Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Characterizing the Collective Personality of Ant Societies: Aggressive Colonies Do Not Abandon Their Home

  • Inon Scharf ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Inon Scharf, Andreas P. Modlmeier

    scharfi@uni-mainz.de

    Affiliation Institute of Zoology, Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz, Mainz, Germany

  • Andreas P. Modlmeier ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Inon Scharf, Andreas P. Modlmeier

    Affiliation Institute of Zoology, Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz, Mainz, Germany

  • Stephan Fries,

    Affiliation Institute of Zoology, Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz, Mainz, Germany

  • Claire Tirard,

    Affiliation Laboratoire Ecologie & Evolution CNRS UMR 7625, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Paris 6, Paris, France

  • Susanne Foitzik

    Affiliation Institute of Zoology, Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz, Mainz, Germany

Abstract

Animal groups can show consistent behaviors or personalities just like solitary animals. We studied the collective behavior of Temnothorax nylanderi ant colonies, including consistency in behavior and correlations between different behavioral traits. We focused on four collective behaviors (aggression against intruders, nest relocation, removal of infected corpses and nest reconstruction) and also tested for links to the immune defense level of a colony and a fitness component (per-capita productivity). Behaviors leading to an increased exposure of ants to micro-parasites were expected to be positively associated with immune defense measures and indeed colonies that often relocated to other nest sites showed increased immune defense levels. Besides, colonies that responded with low aggression to intruders or failed to remove infected corpses, showed a higher likelihood to move to a new nest site. This resembles the trade-off between aggression and relocation often observed in solitary animals. Finally, one of the behaviors, nest reconstruction, was positively linked to per-capita productivity, whereas other colony-level behaviors, such as aggression against intruders, showed no association, albeit all behaviors were expected to be important for fitness under field conditions. In summary, our study shows that ant societies exhibit complex personalities that can be associated to the physiology and fitness of the colony. Some of these behaviors are linked in suites of correlated behaviors, similar to personalities of solitary animals.

Introduction

Variation in heritable traits such as morphology or behavior is expected to be constantly removed from natural populations by drift and natural or sexual selection and it is therefore interesting to study the factors that maintain variation [1], [2]. Behavioral syndromes, defined as the consistency in behavior across different situations and contexts, can explain why behavioral variation is kept. The same behavior may be beneficial in certain contexts but may be maladaptive in other situations (e.g., [3]). This could result in non-directional selection on distinct behavioral types. For example, aggression can be useful against prey or competitors, but it can discourage or even lead to the premature death of potential mates, as it has been shown in a fishing spider [4]. In addition, being active in the absence of predators may be beneficial, but high activity levels in their presence are often risky [5]. Therefore, detecting correlations between behavioral traits is valuable for understanding how intra-population variation is maintained.

Behavioral syndromes have been described in various animal systems, with recurrent correlations between certain behaviors. A common behavioral syndrome is the aggressiveness-boldness syndrome. Aggressive individuals often tend to be more active and they take more risks (e.g., [5], [6]). For instance, aggressive male field crickets went faster out of refuge in a novel environment, that is, they were also bolder [7]. Another common behavioral syndrome is related to activity in general. Many behaviors tend to be positively associated with the activity level of an individual, because they are simultaneously affected by the metabolic rate or time constraints [6], [8]. This should lead to a triplet positive association among aggressiveness, boldness and general activity. Behavioral syndromes in solitary animals are often linked to physiological or life-history traits of the organism, which can help understanding the proximate correlates of behavior. For example, metabolic rate differences are a possible physiological explanation for consistent inter-individual differences in activity and aggression, explaining in part the aggressiveness-boldness-activity syndrome [6], [8].

While behavioral correlations are well documented for some solitary animals, there is still little evidence for behavioral correlations in animal societies (but see three recent papers: [9], [10], [11]). Using behavioral methodologies typical for solitary animals, these studies deal with research questions relevant to social insects, such as behavioral differences between castes or cooperative behavior of the whole colony. For example, Chapman et al. [9] showed that the patroller caste in Myrmica ants exhibited the common aggressiveness-boldness syndrome, while the brood-carer caste did not. In addition, the behavior of these two castes is correlated on the colony level (i.e., the whole colony is sometimes more aggressive and bold). Interestingly, social groups, as a whole, may differ in behavior, which can affect their success. Wray et al. [11] showed that the defensive response of honey bee colonies was correlated with fitness components (the colony weight). In social insects, selection predominantly acts on the colony level and collective behaviors such as communal defense, networking in foraging and nest construction are expected to be strongly linked to colony productivity. Hence, colony behavior can be shaped by natural selection similar to the behavior of multicellular organisms.

