Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

DRD4 Polymorphism Moderates the Effect of Alcohol Consumption on Social Bonding

  • Kasey G. Creswell ,

    kgriffin@pitt.edu

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Michael A. Sayette,

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Stephen B. Manuck,

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Robert E. Ferrell,

    Affiliation Department of Human Genetics, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • Shirley Y. Hill,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America

  • John D. Dimoff

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, United States of America

Abstract

Development of interpersonal relationships is a fundamental human motivation, and behaviors facilitating social bonding are prized. Some individuals experience enhanced reward from alcohol in social contexts and may be at heightened risk for developing and maintaining problematic drinking. We employed a 3 (group beverage condition) ×2 (genotype) design (N = 422) to test the moderating influence of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4 VNTR) polymorphism on the effects of alcohol on social bonding. A significant gene x environment interaction showed that carriers of at least one copy of the 7-repeat allele reported higher social bonding in the alcohol, relative to placebo or control conditions, whereas alcohol did not affect ratings of 7-absent allele carriers. Carriers of the 7-repeat allele were especially sensitive to alcohol's effects on social bonding. These data converge with other recent gene-environment interaction findings implicating the DRD4 polymorphism in the development of alcohol use disorders, and results suggest a specific pathway by which social factors may increase risk for problematic drinking among 7-repeat carriers. More generally, our findings highlight the potential utility of employing transdisciplinary methods that integrate genetic methodologies, social psychology, and addiction theory to improve theories of alcohol use and abuse.

Introduction

Social factors play an instrumental role in the development and maintenance of alcohol use disorders [1], [2]. Older adolescents and young adults do nearly all of their drinking with others [3], [4], suggesting that social processes may be particularly important in shaping drinking behavior early on and may play a key role in the development of problematic drinking [5]. Surveys indicate that people commonly endorse social motives for drinking [6][8], and expectancies of social facilitation are especially powerful in young adult drinkers [9], [10]. Moreover, the belief that alcohol facilitates social functioning is associated with problematic drinking in cross-sectional studies [11][13] and, in prospective studies, predictive of actual alcohol use [14] and alcohol use disorders. For instance, Patrick and colleagues [15] showed that social/recreational reasons for drinking at age 18 predicted symptoms of alcohol use disorders 17 years later, and Beseler and colleagues [16] showed that adults with a family history of alcoholism who drank for social facilitation and to reduce negative affect had a greater risk of alcohol dependence 10 years later.

Despite the general importance of social factors in the etiology of alcohol use disorders, there likely are individual differences in the extent to which alcohol is socially reinforcing. Individuals who experience more reward from alcohol in social settings may be at increased risk to misuse alcohol [17], [18], suggesting that individual differences in the socially reinforcing effects of alcohol may be related to genetic makeup. Social contexts can moderate the impact of genetic risk factors for a wide range of psychopathologies [19] including alcohol-related traits [20]. Indeed, the “contextual triggering” model of Shanahan & Hofer [21] states that social contexts can trigger a genetic predisposition. The social context in which drinking occurs may be an especially salient environmental factor with potential to modulate genetic influences on alcohol response [22][24]. Surprisingly, experimental paradigms designed to examine the reinforcing effects of alcohol have largely failed to consider social context. These laboratory studies recruit participants who almost always drink in social settings [3], but nearly all test these social drinkers in isolation [17]. Accordingly, most studies create uncommon conditions to assess the reinforcing effects of alcohol. Without considering social context, it is unsurprising that investigators have struggled to reliably explain the reinforcing effects of alcohol [25] or genetic mechanisms underlying these effects [26].

Group settings offer a unique chance to uncover important reinforcing effects of alcohol that might otherwise go unnoticed when examining participants in isolation [17], [27]. In fact, many of the subjectively pleasant effects of alcohol that confer increased risk for alcohol misuse (e.g., increased sociability) must be studied in a group setting [28]. There has been little systematic research on the effects of alcohol conducted in group settings, though, and despite the noted importance of contextual variables in the study of genetic effects [29], [30], no prior laboratory study has examined the moderating role of genetic variation on alcohol's reinforcing effects in a controlled group setting.

Because both the reinforcing effects of alcohol [31] and the rewarding effects of social interactions [32] are mediated via dopamine-dependent activity of the brain's mesocorticolimbic reward system, polymorphic variations in dopamine-regulating genes offer rational candidates for the genetic study of problematic drinking [33] and the study of interactions between alcohol abuse and social behaviors [34]. One particularly prominent polymorphism in psychiatric and behavioral genetics consists of a Variable Number of Tandem Repeats (VNTR) in exon 3 of the gene encoding the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4), represented by common length variants of 2, 4, and 7 repeats in most populations [35]. Activation of the G-protein-linked D4 receptor attenuates intracellular signaling by inhibiting adenylyl cyclase coupling, and this inhibitory effect is blunted by presence of the 7-repeat allele [36][38]. It is this attenuated response to dopamine produced by the 7-repeat variant that putatively underlies hypothesized associations of this polymorphism with addiction-related phenotypes [39], [40].

