technological and subsistence behavior: functional interpretations of upper limb morphology.Quaternaria Nova6413447
  18. 18. Churchill SE (2002) Of Assegais and Bayonets: reconstructing prehistoric spear use. Evolutionary Anthropology 11: 185–186.SE Churchill2002Of Assegais and Bayonets: reconstructing prehistoric spear use.Evolutionary Anthropology11185186
  19. 19. Rhodes J, Churchill SE (2009) Throwing in the Middle and Upper Paleolithic: inferences from an analysis of humeral retroversion. Journal of Human Evolution 56: 1–10.J. RhodesSE Churchill2009Throwing in the Middle and Upper Paleolithic: inferences from an analysis of humeral retroversion.Journal of Human Evolution56110
  20. 20. Churchill S, Rhodes J (2009) The evolution of the human capacity of "killing at a distance": the human fossil evidence for the evolution of projectile weaponry In: Hublin J, Richards M, editors. The Evolution of Hominin Diets: Integrating Approaches to the Study of Palaeolithic Subsistence: Springer. pp. 201–210.S. ChurchillJ. Rhodes2009The evolution of the human capacity of "killing at a distance": the human fossil evidence for the evolution of projectile weaponry In: Hublin J, Richards M, editors.The Evolution of Hominin Diets: Integrating Approaches to the Study of Palaeolithic Subsistence: Springer201210
  21. 21. Shaw C, Stock J (2009) Habitual throwing and swimming correspond with upper limb diaphyseal strength and shape in modern human athletes. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 140: 160–172.C. ShawJ. Stock2009Habitual throwing and swimming correspond with upper limb diaphyseal strength and shape in modern human athletes.American Journal of Physical Anthropology140160172
  22. 22. Kennedy K (2004) Slings and arrows of predaceous fortune: asian evidence of prehistoric spear use. Evolutionary Anthropology 13: 127–131.K. Kennedy2004Slings and arrows of predaceous fortune: asian evidence of prehistoric spear use.Evolutionary Anthropology13127131
  23. 23. Auerbach B, Ruff C (2006) Limb bone bilateral asymmetry: variability and commonality among modern humans. Journal of Human Evolution 50: 203–218.B. AuerbachC. Ruff2006Limb bone bilateral asymmetry: variability and commonality among modern humans.Journal of Human Evolution50203218
  24. 24. Ruff CB (2005) Mechanical determinants of bone form: Insights from skeletal remains. Journal of Musculoskeletal Neuronal Interaction 5: 202–212.CB Ruff2005Mechanical determinants of bone form: Insights from skeletal remains.Journal of Musculoskeletal Neuronal Interaction5202212
  25. 25. Rhodes J, Knusel C (2005) Activity-related skeletal change in Medieval Humeri: Cross-sectional and architectural alterations. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 128: 536–546.J. RhodesC. Knusel2005Activity-related skeletal change in Medieval Humeri: Cross-sectional and architectural alterations.American Journal of Physical Anthropology128536546
  26. 26. Weaver AH (2009) The meaning of Neandertal skeletal morphology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106: 16028–16033.AH Weaver2009The meaning of Neandertal skeletal morphology.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences1061602816033
  27. 27. Uomini N (2009) The prehistory of handedness: Archaeological data and comparative ethology. Journal of Human Evolution 57: 411–419.N. Uomini2009The prehistory of handedness: Archaeological data and comparative ethology.Journal of Human Evolution57411419