We used colonies of the European cavity-dwelling ant, Temnothorax nylanderi, to study consistency, variation and the relationship of four important behaviors: (1) aggression towards an intruder, (2) nest relocation, (3) removal of an infected corpse, and (4) nest reconstruction after partial destruction. These behaviors represent important activities of animal societies in general. Collective defense is typical for many groups, and is evident in many bird species living in groups, as mobbing of predators gets more efficient with colony size [12]. In social insects, aggression is vital in defending the nest and has fitness consequences (e.g., [11]). Collective movement is an important trait of animal groups expressed by fish schools, bird flocks, locust swarms and social insects (reviewed in [13]). Nest relocation is a common behavior in many social insect species, which is exhibited when the present nest becomes unsuitable for some reason, such as decomposition of the nesting material in cavity-dwelling ants [14], [15] or local food depletion in army ants (e.g., [16]). Other reasons for nest relocation are reproduction – a large colony splits into two parts and one of them leaves, i.e., reproduction by budding [15] – and simply moving into a better-larger nest [17].

Living in groups increases the parasite burden and the risk of infection by contact-transmitted parasites [12], [18]. Therefore, removal of waste and corpses of dead group members is crucial for colony health [19]. Indeed, waste management has been found in various group-living animals, which live in the same nest sites for longer time periods (e.g., aphids, mites and ants [20], [21], [22]). Nest sites provide a safe environment to raise offspring, but they have to be constructed and maintained, and failure to repair may lead to exposure to external risks, such as predators or parasites [23]. In order to increase defensiveness against intruders, colonies often block or reduce the nest entrance by using soil, sand or wooden pieces (e.g., [14], [24], [25]). Moreover, colonies of the ant genus Temnothorax prefer nest sites with very small entrances so that a single ant could control colony entry [14].

Even before testing behavior under different conditions, testing for repeatability of behavior under the same conditions is a necessary step in characterizing behavioral syndromes and personality [26]. Second, searching for collective colony personality, we looked for positive and/or negative correlations among the four behavioral traits. We predict that similar to the aggressiveness-boldness-activity syndrome in solitary animals, aggressive colonies should be bolder and more active. Therefore, they should show a better performance in the other collective behaviors such as nest reconstruction. However, as ant colonies are also energy limited we expect some behavioral trade-offs. In addition, it is intriguing to relate behavior and personality to fitness components [26], [27], and to understand whether different colony personalities result in the same final fitness. We therefore tested how the four behaviors correlate with per-capita productivity as a measure of colony efficiency (e.g., [28], [29]). The four documented behaviors are important for colony survival, representing ways to overcome stress and threat. We expect in general a positive effect of the measured behaviors on per-capita productivity. However, we do not expect a perfect match, because the same goal can be achieved in parallel ways, and specializing in one behavior may lead to another becoming superfluous.

Immune defense is an important physiological trait in social insects, because frequent interactions of genetically similar individuals lead to a great risk of contagious infections [18], [19]. The level of immune defense may correlate with inter-colony differences in some behaviors, such as corpse removal and nest relocation. Encounters with infected corpses pose a direct threat to the colony, and the ants should react by increasing their immune defense [22]. However, ant colonies that recognize and remove infectious material from the nest faster and thus show a high social immunity, might be able to invest less in the physiological immune defense. Nest relocation may be triggered by exposure to parasites, but during the move ants and their brood are also vulnerable to infection and predation [19]. Similarly, intruders might increase micro-parasite exposure [18]. We therefore expected that colonies which show a high tendency to expose their members to parasites either during nest migration, nest defense or by failing to reconstruct their nest site should invest highly in their immune function. In contrast, ant colonies that remove infected corpses fast from the nest are expected to show low immune functions. Nest relocation should show the best positive correlation with the immune defense level, because during emigrations all colony members are exposed to the surrounding environment.

Methods

Study organisms and sites

Temnothorax nylanderi are small cavity-dwelling ants inhabiting forests in western and central Europe. Their colonies comprise several dozen workers and a single queen. They reside in preformed cavities of various wooden structures on the forest floor providing protection from the outer environment [30], [31]. Due to the decomposition of their nests in the field, these ant colonies are forced to frequently relocate their nests [14], [17]. Temnothorax nylanderi is a suitable species for the research questions because it shows high variability in behavior, mainly in relocation and aggressive tendencies. As mentioned, all studied behaviors have implications for the colony's performance under natural conditions. We collected 50 T. nylanderi colonies in summer 2009 in Sommerhausen, Germany (49.706N, 10.030E). No specific permits were required for the described ant collection (the collection site is not privately owned or protected and the collected ants are not endangered or protected). The colonies were brought to the laboratory and moved to artificial nests (7.5×2.5×0.5 cm) in plastic boxes (10×10×1.5 cm) with a plastered floor. The nests were kept in climate chambers imitating the natural temperatures around the year in the habitat of origin (summer: 20°/15°C day/night; autumn/spring: 15°/10°C day/night; winter: temperatures gradually decreased to −5°C). Colonies were fed weekly with honey, crickets and water. Colonies were kept for ∼1.5 years under standard conditions to moderate environmental effects on behavior, physiology and life-history traits. Consequently, we estimate that more than 50% of the workers emerged in the laboratory. Two weeks prior to the experiment, colonies were moved to summer conditions. The experiments were conducted at room temperature.