The 7-repeat allele of the DRD4 polymorphism has been associated with several behaviors and experiences, such as cigarette smoking [41][43], cue-elicited craving [44][47] but see [48], pathological gambling [49], [50], laboratory measures of financial risk taking and inhibitory motor control (e.g., [51][54]), fairness preference [55], human assortative mating patterns [56], and infidelity/sexual promiscuity [57], as well as disorders, such as Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) [58][60]. Notably, too, a growing literature shows many developmental effects of this VNTR on early behavioral outcomes (e.g., attachment organization, externalizing disorders, sensation seeking, and prosocial behaviors) to vary as a function of naturally occurring or experimentally manipulated environmental exposures [61], which in turn marks this polymorphism as a prime candidate for gene-environment interaction. In particular, the DRD4 genotype pertains to gene-environment interactions involving alcohol-related traits [62], [63].

Two recent studies underscore the importance of social factors in the link between DRD4 genotype and alcohol outcomes. Larsen et al. [62] reported that individuals carrying the 7-repeat allele drank more in the presence of a heavy-drinking confederate than those of other DRD4 genotypes, and Park et al. [63] found college/Greek involvement to be associated with increased risk of alcohol dependence, but only among students with at least one copy of the 7-repeat allele. Taken together, these two studies conducted in two different laboratories suggest a gene- environment interaction, such that the DRD4 VNTR is associated with problematic drinking only in the presence of certain social-environmental factors (specifically, heavy drinking peers and college/Greek involvement). The pathways by which social factors increase risk for problematic drinking among 7-repeat carriers have yet to be articulated. As noted by Park et al. [63], “Specific factors in college environments that interact with the DRD4 gene to increase alcohol dependence in emerging adulthood need to be identified.”

One factor of particular relevance to young adults is the formation of social bonds [64]. To our knowledge, however, no prior study has examined whether effects of alcohol on social bonding may be moderated by DRD4 variation (or any other gene polymorphism). Accordingly, we sought to extend the findings of Larsen et al. [62] and Park et al. [63] to investigate whether experimentally manipulated alcohol consumption would promote social bonding in randomly assigned groups of three unacquainted young adults and would do so differentially among those of differing DRD4 genotype. Each three-person group was assigned to one of three beverage conditions (alcohol, placebo, or non-alcohol control) (i.e., all participants in each group were assigned to the same beverage condition). Within each condition, participants were grouped by presence or absence of the DRD4 7-repeat allele. We hypothesized that alcohol would increase perceived social bonding and that individuals carrying the 7-repeat allele would be especially sensitive to alcohol's effects on social bonding.

Methods

Ethics Statement

Each participant gave informed written consent to take part in this study. All aspects of this research were approved by University of Pittsburgh's Institutional Review Board.

Participants and design

Male and female social drinkers (n =  720) aged 21–28 were recruited via community and university newspaper ads for a parent study of the effects of alcohol on social bonding [65]. A subset of Caucasian participants (n = 422) were genotyped for the DRD4 VNTR. Exclusion criteria included a history of adverse reaction to the type or amount of beverage used in the study, any medical conditions that contraindicated alcohol administration, meeting criteria for past alcohol abuse or dependence, as indexed by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [66], a weight not within 15% of ideal weight for their height [67], illiteracy, pregnancy in females, and smoking 15 or more cigarettes/day (to avoid nicotine withdrawal). Inclusion criteria included drinking a mean of at least two drinks on at least one occasion per 2 weeks, or at least four drinks on at least one occasion per month, over the past year. Participants who consumed alcohol could not drive themselves home from the study.

Study sessions took place on a weekday (Monday-Friday), with the group drinking period beginning at approximately 12 PM. Participants were randomly assigned to groups of three unacquainted persons, and these groups were randomly assigned to drink over 36-min a moderate dose of alcohol, a placebo, or a non-alcoholic control drink (additional details provided below). After drinking, participants were separated, and each completed the Perceived Group Reinforcement Scale (PGRS; described below) and several other measures unrelated to social bonding, which are not reported here.

Genotyping and procedure

Saliva was collected using Oragene kits (DNA Genotek, Ottawa), and genomic DNA was isolated following the manufacturer's protocol. The 48 bp VNTR in Exon 3 of DRD4 was genotyped by the method of Lichter et al. [68], and genotypes were assigned by direct comparison to controls of known genotype. Allele and genotype frequencies are presented in Table 1. Allele frequencies were in Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium (p = .56). Due to the low frequency of individuals homozygous for the 7-repeat allele (2.6%) and in accordance with prior convention (e.g., [62]), participants were classified as 7-present (i.e., homozygous or heterozygous for the 7-repeat allele) or 7-absent (i.e., neither allele is 7-repeat). Most studies examining an association between the DRD4 VNTR and a multitude of disorders and traits, including alcohol-related phenotypes, have assumed that a linear association exists between repeat length and functionality. We rely, however, on data indicating that this is unlikely, with 10 repeats functionally resembling 2 repeats more so than 7 repeats [36][37], [69]. Regardless, there were only 13 individuals (3%) with >7 repeats in our sample, and results were unchanged when using the long/short classification of alleles (i.e., including individuals with repeats >7 in the 7-present classification presented here). As depicted in Table 2, DRD4 genotypes were evenly distributed across beverage conditions, χ2(df = 2, N = 422) = 3.25, p = .20.

thumbnail
Table 2. DRD4 Genotype Distribution Across Beverage Conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028914.t002

Predrink assessment.

Before group formation, participants completed the NEO Five-factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) [70], which reliably assesses five domains of adult personality (neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness), and the Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale (BAES) [71], which includes seven items that assess feelings of stimulation (e.g., energized, excited), and seven that assess feelings of sedation (e.g., heavy head, difficulty concentrating). Several steps were taken to ensure that the groups included 3 unacquainted participants (using methods previously employed in our lab) [17]. An initial blood alcohol content (BAC) breath sample was obtained, and participants completed a subjective intoxication scale (SIS) on which 0 meant not at all intoxicated and 100 meant the most intoxicated I have ever been.