  28. 28. Shipman P (2008) Separating ‘us’ from ‘them’: Neanderthal and modern human behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105: 14241–14242.P. Shipman2008Separating ‘us’ from ‘them’: Neanderthal and modern human behavior.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences1051424114242
  29. 29. D’Errico F (2003) The invisible frontier. A multiple species model for the origin of behavioural modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology 12: 188–202.F. D’Errico2003The invisible frontier. A multiple species model for the origin of behavioural modernity.Evolutionary Anthropology12188202
  30. 30. Gurven M, Hill K (2009) Why do men hunt? A reevaluation of "Man the Hunter" and the sexual dividion of labor. Current Anthropology 50: 51–74.M. GurvenK. Hill2009Why do men hunt? A reevaluation of "Man the Hunter" and the sexual dividion of labor.Current Anthropology505174
  31. 31. Rots V (2009) The functional analysis of the Mousterian and Micoquian assemblages of Sesselfelsgrotte, Germany: aspects of tool use and hafting in the European Late Middle Palaeolithic. Quatär 56: 37–66.V. Rots2009The functional analysis of the Mousterian and Micoquian assemblages of Sesselfelsgrotte, Germany: aspects of tool use and hafting in the European Late Middle Palaeolithic.Quatär563766
  32. 32. Waguespack N, Surovell T, Denoyer A, Dallow A, Savage A, et al. (2009) Making a point: wood- versus stone-tipped projectiles. Antiquity 83: 768–800.N. WaguespackT. SurovellA. DenoyerA. DallowA. Savage2009Making a point: wood- versus stone-tipped projectiles.Antiquity83768800
  33. 33. Currey J (1984) The Mechanical Adaptation of Bones. Princeton: Princeton University Press. J. Currey1984The Mechanical Adaptation of Bones.Princeton: Princeton University Press
  34. 34. Lanyon LE, Rubin C (1984) Static vs. dynamic loads as an influence on bone remodeling. Journal of Biomechanics 17: 897–905.LE LanyonC. Rubin1984Static vs. dynamic loads as an influence on bone remodeling.Journal of Biomechanics17897905
  35. 35. Martin RB, Burr DR (1989) Structure, function, and adaptation of compact bone. New York: Raven Press. RB MartinDR Burr1989Structure, function, and adaptation of compact bone.New York: Raven Press
  36. 36. Rubin C, McLeod K, Basin S (1990) Functional strains and cortical bone adaptation: epigenetic assurance of skeletal integrity. Journal of Biomechanics 23: 43–54.C. RubinK. McLeodS. Basin1990Functional strains and cortical bone adaptation: epigenetic assurance of skeletal integrity.Journal of Biomechanics234354
  37. 37. Lanyon LE (1992) Control of bone architecture by functional load bearing. Journal of Bone and Mineral Research 7: S369–S375.LE Lanyon1992Control of bone architecture by functional load bearing.Journal of Bone and Mineral Research7S369S375
  38. 38. Schmitt D, Churchill SE, Hylander WL (2003) Experimental evidence concerning spear use in neandertals and early modern humans. Journal of Archaeological Sciences 30: 103–114.D. SchmittSE ChurchillWL Hylander2003Experimental evidence concerning spear use in neandertals and early modern humans.Journal of Archaeological Sciences30103114
  39. 39. Aiello L, Wheeler P (2003) Neandertal thermoregulation and the glacial climate. In: van Andel T, Davies W, editors. pp. 147–166.L. AielloP. Wheeler2003Neandertal thermoregulation and the glacial climate.T. van AndelW. Davies147166editors.Neandertals and modern humans in the European landscape during the Last Glaciation: archaeological results of the Stage 3 Project.Cambridge: McDonald Institute. Cambridge: McDonald Institute.