Experimental design

In March–May 2011 we tested the performance of the ant colonies in four standardized tests. We were interested in four important behaviors: (I) aggression against a conspecific intruder, (II) nest relocation, (III) corpse removal out of the nest, and (IV) reconstruction of the nest after partial opening. Each behavior was tested twice for each colony, with one week in-between experiments. The behavioral observations were performed under stereomicroscopes by the two first authors, but all behaviors of a colony were observed by the same observer. In addition, we counted the number of workers (i.e., colony size) before the first behavioral test. After the aggression tests, we counted the number of brood items: New queen, male and worker pupae, pre-pupae and larvae and calculated the per-capita productivity for each colony (colony production divided by colony size). We were also interested in possible correlations of those four behaviors with the immune defense level.

I. Aggression against a conspecific intruder.

Colony aggression was quantified by entering a dead non-nestmate conspecific worker into the nest and measuring all aggressive interactions during the next five minutes (according to the protocol described in [32], [33]). Specifically, we documented every 20–30 seconds how many ants were either antennating or attacking the intruder (11 observations per trial). Dead ants, killed by freezing in −20°C prior to the experiment, were used to eliminate behavioral variation between the stimuli and focus on the focal colony's response to the chemical stimulus. We used the percent of aggressive interactions (no. of mandible spreading, biting, dragging, holding and stinging events divided by all of the above plus antennating events), as a measure of aggression [32], [33]. Aggression tests were performed twice for each colony (one week between the two trials).

II. Nest relocation.

On the day after each aggression test, we gave the ants the opportunity to relocate into new nests, without damaging the original nest. The new nest, which was identical to the original one, was placed seven centimetres from the occupied nest, and we noted after 24 hours whether the colonies relocated. It was a reasonable time, since after 24 hours ∼40% of the colonies moved at least three workers to the new nest. We scored the relocation tendency according to four levels: No relocation (0), three or more workers without brood occur in the new nest (1), workers and brood occur in the new nest but still also occupy the old one, i.e., the colony has split (possible in T. nylanderi) (2), and complete relocation, the old nest is empty (3).

III. Removal of a corpse out of the nest.

Three weeks later we sacrificed a large T. nylanderi colony by freezing at −20°C, opened its nest, added some water and let the ants decompose for 48 hours at room temperature (20°C). After that time the ant corpses were covered by white unidentified fungi (Fig. 1). We entered three ant corpses (randomly chosen) to each nest and measured the time the colony required to remove the first dead decomposed ant. We obtained the exact time of the corpse removal, if it was removed within the first 16 minutes. We took another observation after 30 minutes. If the corpse was removed after 16 minutes and before 30 minutes, the colony received the value “23 minutes”, and if no corpse was removed after 30 minutes, the colony received the maximal value of “30 minutes” (occurred in 5% of the cases). We repeated this procedure a week later.

thumbnail
Figure 1. A decaying corpse of a T. nylanderi ant covered by unidentified fungi, which was used for the corpse removal experiment.

A conspecific in such a condition presumably poses some risk of micro-parasite infection to the colony and should be removed from the nest.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033314.g001

IV. Nest reconstruction after partial opening.

Two weeks after the corpse removal experiment we measured the last behavior, the reconstruction of the nest. We first removed parts of the plastic nest, so that the nest entrance was increased from 0.3 cm to 1.5 cm. We provided the ant nests with a standardized amount of fine sand (1 ml), which could be used by the workers to block part of the now widened nest entrance. Previous experiments with Temnothorax ants have shown that these ants prefer nest sites with small entrances, which are easier to defend against larger ants [14]. Two days later we photographed the nest entrance and measured the percentage of the entrance which was covered with sand particles (Fig. 2): We drew a line closing the entrance and calculated the proportion of this line covered with sand. Digital measurements were done by the first author using the software ImageJ. We repeated the whole procedure a week later.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Two T. nylanderi nests representing two extremes of nest reconstruction after partial opening: (A) entrance is almost fully blocked again; (B) entrance is almost unblocked.