Drink administration.

Group members were informed that they would consume their drinks together before they would complete tasks related to memory and cognitive performance (the ostensible study aim). Participants were told that the group drinking format made it easier to monitor their beverage consumption. All participants in each group drank their beverages seated around a circular table (see [17]). Participants were asked not to mention how intoxicated they might be feeling. To increase credibility in the placebo condition, drinks were mixed in front of participants and the glass was smeared with vodka [72]. The alcoholic beverage was 1 part vodka and 3.5 parts cranberry juice cocktail (Ocean Spray). For those drinking alcohol, the vodka bottle contained 100-proof vodka (Smirnoff); for those drinking a placebo, the vodka bottle contained flattened tonic water (Schweppes). Control participants were told they did not receive alcohol and were given cranberry juice in equal volume. After participants were given one third of the drink [alcohol participants were given one third of a moderate dose of alcohol (0.82 g/kg males/0.74 g/kg females)] and asked to consume it evenly over 12 min, the experimenter exited the room. The experimenter re-entered the room just before the end of each 12-min drinking block (at 12- and 24-min) to give participants the middle and final thirds of the drink. During each pour, participants were asked to consume the beverage evenly over 12-min intervals. Other than briefly entering the room to fill participants' glasses, the experimenter was not present during the group drink period.

Postdrink assessment.

After drinking the final third (36-min), participants were separated and BAC and SIS ratings were recorded. To help control for dosage set, placebo participants received a BAC reading ranging from .041% to .043% (randomly assigned), which is about the highest credible reading for deceived participants (see [72]). This false reading aids in placebo deception [73] (actual BAC readings were also recorded). Participants then completed the Perceived Group Reinforcement Scale (PGRS) to assess the perception of social bonding and the BAES. The PGRS included 12 items, such as “I like this group” and “The members of this group are interested in what I have to say,” which were summarized as a composite score (Cronbach's α = .90). Items were adapted from the Group Attitude Scale [74] and the Perceived Cohesion Scale [75]. The PGRS has good face validity (see Table 3 for the individual items comprising the scale), and it has proven sensitive to the effects of alcohol on social bonding in our prior research. Importantly, the PGRS demonstrates good convergent validity as well, as it correlates with other non-verbal measures of social bonding (see [17]). BAC and SIS were again obtained about 10-min after completing these scales. Placebo participants were presented with a false BAC reading between .039% and .037% and, along with control participants, were debriefed, paid $60, and allowed to leave. Alcohol participants recorded their BACs and ate lunch/relaxed. When their BACs dropped below .025%, they were debriefed, paid $60, and allowed to leave (they were not permitted to drive).

Results

Participants (males = 51.4%) had a mean age of 22.4 years (SD = 1.8). Gender, age, marital status, income, felt stimulation/sedation prior to drinking (as assessed by the BAES), prior drinking patterns, extraversion (along with the other 4 personality dimensions on the NEO-FFI), and smoking status were equivalent across drink conditions. Participants drank on average slightly more than twice a week [M =  3.78 (SD = 0.90) using a 7-point scale with “3” = 1–2 occasions/week and “4” = 2–3 occasions/week] and consumed an average of 4.32 (SD = 1.92) drinks per occasion.

BACs and SIS scores recorded after drinking and after completing the PGRS and BAES across drink conditions appear in Table 4. Mean BAC values indicate alcohol participants were on the ascending limb of the BAC curve with a BAC about .06% when completing the PGRS and BAES. As expected, placebo participants felt significantly more intoxicated than control participants and significantly less intoxicated than alcohol participants.

Statistical Analysis.

Given the hierarchical structure of the data (each individual is nested within one drinking group), it is important to account for the potential interdependence of participants' PGRS responses [76]. The intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC), which assesses the degree of clustering or non-independence of PGRS scores among group members, was calculated to be .23. This ICC value indicates that there is substantial clustering of PGRS scores within groups [77], which violates a key assumption of the statistical model used by ANOVA (i.e., independence of observations). As such, a hierarchical linear model was used to model PGRS scores (a continuous variable) by beverage content condition (three levels: alcohol, placebo, or control) and DRD4 (two levels: 7-present vs. 7-absent) using the SAS PROC MIXED procedure. Because group size was small (n = 3), intercepts but not slopes were allowed to vary randomly across groups [76]. As noted by Kenny and colleagues [78], it is the variation in these intercepts that models the non-independence of groups (pg. 132).

Drink Condition and DRD4 Genotype Effects on Social Bonding

The tests of fixed effects are depicted in Table 5. A main effect of beverage condition on PGRS ratings (p = .001) revealed that alcohol participants reported higher PGRS scores than placebo participants (p = . 0003), but similar scores to control participants (p = .36). In addition, control participants reported higher PGRS scores than placebo participants (p = .008). As expected, there was no main effect of DRD4 on PGRS scores (p = .24). Importantly, there was a significant 3 (BEVERAGE)×2 (DRD4) interaction (p = .022). PGRS scores (M±SE) across beverage conditions and genotypes are shown in Table 6. As depicted in Figure 1, 7-repeat carriers reported higher PGRS ratings in the alcohol condition than in either the placebo (p<.0001) or control conditions (p<.04), whereas alcohol did not significantly affect ratings of 7-absent carriers. The current model explained 3% of level-1 variance and 13% of level-2 variance (represented by proportional reductions in the variance-component residual in comparison with the empty model without explanatory variables; [79]). Table 7 shows the variance components and goodness of fit characteristics associated with each model we estimated. As can be seen, the model with the interaction term included provided a significantly better fit to the data than both the empty model and the model including only main effect estimates.