  40. 40. White M (2006) Things to do in Doggerland when you’re dead: surviving OIS3 at the northwestern-most fringe of Middle Palaeolithic Europe. World Archaeology 38: 547–575.M. White2006Things to do in Doggerland when you’re dead: surviving OIS3 at the northwestern-most fringe of Middle Palaeolithic Europe.World Archaeology38547575
  41. 41. Watkins S (1984) Clothing: The portable environment. Ames, IA: Iowa State University Press. S. Watkins1984Clothing: The portable environment.Ames, IA: Iowa State University Press
  42. 42. Cena K (1978) Thermal insulation of animal coats and human clothing. Physics in Medicine and Biology 23: 565–591.K. Cena1978Thermal insulation of animal coats and human clothing.Physics in Medicine and Biology23565591
  43. 43. Hammel H (1955) Thermal properties of fur. American Journal of Physiology 182: 369–376.H. Hammel1955Thermal properties of fur.American Journal of Physiology182369376
  44. 44. Moote I (1955) The thermal insulation of caribou pelts. Textile Research Journal 25: 832–837.I. Moote1955The thermal insulation of caribou pelts.Textile Research Journal25832837
  45. 45. Bordes F (1961) Typologie du Paléolithique Ancien et Moyen. Bordeaux: Delmas. F. Bordes1961Typologie du Paléolithique Ancien et Moyen.Bordeaux: Delmas
  46. 46. Rios-Garaizar J (2010) Organizacion economica de las sociedades Neandertales: El caso del Nivel VII de Amalda (Zestoa, Gipuzkoa). Zephyrus Junio. pp. 15–37.J. Rios-Garaizar2010Organizacion economica de las sociedades Neandertales: El caso del Nivel VII de Amalda (Zestoa, Gipuzkoa).Zephyrus Junio1537
  47. 47. Hardy B, Moncel M (2011) Neanderthal use of fish, mammals, birds, starchy plants and wood 125–250,000 years ago. Public Library of Science One 6: e23768.B. HardyM. Moncel2011Neanderthal use of fish, mammals, birds, starchy plants and wood 125–250,000 years ago.Public Library of Science One6e23768
  48. 48. Beyries S (1987) Variabilité de l’Industrie au Moustérian: approache fonctionelle sur quelques gisements Francais. British Archaeological Reports, International Series S328. S. Beyries1987Variabilité de l’Industrie au Moustérian: approache fonctionelle sur quelques gisements Francais.British Archaeological Reports, International Series S328
  49. 49. Oakes J (1991) Chapter 4: Skin Preparation Techniques. Copper & Cariboo Inuit skin clothing production. pp. 102–113.J. Oakes1991Chapter 4: Skin Preparation Techniques. Copper & Cariboo Inuit skin clothing production.102113 Photocopied version ed.Hull, Quebec: Canadian Museum of Civilizations. Hull, Quebec: Canadian Museum of Civilizations.
  50. 50. Hayden B (1990) The right rub: hideworking in high ranking households. In: Graslund B, editor. pp. 89–102.B. Hayden1990The right rub: hideworking in high ranking households.B. Graslund89102editor.the interpretative possibilities of microwear studies.Uppsala: Societas-Archaeologica Upsaliensis. Uppsala: Societas-Archaeologica Upsaliensis.
  51. 51. Takase K (2004) Hide processing of Even and Koryak: an ethnoarchaeological survey in Kamchatka Peninsula, Russia. Material Culture 77: 57–84.K. Takase2004Hide processing of Even and Koryak: an ethnoarchaeological survey in Kamchatka Peninsula, Russia.Material Culture775784
  52. 52. Albright A (1984) Tahltan ethnoarchaeology. Vancouver: Department of Archaeology, Simon Fraser University. A. Albright1984Tahltan ethnoarchaeology.Vancouver: Department of Archaeology, Simon Fraser University
  53. 53. Ewers J (1930) Blackfeet crafts. Washington, DC: Department of the Intertior Bureau of Indian Affairs. J. Ewers1930Blackfeet crafts.Washington, DC: Department of the Intertior Bureau of Indian Affairs
  54. 54. Turner L (1894) Ethnology of the Ungava District: Hudson Bay Territory. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institute. L. Turner1894Ethnology of the Ungava District: Hudson Bay Territory.Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institute
  55. 55. Clark J, Kurashina H (1981) A study of the work of a modern tanner in Ethiopia and its relavance for archaeological interpretation. In: Gould R, Schiffer M, editors. pp. 303–343.J. ClarkH. Kurashina1981A study of the work of a modern tanner in Ethiopia and its relavance for archaeological interpretation.R. GouldM. Schiffer303343editors.Modern material Culture: the archaeology of us.New York: Academic Press. New York: Academic Press.