Entrance blocking should protect better against invasions to the nest.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033314.g002

V. Immune defense level.

After the behavioral experiments, we randomly chose 21 colonies and collected two foragers, two ants located next to the entrance and two ants taking care of the brood from each colony for immune defense measurements (a total of six ants, representing the colony immune defense level). We chose ants performing different tasks, as a previous study pointed to differences in immune defense among castes or workers performing different tasks (e.g., [34]). As a proxy of immune defense level, we measured the activity of the phenoloxidase enzyme (PO). This enzyme is mainly found in the hemolymph as pro-phenoloxidase (PPO) and is activated into phenoloxidase prior to measurement. We therefore measured the total amount of PO potentially available for the individual (both activated and stored pro-enzyme). PO is an important component of invertebrates' immune defense and is involved in melanisation, cellular defense response and wound healing processes [35].

We used a similar procedure to Bocher et al. [34]. We placed individual ants in 20 µL sodium cacodilate/CaCl2 buffer (0.01 M Na-Cac, 0.005 M CaCl2), cut them with small scissors, and centrifuged using a cooling device. We moved the liquid part to new Eppendorf tubes and added 10 µL chymotrypsin to activate the PPO. As a substrate we used L-DOPA (4 mg mL−1 in distilled water) and the reaction was performed at 30°C in a temperature-controlled spectrophotometer (Multiscan FC, Thermo Scientific, Vantaa, Finland) for 40 min. We measured the absorbance at 492 nm every 25 s. The enzyme activity was determined according to the slope of the linear phase of the reaction (200–675 s after the reaction's start). In each run of the spectrophotometer we used 3 negative controls. We removed cases in which the absorbance curve was too irregular. The amount of PPO may correlate with body size. In order to correct for that, each slope was divided by the head width of each ant, photographed prior to the PPO measurement using a digital camera and a binocular.

Statistics

Consistency in behaviour.

Prior to analysis, we used the Z-score transformation controlling for the variance of the variables in different units, i.e., subtracting the mean and dividing by the standard deviation [36], and the transformed values were used in all further statistical tests. In order to test for consistency in behavior of the first and second trial, we used three different tests: (1) The Individual Stability Statistic (hereafter, ISS [37]): , where zt1 and zt2 are the values of the two trials of each behavior; (2) The Cronbach's α, measuring reliability of repeated tests [11]; (3) Pearson correlation. Higher values in all three tests point to high consistency in behavior while lower values indicate higher variance. We then used the Pearson correlation to correlate among all four ISS values (a single value for each behavior tested, based on the two trials). The purpose was to detect whether colonies showing consistent behavior in one trait are also consistent in other traits (i.e., ‘consistent in consistency’). It is plausible that colony size affects different behaviors and/or the level of consistency. In order to test for that, we correlated colony size with the four behaviors and the ISS levels.

Characterization of behavioral syndromes.

We used the raw data (not transformed) to calculate the mean of the two trials documented for every behavioral trait (note that means enabled intermediate levels of relocation behavior, seven levels in total). Then we used the Z-score transformation on those mean values and performed a Factor Analysis (FA) on the four behavioral traits [36]. We also applied a varimax rotation to facilitate interpretation. Time to dead conspecific removal was given in negative values, so fast removal will score highly, similar to high aggression and high levels of nest reconstruction (high values represent stronger expression of each behavior). FA is a common way to characterize behavioral syndromes [11], [38]. The factors with eigenvalues larger than one should represent sets of behaviors. To further investigate these sets, we used a Pearson correlation.

Linking behavior with productivity and immune defense.

We aimed at testing how the behavioral traits are linked to an important fitness component, the per-capita productivity. We already know that there is a negative link between colony size and per-capita productivity [28]. We therefore did not correlate the 1st and 2nd factors with per-capita productivity directly, but with the residuals of the linear regression of colony size and per-capita productivity (hereafter ‘productivity residuals’), in order to control for colony size. Then we used a two-way regression with the 1st and 2nd factors and their interaction as explanatory variables and the productivity residuals as the dependent variable. Regarding the immune defense level, we first tested whether colonies and ants performing different tasks differ in the PPO levels (a two-way ANOVA). Then, we used the average value of PPO for each colony (over six workers) and correlated the PPO levels with all four behavioral traits, using Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. We used STATISTICA v. 9.1 (StatSoft.Inc, Tulsa, OK, USA) and SYSTAT v. 11 (SYSTAT Software, San Jose, CA, USA) for all statistical analyses. See (Table S1) for the raw data of all experiments.