thumbnail
Figure 1. PGRS Scores (Mean, SE) by DRD4 Genotype and Beverage Condition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028914.g001

thumbnail
Table 5. Tests of Fixed Effects: Results of Hierarchical Linear Modeling.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028914.t005

thumbnail
Table 6. PGRS scores (M±SE) by Beverage Condition and Genotype.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028914.t006

As a supplementary analysis, we collapsed across the two nonalcohol conditions (the placebo and control groups) and modeled PGRS scores by beverage content condition (two levels: alcohol vs. no-alcohol) and DRD4 (two levels: 7-present vs. 7-absent) using the SAS PROC MIXED procedure. Alcohol participants reported higher PGRS scores (M = 7.2, SE = .12) than no-alcohol participants (M = 6.8, SE = .09), (F (1, 203) = 6.75, p = .01). Again, there was no main effect of DRD4 on PGRS scores (p = .8). There was a significant 2 (BEVERAGE)×2 (DRD4) interaction, F (1, 405) = 7.42, p = .007). Consistent with the results above, 7-present individuals reported higher PGRS ratings in the alcohol-consuming condition (M = 7.4, SE = .17) than in the no-alcohol consuming condition (M = 6.6, SE = .14; p = .0006), whereas alcohol consumption did not significantly affect ratings of 7-absent carriers (alcohol; M = 7.0, SE = .15: no-alcohol; M = 7.0, SE = .11; p = .82).

Gender, age, extraversion (along with the other 4 personality dimensions on the NEO-FFI), and drinking history were equivalent across genotypes. Additionally, among those drinking alcohol, there were no differences between 7-present and 7-absent genotypes on BACs, on ratings of subjective intoxication (as assessed by the SIS), and on felt stimulation and sedation (as assessed by the BAES) after alcohol consumption. These results suggest that the findings are unlikely due to systematic differences between the two genotype groups on the above mentioned variables.

Discussion

This study provides initial evidence for a moderating effect of the DRD4 polymorphism on the relationship between alcohol consumption and social bonding. 7-present individuals reported increased perceived social bonding in an unstructured group setting after drinking alcohol, compared to placebo and non-alcohol control beverages. In contrast, alcohol did not affect perceived social bonding of 7-absent individuals. Our findings converge with and extend those of Larsen et al. [62] and of Park et al. [63] suggesting that DRD4 may be linked to the development of problematic drinking partly through the formation of social relationships. Developing interpersonal relationships is a fundamental human motivation [64], and behaviors that support the formation of social bonds are highly rewarding (e.g., [80]). Our results suggest that one possible pathway by which alcohol may become more reinforcing for 7-repeat carriers is by the facilitation of perceived social bonding.

The current study is the first to examine the impact of genetic variation, alcohol consumption, and perceived social bonding among previously unacquainted individuals in a controlled group setting. Because the typical effect size for genetic variation acting on behavioral phenotypes is small [21], large samples are necessary to draw firm conclusions about how certain polymorphisms modulate the experience of alcohol. In one of the largest alcohol administration studies yet conducted, we were able to detect small though potentially meaningful genetic effects. Use of an ecologically valid social drinking context, in which unacquainted young adults consumed alcohol together, increases the generalizability of our results to the natural environment. More generally, this design, which manipulated the environment through random assignment, uniquely allows us to draw causal inferences regarding this gene- environment interaction [81].

Social reward and the reinforcing effects of drugs of abuse, including alcohol, are mediated in part through the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system [32], and recent studies have focused on the role of dopamine in regulating interactions between alcohol and social factors (e.g., [34]). Because 7-repeat carriers may be more sensitive to the dopamine response triggered by priming doses of alcohol and alcohol-related cues [45], [47], [62], they may perceive enhanced social bonding while drinking due to an augmented dopamine response in the brain's reward circuitry. This explanation is generally consistent with prior reports showing that 7-present individuals respond to alcohol consumption with increased craving (e.g., [45]) and respond to positive-feedback with increased reward-related reactivity in the ventral striatum [82] compared to 7-absent individuals.

Consistent with prior studies [45], [47], 7-present individuals did not report feeling more intoxicated nor did they report more stimulation (e.g., elated, energetic, excited) after alcohol consumption, indicating that 7-present individuals did not appear to be generally feeling more of the euphoric effects of alcohol on the ascending limb of absorption than 7-absent individuals. Rather, our results suggest a separate pathway by which alcohol becomes more rewarding for 7-present individuals by increasing their perceived ability to bond with their peers. Future work should examine the relationship between increased stimulation/euphoria and enhanced perception of social bonding more fully, though, as these results are based only on the BAES. Furthermore, we found a statistical trend for a difference in perceived social bonding between 7-repeat carriers and non-carriers within the alcohol condition at this alcohol dose (p = .10) such that, as expected, carriers of the 7-repeat reported increased perceived social bonding compared to 7-absent individuals. Further research is indicated that varies alcohol dose, as higher doses might generate more pronounced effects.