  56. 56. Haaland R (1987) Socio-economic differentiation in the Neolithic Sudan. Oxford: Archaeopress. R. Haaland1987Socio-economic differentiation in the Neolithic Sudan.Oxford: Archaeopress
  57. 57. Williams E, Gordon A, Richmond B (2010) Upper limb kinematics and the role of the wrist during stone tool production. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 143: 134–145.E. WilliamsA. GordonB. Richmond2010Upper limb kinematics and the role of the wrist during stone tool production.American Journal of Physical Anthropology143134145
  58. 58. Williams E, Richmond B (2012) Manual pressure distribution during stone tool use. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 147: S303.E. WilliamsB. Richmond2012Manual pressure distribution during stone tool use.American Journal of Physical Anthropology147S303
  59. 59. Judex S, Carlson K (2009) Is bone’s response to mechanical signals dominated by gravitational loading? Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. S. JudexK. Carlson2009Is bone’s response to mechanical signals dominated by gravitational loading?Medicine & Science in Sports & ExerciseDOI:10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181a8c6e5:2037–2043. DOI:10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181a8c6e5:2037–2043.
  60. 60. Lanyon L, Hampson W, Goodship A, Shah J (1975) Bone deformation recorded in vivo from strain gauges attached to the human tibial shaft. Acta Orthop Scand 46: 256–268.L. LanyonW. HampsonA. GoodshipJ. Shah1975Bone deformation recorded in vivo from strain gauges attached to the human tibial shaft.Acta Orthop Scand46256268
  61. 61. Frost H (2003) Bone’s Mechanostat: a 2003 update. The Anatomical Record 275A: 1081–1101.H. Frost2003Bone’s Mechanostat: a 2003 update.The Anatomical Record275A10811101
  62. 62. Rubin C, McLeod K (1994) Promotion of bony ingrowth by frequency-specific, low-amplitude mechanical strain. Clinical Orthopaedics and Related Research 298: 165–174.C. RubinK. McLeod1994Promotion of bony ingrowth by frequency-specific, low-amplitude mechanical strain.Clinical Orthopaedics and Related Research298165174
  63. 63. Qin Y, Rubin C, McLeod K (1998) Nonlinear dependence of loading intensity and cycle number in the maintenance of bone mass and morphology. Journal of Orthopaedic Research 16: 482–489.Y. QinC. RubinK. McLeod1998Nonlinear dependence of loading intensity and cycle number in the maintenance of bone mass and morphology.Journal of Orthopaedic Research16482489
  64. 64. Fritton S, McLeod K, Rubin C (2000) Quantifying the strain history of bone: spatial uniformity and self similarity of low magnitude strains. Journal of Biomechanics 33: 317–325.S. FrittonK. McLeodC. Rubin2000Quantifying the strain history of bone: spatial uniformity and self similarity of low magnitude strains.Journal of Biomechanics33317325
  65. 65. Liapunova R (1975) Essays on the ethnography of the Aleuts. Fairbanks: University of Alaska Press. R. Liapunova1975Essays on the ethnography of the Aleuts.Fairbanks: University of Alaska Press
  66. 66. Beyries S, Karlin C, Tchesnokov Y (2000) Living in the skin of my reindeer: traditional leathercraft in Siberia (Documentary). France: The French Institute for Polar Research and Technology. S. BeyriesC. KarlinY. Tchesnokov2000Living in the skin of my reindeer: traditional leathercraft in Siberia (Documentary).France: The French Institute for Polar Research and Technology
  67. 67. Ben-Itzhak S, Smith P, Bloom R (1988) Radiographic study of the humerus in Neandertals and Homo sapiens sapiens. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 77: 231–242.S. Ben-ItzhakP. SmithR. Bloom1988Radiographic study of the humerus in Neandertals and Homo sapiens sapiens.American Journal of Physical Anthropology77231242
  68. 68. Frayer D, Lozano M, Bermudez de Castro J, Carbonell E, Arsuaga J, et al. (2012) More than 500,000 years of right-handedness in Europe. Laterality 17. D. FrayerM. LozanoJ. Bermudez de CastroE. CarbonellJ. Arsuaga2012More than 500,000 years of right-handedness in Europe.Laterality 17
  69. 69. Wolpoff M (1999) Paleoanthropology. University of Michigan: Mc Graw-Hill. M. Wolpoff1999Paleoanthropology.University of Michigan: Mc Graw-Hill