Results

Consistency in behavior between the two trials, represented by the Individual Stability Statistic (ISS), was lower for aggressive behavior and corpse removal compared to nest reconstruction and relocation (Fig. 3). There was no correlation among consistencies (P>0.26 for all pairwise comparisons), meaning that there was no clear relationship among the consistency levels shown by colonies for different behavioral traits, i.e., high consistency in one behavior was not associated with high consistency in another one. When correlating pairs of trials separately for each behavior, significant positive correlations were evident for all behaviors except for aggression (aggression: Bartlett χ2 statistic = 2.27, df = 1, P = 0.13; nest relocation: χ2 = 13.36, df = 1, P = 0.0003; corpse removal: χ2 = 5.38, df = 1, P = 0.020; nest reconstruction: χ2 = 29.14, df = 1, P<0.0001). Cronbach's α tests produced similar results (Fig. 3), in accordance with the prior analysis using ISS values. Colony size was not correlated with any of the observed behaviors (P>0.32) nor with behavioral consistency (ISS values; P>0.39), even without a Bonferroni correction, suggesting no link of colony size with the behaviors observed. Table 1 presents summary statistics for the two trials of the four behaviors observed.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Consistency values (ISS; bright grey, left) of the two behavioral trials for each of the four behaviors measured (means±1 SE), and Cronbach's α coefficients of reliability tests (dark grey, right).

High values represent a higher consistency level.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033314.g003

thumbnail
Table 1. Summary statistics for the two trials of the four behaviors observed.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033314.t001

We performed factor analysis (FA) on transformed means of the four behavioral traits. Eigenvalues, the percentage of the variance explained and factor loadings for different behaviors on all the FA axes are presented in Table 2. The 1st factor represented an ‘aggressive vs. emigration-prone’ colony personality, because aggression and nest relocation had high loadings on this factor, but in opposite signs (positive and negative respectively). Corpse removal loaded positively, similar to aggression but to a lesser extent. We performed a Pearson correlation between the untransformed means of the two main behaviors of the 1st factor, aggression and nest relocation, that confirmed to some extent the negative relationship (a marginally non-significant trend: Bartlett χ2 statistic = 3.12, df = 1, P = 0.077). The 2nd factor was composed mainly of nest reconstruction (negative), while other behaviors showed loadings close to zero. The two other factors, the 3rd and 4th ones, had eigenvalues lower than 1 and are not further discussed. The 2nd factor negatively correlated with the productivity residuals (coefficient = −0.355, P = 0.019). It suggests a positive link between nest reconstruction and per-capita productivity (note that nest reconstruction scored negatively on the 2nd factor, and therefore a negative correlation of productivity with the 2nd factor implied on a positive correlation with nest reconstruction; Fig. 4). The 1st factor showed a marginally non-significant negative correlation with the productivity residuals (coefficient = −0.264, P = 0.077). It might suggest a negative effect of high aggression and a positive effect of relocation tendency on per-capita productivity, but since it is not significant, this possible association is not further discussed. The interaction term was not significant (P = 0.94) and was removed from analysis; statistics for the whole model are: F2,47 = 4.59, R2 = 0.164, P = 0.015. In summary, the 1st factor showed a trade-off between aggression and corpse removal vs. nest relocation, while the 2nd factor was mainly composed of nest reconstruction. The correlation of the 2nd factor with productivity suggests that reconstruction contributes to this fitness component, but a manipulation is required to better support this association.

thumbnail
Figure 4. The positive relationship between nest reconstruction (proportion) and per-capita productivity (total number of brood divided by number of workers).

The trend line is the best-fitted line according to a linear regression test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033314.g004

thumbnail
Table 2. Results of the two first factors of the factor analysis performed on mean behaviors of two trials after applying a Z-score transformation and a varimax rotation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033314.t002

Immune defense levels, represented by phenol- and pro-phenoloxidase (PPO), were evenly distributed among ants performing different tasks (F2,56 = 0.51, P = 0.60), but differed between colonies (F20,56 = 2.08, P = 0.017). The two-way interaction term was not significant (F40,56 = 1.08, P = 0.39). PPO levels were positively correlated with the relocation tendency (Pearson coefficient = 0.59, P = 0.020; Fig. 5), that is colonies which tended to relocate their nests show higher immune defense levels. All other behavioral traits were not correlated with PPO levels after Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

thumbnail
Figure 5. The positive relationship between the nest relocation tendency and the immune defense level, corrected for body size (head width of individual ants).

The trend line is the best-fitted line according to a linear regression test.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033314.g005

Discussion

Behavioral syndromes and personalities/temperament have often been demonstrated for solitary animals, but evidence for syndromes in insect societies or characterization of collective personality are still rare (but see [9], [10], [11]). Our study is one of the first to show collective personality on the colony level. The most important result is the evidence for a collective personality: colonies that defend their nest, either by fighting against intruders more aggressively or by removing infected corpses more efficiently, are less likely to relocate after a disturbance. It fits a common trade-off between competitiveness and emigration tendencies (e.g., [39], [40]). The behavioral consistency was the highest for nest reconstruction and relocation, less strong for removal of corpses and non-significant for aggression. This difference is probably related to the level of specialization each activity requires. Interestingly, the immune defense level was correlated with the nest relocation tendency, but with no other behavior, possibly because nest emigration is the only action exposing the whole colony to the surrounding environment. Finally, there is a positive correlation between per-capita productivity and nest reconstruction, suggesting a link between behavior and a fitness component.