It remains unclear whether 7-repeat carriers possess an actual increased ability to bond with others or if they only perceive their ability to be enhanced. Regardless of this distinction, though, it may be that their perception of increased social bonding is what leads to problematic drinking. Future work is indicated, however, that examines whether 7-repeat carriers are rated as being more sociable by their peers under conditions of alcohol. Research also would be useful to further probe the role of dosage-set, as the present data reveal that placebo participants reported lower PGRS scores than did control participants. This seemingly counterintuitive pattern has been observed for cognitive processes where compensatory mechanisms are implicated [83], but it is unclear how this would apply to our social interaction.

Despite notable strengths, the present study did have limitations. Among these was the fact that the alcohol participants did not differ significantly from the control (no alcohol) participants on the PGRS. This may indicate that a higher dose of alcohol might have been more useful to test. In addition, while the group drinking period started at approximately the same time of day for all participants, we did not control for day of the week, which may have influenced participants' responses. Furthermore, while the distribution of group gender compositions was evenly distributed across the six cells of the experiment, we did not control for this variable and the study was not sufficiently powered to examine its influence on the results. Future studies with even larger samples would permit the examination of potentially interesting three-way interactions including gender and group gender composition as variables. It will also be essential for future studies to test whether carriers of the 7-repeat allele choose to drink more alcohol in social contexts as a result of their perception of enhanced social bonding.

The present findings are preliminary and will need to be replicated. While some argue that genotype-dependent interactions should be the primary focus of alcohol research (e.g. [84]), there is also growing skepticism about the utility of examining gene-environment interactions in the context of addiction and psychopathology. This is mainly due to the fact that some initial, positive gene-environment interaction findings failed to replicate in other samples [85], [86] but see [87], [88]. In general, many of the notable replication difficulties in the literature relate to studies of distal behavioral phenotypes (e.g., depression) and molar environmental moderators (e.g., life events), where layers of methodological variation among studies may yield unstable findings [88]. It is worth noting that, in the case of the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) literature, gene-environment interactions in experimental studies (e.g., effects of transporter variation on amygdala response to experimentally manipulated exposures to emotional stimuli) have fared well in terms of replication (see [89]). Still, although the present study utilizes an experimental design and builds upon an emerging literature highlighting the importance of social factors in the association of DRD4 genotype and drinking outcomes [62], [63], replication is crucial.

In contrast to gene-environment studies focusing on naturally occurring variation in putative environmental moderators and down-stream behavioral phenotypes, studies of genetic influences moderated by experimentally manipulated environmental exposures (as is the case in the present study) have at least two advantages. First, these designs allow for observations to be made under controlled and uniform stimulus conditions, and second, these paradigms better permit causal inferences, because the environmental effect is not subject to contamination by gene-environment correlation [90]. The present study examined a gene-environment interaction in the context of an experimentally manipulated environmental factor, which presumably offers a more powerful tool for identifying gene-environment interactions than do population based studies [81], [90]. A large number of participants received alcohol, and drinking patterns were equivalent across groups. Personality traits thought to relate to social bonding also did not vary across groups. Though such studies raise ethical considerations, it potentially would be valuable to extend these findings in individuals who meet criteria for alcohol use disorders. Additional work that examines other polymorphisms in relation to alcohol-induced bonding and studies that investigate alcohol's effects on non-verbal measures of social-emotional responding throughout a drinking interval also would be useful. Nevertheless, together with other recent findings targeting DRD4, these results suggest that interventions may benefit from focusing on social reward as an important underlying mechanism for the development of problematic drinking in a subset of young adults. More generally, our findings highlight the potential utility of employing transdisciplinary methods that integrate genetic methodologies, social psychology, and addiction theory to improve theories of alcohol use and abuse.

Acknowledgments

We thank Nancy Petro, Catharine Fairbairn, Feifei Ye, and the students and staff at the Alcohol and Smoking Research Laboratory for their assistance.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: KGC MAS SBM SYH REF. Performed the experiments: KGC MAS JDD. Analyzed the data: KGC. Wrote the paper: KGC MAS SBM.