  70. 70. Clement A, Hillson S, Aiello L (2012) Tooth wear, neanderthal facial morphology and the anterior dental loading hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution 62: 367–376.A. ClementS. HillsonL. Aiello2012Tooth wear, neanderthal facial morphology and the anterior dental loading hypothesis.Journal of Human Evolution62367376
  71. 71. Semenov S (1964) Prehistoric technology. London: Cory, Adams and Mackay. S. Semenov1964Prehistoric technology.London: Cory, Adams and Mackay
  72. 72. Keeley L (1980) Experimental determination of stone tool uses: a microwear analysis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. L. Keeley1980Experimental determination of stone tool uses: a microwear analysis.Chicago: University of Chicago Press
  73. 73. Shea J (1992) Lithic microwear analysis in archeology. Evolutionary Anthropology 1: 143–150.J. Shea1992Lithic microwear analysis in archeology.Evolutionary Anthropology1143150
  74. 74. Mellars P (1996) The Neandertal Legacy. Princeton: Princeton University Press. P. Mellars1996The Neandertal Legacy.Princeton: Princeton University Press
  75. 75. Anderson-Gerfaud P (1990) Aspects of behaviour in the Middle Palaeolithic: functional analysis of stone tools from southwest France. In: Mellars P, editor. pp. 389–418.P. Anderson-Gerfaud1990Aspects of behaviour in the Middle Palaeolithic: functional analysis of stone tools from southwest France.P. Mellars389418editor.The emergence of modern humans.Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
  76. 76. Hardy B (2004) Neanderthal behaviour and stone tool function at the Middle Palaeolithic site of La Quina, France. Antiquity 78: 547–565.B. Hardy2004Neanderthal behaviour and stone tool function at the Middle Palaeolithic site of La Quina, France.Antiquity78547565
  77. 77. Faurie C, Schiefenhövel W, Le Bomin S, Billiard S, Raymond M (2005) Variation in the frequency of left-handers in traditional societies. Current Anthropology 46: 142–147.C. FaurieSchiefenhövel W, Le Bomin S, Billiard S, Raymond M2005Variation in the frequency of left-handers in traditional societies.Current Anthropology46142147
  78. 78. Raymond M, Pontier D (2004) Is there geographical variation in human handedness? Laterality 9: 35–51.M. RaymondD. Pontier2004Is there geographical variation in human handedness?Laterality93551
  79. 79. Faurie C, Raymond M (2003) Handedness frequency over more than ten thousand years. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 271: S43–S45.C. FaurieM. Raymond2003Handedness frequency over more than ten thousand years.Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences271S43S45
  80. 80. Lozano M, Mosquera M, Bermudez de Castro J, Arsuaga J, Carbonell E (2009) Right handedness of Homo heidelbergensis from Sima de los Huesos (Atapuerca, Spain) 500,000 years ago. Evolution and Human Behavior 30: 369–376.M. LozanoM. MosqueraJ. Bermudez de CastroJ. ArsuagaE. Carbonell2009Right handedness of Homo heidelbergensis from Sima de los Huesos (Atapuerca, Spain) 500,000 years ago.Evolution and Human Behavior30369376
  81. 81. Walker A, Leakey R (1993) The Nariokotome Homo erectus Skeleton. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. A. WalkerR. Leakey1993The Nariokotome Homo erectus Skeleton.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
  82. 82. Toth N (1985) Archaeological evidence for preferential right-handedness in the Lower and Middle Pleistocene, and its possible implications. Journal of Human Evolution 14: 607–614.N. Toth1985Archaeological evidence for preferential right-handedness in the Lower and Middle Pleistocene, and its possible implications.Journal of Human Evolution14607614
  83. 83. Stock JT, Pfeiffer S (2004) Long bone robusticity and subsistence behavior among Later Stone Age foragers of the forest and fynbos biomes of South Africa. Journal of Archaeological Sciences 31: 999–1013.JT StockS. Pfeiffer2004Long bone robusticity and subsistence behavior among Later Stone Age foragers of the forest and fynbos biomes of South Africa.Journal of Archaeological Sciences319991013
  84. 84. Oldfield R (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9: 97–113.R. Oldfield1971The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory.Neuropsychologia997113
  85. 85. Cram J, Kasman G (1998) Electrode placement. Aspen: Gaithersburgh. J. CramG. Kasman1998Electrode placement.Aspen: Gaithersburgh