The trade-off between territory defense, either by defending against intruders (elevated aggression) or parasites (efficient corpse removal), and the tendency to relocate is often shown by solitary animals. We suggest several explanations for this trade-off in ant societies. First, some nests might be considered to be of better quality than others and were therefore fiercer defended and less easily abandoned. Temnothorax colonies easily distinguish between nest types and often move if a better nest is available [14], [17]. Second, defending the nest implies that ants invested effort and energy, and therefore are reluctant to move out. Third, after colonies fail for some reason to fight back against intruders or to remove potential source of infection, they tend to relocate more readily to another nest, which might be more defensible. This trade-off between defense against intruders and relocation tendency has parallels in solitary animals owning a territory. For example, Cichlid fish males tended less to abandon their territory in the presence of predators if they were defending it before the encounter with predators [41], and less aggressive Cichlid females were more likely to emigrate than more aggressive ones [42]. Yet, the aggressiveness-relocation trade-off shown here also fits a more general ecological pattern: more competitive animals usually stay while less competitive ones emigrate, e.g., flour beetles [39], [40]. In related T. longispinosus colonies, aggressive colonies were more often found in dense areas [43]. This suggests that aggressive colonies remain in these dense areas and do not relocate despite frequent disturbances by intruders while less aggressive ones may move away.

The consistency in behavior was higher for nest reconstruction and relocation than aggression and removal of corpses. The two latter behaviors are performed by specialist ants, while the two former are a true collective behavior of the whole colony. Therefore, the repeatability of behaviors based on only a few specialist ants is possibly weaker, because specialist ants could have died between trials or on the other hand can also improve their efficiency with trials. Similarly, corpse removal in other social insects is done by few specialists, to minimize the exposure to contagious elements. This behavior may vary according to response thresholds to corpse/waste removal [19], [22], [44]. Similarly, aggression and kin discrimination are presumably carried out by a small group of specialist workers [45], leading to the same pattern of low consistency in behavior. In comparison, nest relocation requires a more coordinated effort: In a related Temnothorax species, one third of the colony recruits nestmates, actively participating in nest relocation [46]. We believe that the link between worker specialization and the consistency in colony behavior is important for further understanding behavioral syndromes in social insects. An interesting future direction would be to increase environmental heterogeneity and look for behavioral consistencies, expecting that consistency would be negatively correlated with environmental heterogeneity in space or time [47].

The immune defense level was positively correlated with the tendency of nest relocation, but not with any other behavior. Further research is required to establish more firmly the relation between immune defense level and relocation tendency, also in interaction with other environmental factors. We suggest that nest relocation is the only tested behavior exposing all colony members to the external environment, while the three other behaviors are carried out by a small fraction of the colony workers. Specialized workers can prevent the exposure of the whole colony to external risks in such cases, but exposure is inevitable during emigration. We suggest that colonies try to behaviorally adjust to the risk, but when not possible, react physiologically.

The relationship between different behaviors and fitness components is often taken for granted but is a fundamental issue in behavioral ecology (i.e., behavior is assumed to optimize fitness). Specifically in the field of behavioral syndromes and animal personality, there is a need for a better link with fitness [5], [26]. Smith and Blumstein [27] reviewed fitness consequences of animal personality, and showed that exploration was positively correlated with animal survival, and aggression increased with reproductive success. In social insects, Wray et al. showed a link between foraging and defense behaviors with productivity and survival [11], and Modlmeier and Foitzik demonstrated a positive relationship between the within-colony variance in behavior and productivity in the field [43] and in the laboratory [48]. We showed here a possible link between the nest reconstruction behavior and productivity.

Social insect colonies often prefer small entrances to their nests, as small entrances are more easily defendable (e.g., [14]). Nest usurpation of Temnothorax colonies is a common phenomenon, often by other ant species of larger colonies (e.g., [49]). Presumably to avoid invasions of different natures, Temnothorax species often reduce the entrance further more by accumulating dirt particles [25]. Other social insect species close their entrance in various ways in order to protect the colony against invasions [24], [50]. But to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study showing a positive correlation of nest reconstruction with some fitness component. Such a link can result from the positive contribution of this behavior to fitness or alternatively may indicate that colonies having more brood relative to workers block their entrance more intensively, because they have more to lose from invasions to their nests than colonies with less brood. The other observed behaviors did not correlate with per-capita productivity. A possible reason may be that after a while in lab conditions, behaviors which enhance survival and productivity under field conditions become less relevant. In general, measuring fitness of insect societies is challenging, because of their complex life cycle and long life span. It is also plausible that colonies would employ different short- and long-term strategies, resulting in various effects on fitness components [29]. Finally, a more experimental approach would be to investigate the suggested trade-off of aggression with nest relocation. Manipulating the nest condition/quality or alternatively inducing different levels of aggression can be a promising approach.