References

  1. 1. Kendler KS, Gardner CO, Prescott CA (2011) Toward a comprehensive developmental model for alcohol use disorders in men. Twin Res Hum Genet 14: 1–15.
  2. 2. Sher KJ, Grekin ER, Williams NA (2005) The Development of Alcohol Use Disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 1: 493–523.
  3. 3. Bachman JG, Johnston LD, O'Malley PM, Schulenberg JE (2006) The Monitoring the Future project after thirty-two years: Follow-up surveys of young adults (Monitoring the Future Occasional Paper No.64). Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research.
  4. 4. Kahler CW, Read JP, Wood MD, Palfai TP (2003) Social environmental selection as a mediator of gender, ethnic, and personality effects on college student drinking. Psychol Addict Behav 17: 226–234.
  5. 5. McGue M, Iacono WG (2004) The Initiation of Substance Use in Adolescence: A Behavioral Genetics Perspective. In: Lisabeth DF, editor. Behavior genetics principles: Perspectives in development, personality, and psychopathology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. pp. 41–57.
  6. 6. Cooper ML (1994) Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: Development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychological Assessment 6: 117–128.
  7. 7. Cooper ML, Frone MR, Russell M, Mudar P (1995) Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: A motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69: 990–1005.
  8. 8. Goldman MS, Brown SA, Christiansen BA (1987) Expectancy theory: Thinking about drinking. In: Blane HT, Leonard KE, editors. Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 181–226.
  9. 9. Park A, Sher KJ, Krull JL (2008) Risky drinking in college changes as fraternity/sorority affiliation changes: a person-environment perspective. Psychol Addict Behav 22: 219–229.
  10. 10. Patrick ME, Schulenberg JE, O'Malley PM, Maggs JL, Kloska DD, et al. (2011) Age-related changes in reasons for using alcohol and marijuana from ages 18 to 30 in a national sample. Psychol Addict Behav 25: 330–339.
  11. 11. Conway KP, Swendsen JD, Merikangas KR (2003) Alcohol expectancies, alcohol consumption, and problem drinking: the moderating role of family history. Addict Behav 28: 823–836.
  12. 12. Engels RCME, Wiers R, Lemmers L, Overbeek GJ (2005) Drinking motives, alcohol expectancies, self-efficacy, and drinking patterns. J Drug Educ 35: 147–166.
  13. 13. Mann LM, Chassin L, Sher KJ (1987) Alcohol expectancies and the risk for alcoholism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 55: 411–417.
  14. 14. Smith GT, Goldman MS, Greenbaum PE, Christiansen BA (1995) Expectancy for social facilitation from drinking: the divergent paths of high-expectancy and low-expectancy adolescents. J Abnorm Psychol 104: 32–40.
  15. 15. Patrick ME, Schulenberg JE, O'Malley PM, Johnston LD, Bachman JG (2011) Adolescents' reported reasons for alcohol and marijuana use as predictors of substance use and problems in adulthood. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 72: 106–116.
  16. 16. Beseler CL, Aharonovich E, Keyes KM, Hasin DS (2008) Adult transition from at-risk drinking to alcohol dependence: the relationship of family history and drinking motives. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 32: 607–616.
  17. 17. Kirchner TR, Sayette MA, Cohn JF, Moreland RL, Levine JM (2006) Effects of alcohol on group formation among male social drinkers. J. Stud. Alcohol 67: 785–793.
  18. 18. Sher KJ, Wood MD (2005) Subjective effects of alcohol II. In: Earleywine M, editor. Mind-altering drugs: The science of subjective experience. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 135–153.
  19. 19. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Rutter M (2006) Gene-environment interplay and psychopathology: multiple varieties but real effects. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 47: 226–261.
  20. 20. Dick DM, Rose RJ, Viken RJ, Kaprio J, Koskenvuo M (2001) Exploring gene-environment interactions: Socioregional moderation of alcohol use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 110: 625–632.
  21. 21. Shanahan MJ, Hofer SM (2005) Social Context in Gene-Environment Interactions: Retrospect and Prospect. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 60: 65–76.
  22. 22. Kendler KS, Gardner C, Dick DM (2011) Predicting alcohol consumption in adolescence from alcohol-specific and general externalizing genetic risk factors, key environmental exposures and their interaction. Psychol Med 41: 1507–1516.
  23. 23. Slutske WS, Hunt-Carter EE, Nabors-Oberg RE, Sher KJ, Bucholz KK, et al. (2004) Do college students drink more than their non-college-attending peers? Evidence from a population-based longitudinal female twin study. J Abnorm Psychol 113: 530–540.
  24. 24. Volkow ND, Li T-K (2004) Drug addiction: the neurobiology of behaviour gone awry. Nat Rev Neurosci 5: 963–970.
  25. 25. Sayette MA (1993) An appraisal-disruption model of alcohol's effects on stress responses in social drinkers. Psychological Bulletin 114: 459–476.
  26. 26. Fromme K, de Wit H, Hutchison KE, Ray L, Corbin WR, Cook TAR, Wall TL, Goldman D (2004) Biological and behaioral markers of alcohol sensitivity. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 28: 247–256.
  27. 27. Doty P, de Wit H (1995) Effect of setting on the reinforcing and subjective effects of ethanol in social drinkers. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 118: 19–27.
  28. 28. de Wit H (2005) Relationships Between Personality and Acute Subjective Responses to Stimulant Drugs. Mind-Altering Drugs. NY: Oxford University Press, Vol. 1. pp. 258–275.
  29. 29. Ducci F, Goldman D (2008) Genetic approaches to addiction: genes and alcohol. Addiction 103: 1414–1428.