Supporting Information

Table S1.

Raw dataset used for the statistical analysis. For further information, please contact the first or second author.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033314.s001

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Heike Stypa for her help in the lab, and Claudie Doums for her helpful comments on a previous version of the manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: IS APM S. Foitzik. Performed the experiments: IS APM S. Fries. Analyzed the data: IS APM S. Foitzik. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: CT S. Foitzik. Wrote the paper: IS APM S. Foitzik. Established the immunological analyses for the study species: CT.

References

  1. 1. Brockmann HJ (2001) The evolution of alternative strategies and tactics. Advances in the Study of Behavior 30: 1–51.
  2. 2. Fitzpatrick BM, Shook K, Izally R (2009) Frequency-dependent selection by wild birds promotes polymorphism in model salamanders. BMC Ecology 9: 12.
  3. 3. Dall SRX, Houston AI, McNamara JM (2004) The behavioural ecology of personality: consistent individual differences from an adaptive perspective. Ecology Letters 7: 734–739.
  4. 4. Johnson JC, Sih A (2005) Precopulatory sexual cannibalism in fishing spiders (Dolomedes triton): a role for behavioral syndromes. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology 58: 390–396.
  5. 5. Sih A, Bell A, Johnson JC (2004) Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 19: 372–378.
  6. 6. Moretz JA, Martinus EP, Robison BD (2007) Behavioral syndromes and the evolution of correlated behavior in zebrafish. Behavioral Ecology 18: 556–562.
  7. 7. Kortet R, Hedrick A (2007) A behavioural syndrome in the field cricket Gryllus integer: intrasexual aggression is correlated with activity in a novel environment. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 91: 475–482.
  8. 8. Biro PA, Stamps JA (2010) Do consistent individual differences in metabolic rate promote consistent individual differences in behavior? Trends in Ecology & Evolution 25: 653–659.
  9. 9. Chapman BB, Thain H, Coughlin J, Hughes WOH (2011) Behavioural syndromes at multiple scales in Myrmica ants. Animal Behaviour 82: 391–397.
  10. 10. Gordon DM, Guetz A, Greene MJ, Holmes S (2011) Colony variation in the collective regulation of foraging by harvester ants. Behavioral Ecology 22: 429–435.
  11. 11. Wray MK, Mattila HR, Seeley TD (2011) Collective personalities in honeybee colonies are linked to colony fitness. Animal Behaviour 81: 559–568.
  12. 12. Krause J, Ruxton GD (2002) Living in groups. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  13. 13. Sumpter DJT (2006) The principles of collective animal behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 361: 5–22.
  14. 14. Pratt SC, Pierce NE (2001) The cavity-dwelling ant Leptothorax curvispinosus uses nest geometry to discriminate between potential homes. Animal Behaviour 62: 281–287.
  15. 15. Visscher PK (2007) Group decision making in nest-site selection among social insects. Annual Review of Entomology 52: 255–275.
  16. 16. Schöning C, Njagi WM, Franks NR (2005) Temporal and spatial patterns in the emigrations of the army ant Dorylus (Anomma) molestus in the montane forest of Mt Kenya. Ecological Entomology 30: 532–540.
  17. 17. Dornhaus A, Franks NR, Hawkins RM, Shere HNS (2004) Ants move to improve: colonies of Leptothorax albipennis emigrate whenever they find a superior nest site. Animal Behaviour 67: 959–963.
  18. 18. Schmid-Hempel P (1998) Parasites in social insects. Princeton, NJ, USA: Princeton University Press.
  19. 19. Cremer S, Armitage SAO, Schmid-Hempel P (2007) Social immunity. Current Biology 17: R693–R702.
  20. 20. Benton TG, Foster WA (1992) Altruistic housekeeping in a social aphid. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 247: 199–202.
  21. 21. Sato Y, Saito Y (2006) Nest sanitation in social spider mites: interspecific differences in defecation behavior. Ethology 112: 664–669.
  22. 22. Diez L, Deneubourg JL, Hoebeke L, Detrain C (2011) Orientation in corpse-carrying ants: memory or chemical cues? Animal Behaviour 81: 1171–1176.
  23. 23. Pike N, Foster W (2004) Fortress repair in the social aphid species Pemphigus spyrothecae. Animal Behaviour 67: 909–914.
  24. 24. Seeley TD, Morse RA (1978) Nest site selection by the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Insectes Sociaux 25: 323–337.
  25. 25. Herbers JM, Banschbach V (1995) Size-dependent nest site choice by cavity-dwelling ants. Psyche 102: 13–17.
  26. 26. Réale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall PT, Dingemanse NJ (2007) Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biological Reviews 82: 291–318.
  27. 27. Smith BR, Blumstein DT (2008) Fitness consequences of personality: a meat-analysis. Behavioral Ecology 19: 448–455.
  28. 28. Michener CD (1964) Reproductive efficiency in relation to colony size in hymenopterous societies. Insectes Sociaux 4: 317–341.
  29. 29. Scharf I, Bauer S, Fischer-Blass B, Foitzik S (2011) Impact of a social parasite on ant host populations depends on host species, habitat and year. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 103: 559–570.
  30. 30. Foitzik S, Heinze J (1998) Nest site limitation and colony takeover in the ant Leptothorax nylanderi. Behavioral Ecology 9: 367–375.
  31. 31. Foitzik S, Strätz M, Heinze J (2003) Ecology, life history and resource allocation in the ant, Leptothorax nylanderi. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 16: 670–680.
  32. 32. Pamminger T, Scharf I, Pennings PS, Foitzik S (2011) Increased host aggression as an induced defense against slave-making ants. Behavioral Ecology 22: 255–260.
  33. 33. Scharf I, Pamminger T, Foitzik S (2011) Differential response of ant colonies to intruders: attack strategies correlate with potential threat. Ethology 117: 731–739.
  34. 34. Bocher A, Tirard C, Doums C (2007) Phenotypic plasticity of immune defence linked with foraging activity in the ant Cataglyphis velox. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 20: 2228–2234.
  35. 35. Cerenius L, Lee BL, Söderhäll K (2008) The proPO-system: pros and cons for its role in invertebrate immunity. Trends in Immunology 29: 263–271.
  36. 36. Gotelli NJ, Ellison AM (2004) A primer of ecological statistics. Sunderland, MA, USA: Sinauer.
  37. 37. Asendorpf JB (1990) The measurement of individual consistency. Methodika 4: 1–23.
  38. 38. Dingemanse NJ, Dochtermann N, Wright J (2010) A method for exploring the structure of behavioural syndromes to allow formal comparison within and between data sets. Animal Behaviour 79: 439–450.
  39. 39. Korona R (1989) Evolutionarily stable strategies in competition for resource intake rate maximization: II. Oviposition behavior in Tribolium confusum. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology 25: 201–205.
  40. 40. Zirkle DF, Dawson PS, Lavie B (1988) An experimental analysis of the genetic relationships among life-history traits and emigration behavior in Tribolium castaneum. Oikos 53: 391–397.
  41. 41. Markert JA, Arnegard ME (2007) Size-dependent use of territorial space by a rock-dwelling cichlid fish. Oecologia 154: 611–621.
  42. 42. Schradin C, Lamprecht J (2002) Causes of female emigration in the group-living Cichlid fish Neolamprologus multifasciatus. Ethology 108: 237–248.
  43. 43. Modlmeier AP, Foitzik S (2011) Productivity increases with variation in aggression among groups members in Temnothorax ants. Behavioral Ecology 22: 1026–1032.
  44. 44. Waddington SJ, Hughes WOH (1010) Waste management in the leaf-cutting ant Acromyrmex echinatior: the role of worker size, age and plasticity. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology 64: 1219–1228.
  45. 45. Crosland MWJ (1990) Variation in ant aggression and kin discrimination ability within and between colonies. Journal of Insect Behavior 3: 359–379.
  46. 46. Pratt SC, Mallon EB, Sumpter DJT, Franks NR (2002) Quorum sensing, recruitment, and collective decision-making during colony emigration by the ant Leptothorax albipennis. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology 52: 117–127.
  47. 47. Stamps J, Groothuis TGG (2010) The development of animal personality: relevance, concepts and perspectives. Biological Reviews 85: 301–325.
  48. 48. Modlmeier AP, Liebmann JE, Foitzik S (2012) Diverse societies are more productive: a lesson from ants. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, published online before print January 25, 2012, doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.2376.
  49. 49. Yamaguchi T (1992) Interspecific interference for nest sites between Leptothorax congruus and Monomorium intrudens. Insectes Sociaux 39: 117–127.
  50. 50. Cerdá X, Retana J (1998) Interference interactions and nest usurpation between two subordinate ant species. Oecologia 113: 577–583.