  30. 30. Goldman D, Oroszi G, Ducci F (2006) The Genetics of Addictions: Uncovering the Genes. Focus 4: 401–415.
  31. 31. Weiss F, Koob GF (1991) The neuropharmacology of ethanol self-administration. In: Meyer RE, Koob GF, Lewis MJ, Paul SM, editors. Neuropharmacology of ethanol. Boston: Birkhauser. pp. 125–162.
  32. 32. Krach S, Paulus FM, Bodden M, Kircher T (2010) The rewarding nature of social interactions. Front Behav Neurosci 4: 22.
  33. 33. Hill SY, Zezza N, Wipprecht G, Xu J, Neiswanger K (1999) Linkage studies of D2 and D4 receptor genes and alcoholism. Am J Med Genet 88: 676–685.
  34. 34. Young KA, Gobrogge KL, Wang Z (2011) The role of mesocorticolimbic dopamine in regulating interactions between drugs of abuse and social behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35: 498–515.
  35. 35. Van Tol HH, Wu CM, Guan HC, Ohara K, Bunzow JR, et al. (1992) Multiple dopamine D4 receptor variants in the human population. Nature 358: 149–152.
  36. 36. Asghari V, Sanyal S, Buchwaldt S, Paterson A, Jovanovic V, et al. (1995) Modulation of Intracellular Cyclic AMP Levels by Different Human Dopamine D4 Receptor Variants. Journal of Neurochemistry 65: 1157–1165.
  37. 37. Oak JN, Oldenhof J, Van Tol HH (2000) The dopamine D(4) receptor: one decade of research. Eur J Pharmacol 405: 303–327.
  38. 38. Ding Y-C, Chi H-C, Grady DL, Morishima A, Kidd JR, et al. (2002) Evidence of positive selection acting at the human dopamine receptor D4 gene locus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 99: 309–314.
  39. 39. McGeary J (2009) The DRD4 exon 3 VNTR polymorphism and addiction-related phenotypes: a review. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 93: 222–229.
  40. 40. Wang E, Ding Y-C, Flodman P, Kidd JR, Kidd KK, et al. (2004) The Genetic Architecture of Selection at the Human Dopamine Receptor D4 (DRD4) Gene Locus. Am J Hum Genet 74: 931–944.
  41. 41. Laucht M, Hohm E, Esser G, Schmidt MH, Becker K (2007) Association between ADHD and smoking in adolescence: shared genetic, environmental and psychopathological factors. J Neural Transm 114: 1097–1104.
  42. 42. Laucht M, Becker K, El-Faddagh M, Hohm E, Schmidt MH (2005) Association of the DRD4 exon III polymorphism with smoking in fifteen-year-olds: a mediating role for novelty seeking? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 44: 477–484.
  43. 43. Le Foll B, Gallo A, Le Strat Y, Lu L, Gorwood P (2009) Genetics of dopamine receptors and drug addiction: a comprehensive review. Behav Pharmacol 20: 1–17.
  44. 44. Filbey FM, Ray L, Smolen A, Claus ED, Audette A, et al. (2008) Differential neural response to alcohol priming and alcohol taste cues is associated with DRD4 VNTR and OPRM1 genotypes. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 32: 1113–1123.
  45. 45. Hutchison KE, McGeary J, Smolen A, Bryan A, Swift RM (2002) The DRD4 VNTR polymorphism moderates craving after alcohol consumption. Health Psychol 21: 139–146.
  46. 46. Mackillop J, Menges DP, McGeary JE, Lisman SA (2007) Effects of craving and DRD4 VNTR genotype on the relative value of alcohol: an initial human laboratory study. Behav Brain Funct 3: 11.
  47. 47. Ray LA, Miranda R , Tidey JW, McGeary JE, MacKillop J, et al. (2010) Polymorphisms of the mu-opioid receptor and dopamine D4 receptor genes and subjective responses to alcohol in the natural environment. J Abnorm Psychol 119: 115–125.
  48. 48. van den Wildenberg E, Janssen RGJH, Hutchison KE, van Breukelen GJP, Wiers RW (2007) Polymorphisms of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4 VNTR) and cannabinoid CB1 receptor gene (CNR1) are not strongly related to cue-reactivity after alcohol exposure. Addict Biol 12: 210–220.
  49. 49. Pérez de Castro I, Ibáñez A, Torres P, Sáiz-Ruiz J, Fernández-Piqueras J (1997) Genetic association study between pathological gambling and a functional DNA polymorphism at the D4 receptor gene. Pharmacogenetics 7: 345–348.
  50. 50. Comings DE, Gade-Andavolu R, Gonzalez N, Wu S, Muhleman D, et al. (2001) The additive effect of neurotransmitter genes in pathological gambling. Clinical Genetics 60: 107–116.
  51. 51. Kuhnen CM, Chiao JY (2009) Genetic Determinants of Financial Risk Taking. PLoS ONE 4: e4362.
  52. 52. Eisenegger C, Knoch D, Ebstein RP, Gianotti LRR, Sándor PS, et al. (2010) Dopamine receptor D4 polymorphism predicts the effect of L-DOPA on gambling behavior. Biol Psychiatry 67: 702–706.
  53. 53. Dreber A, Apicella CL, Eisenberg DTA, Garcia JR, Zamore RS, et al. (2009) The 7R polymorphism in the dopamine receptor D4 gene (DRD4) is associated with financial risk taking in men. Evolution and Human Behavior 30: 85–92.
  54. 54. Congdon E, Lesch KP, Canli T (2008) Analysis of DRD4 and DAT polymorphisms and behavioral inhibition in healthy adults: implications for impulsivity. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 147B: 27–32.
  55. 55. Zhong S, Israel S, Shalev I, Xue H, Ebstein RP, et al. (2010) Dopamine D4 Receptor Gene Associated with Fairness Preference in Ultimatum Game. PLoS ONE 5: e13765.
  56. 56. Eisenberg DTA, Apicella CL, Campbell BC, Dreber A, Garcia JR(June) Assortative human pair-bonding for partner ancestry and allelic variation of the dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) gene. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5: 194–202.
  57. 57. Garcia JR, MacKillop J, Aller EL, Merriwether AM, Wilson DS, et al. (2010) Associations between Dopamine D4 Receptor Gene Variation with Both Infidelity and Sexual Promiscuity. PLoS ONE 5: e14162.
  58. 58. Faraone SV, Doyle AE, Mick E, Biederman J (2001) Meta-Analysis of the Association Between the 7-Repeat Allele of the Dopamine D4 Receptor Gene and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Am J Psychiatry 158: 1052–1057.
  59. 59. Li D, Sham PC, Owen MJ, He L (2006) Meta-analysis shows significant association between dopamine system genes and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Hum Mol Genet 15: 2276–2284.
  60. 60. Gizer IR, Ficks C, Waldman ID (2009) Candidate gene studies of ADHD: a meta-analytic review. Hum Genet 126: 51–90.
  61. 61. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijzendoorn MH (2011) Differential susceptibility to rearing environment depending on dopamine-related genes: New evidence and a meta-analysis. Dev Psychopathol 23: 39–52.
  62. 62. Larsen H, van der Zwaluw CS, Overbeek G, Granic I, Franke B, et al. (2010) A variable-number-of-tandem-repeats polymorphism in the dopamine D4 receptor gene affects social adaptation of alcohol use: investigation of a gene-environment interaction. Psychol Sci 21: 1064–1068.
  63. 63. Park A, Sher KJ, Todorov AA, Heath AC (2011) Interaction between the DRD4 VNTR polymorphism and proximal and distal environments in alcohol dependence during emerging and young adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 120: 585–595.
  64. 64. Baumeister RF, Leary MR (1995) The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin 117: 497–529.
  65. 65. Sayette MA, Creswell KG, Dimoff JD, Fairbairn CE, Cohn JF, Heckman BW, et al. Alcohol and group formation: A multimodal investigation of the effects of alcohol on emotion and social bonding. Psychological Science. (in press).
  66. 66. American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: Author.
  67. 67. Harrison GG (1985) Height-weight tables. Ann Intern Med 103: 989–994.
  68. 68. Lichter JB, Barr CL, Kennedy JL, Van Tol HH, Kidd KK, et al. (1993) A hypervariable segment in the human dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) gene. Hum Mol Genet 2: 767–773.
  69. 69. Jovanovic V, Guan HC, Van Tol HH (1999) Comparative pharmacological and functional analysis of the human dopamine D4.2 and D4.10 receptor variants. Pharmacogenetics 9: 561–568.
  70. 70. Costa PT, McCrae RR (1992) Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  71. 71. Martin CS, Earleywine M, Musty RE, Perrine MW, Swift RM (1993) Development and validation of the Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 17: 140–146.
  72. 72. Martin CS, Sayette MA (1993) Experimental design in alcohol administration research: limitations and alternatives in the manipulation of dosage-set. J Stud Alcohol 54: 750–761.
  73. 73. Rohsenow DJ, Marlatt GA (1981) The balanced placebo design: Methodological considerations. Addictive Behaviors 6: 107–122.
  74. 74. Evans NJ, Jarvis PA (1986) The Group Attitude Scale. Small Group Research 17: 203–216.
  75. 75. Bollen KA, Hoyle RH (1990) Perceived Cohesion: A Conceptual and Empirical Examination. Social Forces 69: 479–504.
  76. 76. Kashy DA, Kenny DA (2000) The analysis of data from dyads and groups. In: Reis HT, Judd CM, editors. Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. pp. 451–477.
  77. 77. Singer JD (1998) Using SAS PROC MIXED to fit multilevel models, hierarchical models, and individual growth models. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics 24: 323–355.
  78. 78. Kenny DA, Mannetti L, Pierro A, Livi S, Kashy DA (2002) The statistical analysis of data from small groups. J Pers Soc Psychol 83: 126–137.
  79. 79. Singer JD, Willett JB (2003) Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis: Modeling Change and Event Occurrence. Oxford: University Press.
  80. 80. Shore DM, Heerey EA (2011) The value of genuine and polite smiles. Emotion 11: 169–174.
  81. 81. Rutter M, Pickles A, Murray R, Eaves L (2001) Testing hypotheses on specific environmental causal effects on behavior. Psychol Bull 127: 291–324.
  82. 82. Forbes EE, Brown SM, Kimak M, Ferrell RE, Manuck SB, et al. (2009) Genetic variation in components of dopamine neurotransmission impacts ventral striatal reactivity associated with impulsivity. Mol Psychiatry 14: 60–70.
  83. 83. Vogul-Sprott M, Fillmore MT (1999) Learning theory and research. In: Leonard K, Blane HT, editors. Psychological Theories of Drinking and Alcoholism (2nd ed) New York: Guilford Press.
  84. 84. Heath AC, Nelson EC (2002) Effects of the interaction between genotype and environment: Research into the genetic epidemiology of alcohol dependence. Alcohol Research & Health 26: 193–201.
  85. 85. Munafo MR, Durrant C, Lewis G, Flint J (2009) Gene environment interactions at the serotonin transporter locus. Biological Psychiatry 65: 211–219.
  86. 86. Risch N, Herrell R, Lehner T, Liang K, Eaves L, Hoh J, et al. (2009) Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), stressful life events, and risk of depression: A meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association 301: 2462–2471.
  87. 87. Karg K, Burmeister M, Shedden K, Sen S (2011) The serotonin transporter promoter variant (5-HTTLPR), stress, and depression meta-analysis revisited: Evidence of genetic moderation. Arch Gen Psychiatry 68: 444–454.
  88. 88. Monroe SM, Reid MW (2008) Gene-environment interactions in depression research: Genetic polymorphisms and life-stress polyprocedures. Psych Science 19: 947–956.
  89. 89. Munafò MR, Brown SM, Hariri AR (2008) Serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) genotype and amygdala activation: A meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry 63: 852–857.
  90. 90. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Rutter (2006) Measured gene-environment interactions in psychopathology: Concepts, research strategies, and implications for research, intervention, and public understanding of genetics. Perspectives on Psych Science 1: 5–27.