Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Parental Age and Lifespan Influence Offspring Recruitment: A Long-Term Study in a Seabird

Parental Age and Lifespan Influence Offspring Recruitment: A Long-Term Study in a Seabird

  • Roxana Torres, 
  • Hugh Drummond, 
  • Alberto Velando
PLOS
x

Abstract

Recent studies of wild populations provide compelling evidence that survival and reproduction decrease with age because of senescence, a decline in functional capacities at old ages. However, in the wild, little is known about effects of parental senescence on offspring quality. We used data from a 21-year study to examine the role of parental age on offspring probability of recruitment in a long-lived bird, the blue-footed booby (Sula nebouxii). Offspring probability of recruiting into the breeding population varied over the life of parents and effects age were similar in mothers and fathers. Offspring recruitment was high when parents were roughly 6–12 years old and low before and after then. Effects of parental age on offspring recruitment varied with lifespan (parental age at last reproduction) and previous breeding experience. Offspring recruitment from young and old parents with long reproductive lifespans was greater than that of offspring from parents with short lifespans at young and old ages. For parents with little previous breeding experience recruitment of offspring decreased with their hatch date, but experienced parents were no similarly affected. We found evidence of terminal effects on offspring recruitment in young parents but not in older parents, suggesting that senescence is more likely a gradual process of deterioration than a process of terminal illness. Failure to recruit probably reflects mortality during the first years after independence but also during the fledgling transition to full independence. Our results show effects of parental age and quality on offspring viability in a long-lived wild vertebrate and support the idea that wild populations are composed of individuals of different quality, and that this individual heterogeneity can influence the dynamics of age-structured populations.

Introduction

Parents can influence offspring phenotype through genetic and non-genetic effects, the latter resulting from factors other than simple nuclear DNA [1]. Understanding evolution by natural selection requires understanding both genetic and environmental influences of parents on offspring fitness [2], [3]. In iteroparous animals reproductive performance generally varies with age [4], [5], suggesting age-dependent parental effects. These effects can affect variation in the force of natural selection across the lifespan whenever different age-classes contribute differentially to the gene pool [6][8].

In natural populations, effects of parental age on the number of offspring reared to independence are well documented [9][12]. Typically, young and older parents produce fewer offspring than middle-age parents. However, determining only the number of offspring produced could be insufficient to estimate the magnitude of parental age effects if, for example, offspring quality (i.e. offspring survival and reproduction after independence) is also affected by parental age [13]. Some studies in the laboratory suggest that parental age at conception can influence offspring survival after independence and life history trajectory [14], [15]. For example, in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster, parental age, particularly the age of the mother, influences offspring longevity and the shape of the curve of age-specific mortality: older mothers generally produce shorter-lived offspring, although the exact effect of maternal age on offspring longevity depends on initial genetic heterogeneity among parents [16], [17]. However, the generality and overall importance to other systems of parental age effects demonstrated in laboratory organisms has been questioned [18], [19].

Studies of parental age effects on offspring quality in wild populations have only recently been carried out and results are mixed. In the red squirrel, Tamiasciusus hudsonicus, offspring survival from weaning to one year of age declined with increasing mother age [20], and in the great tit, Parus major, proportion of young that recruited to the breeding population decreased with female age [21]. Furthermore, great tit females hatched from older mothers displayed a stronger rate of reproductive senescence in the number of hatchlings and recruits produced than females hatched from younger mothers; nonetheless, reproductive lifespan and lifetime reproductive success (total number of recruits produced) were unaffected by maternal age [22]. In contrast, in the red-billed chough, Pyrrhocorax pyrrhocorax, parental age did not influence fledglings first-year survival [13]. Hence, in wild vertebrates effects of parental age on offspring performance after independence are yet poorly understood, probably because long-term monitoring of large populations is required.

Intrinsic parental factors may influence offspring during early development or later in life, such as parental germline defects [23][24] or egg maternal effects [17], [25]. In birds, age affects germline quality [25][27], with potential consequences for offspring development and survival [28], [29]. Old parents may also provide less or poorer parental care [25]. Furthermore, previous reproductive effort could positively or negatively influence the number and quality of offspring produced. In young animals, previous effort could positively influence breeding performance [30][32], because experienced parents frequently have greater skills and better quality mates or territories [33][36]. However, previous reproductive effort can increase somatic deterioration, reducing parental performance, particularly of older animals [37].

Individual heterogeneity may strongly impact age-reproductive patterns in animals. Recent studies suggest that populations are composed of individuals of different quality [30], [38]. Thus, population-level effects of parental age might result from selective disappearance (or appearance) of certain phenotypes, rather than within-individual processes, such as senescence [13], [39][41]. Selective disappearance might occur, for example, when differential mortality of individuals that invest heavily in reproduction leads to progressive disappearance of good reproducers [40]. Detecting parental age effects may be difficult when there is individual heterogeneity because senescence patterns may vary between individuals of different quality [40]. Lastly, effects of parental age may be confounded or obscured by terminal effects, when individuals close to death are sick or in poor condition (terminal illness[42][44]), or increase effort on their last reproductive event (terminal investment [45], [46]).

Here, we used data from a 21-year study to examine effects of parental age on offspring recruitment in the blue-footed booby, Sula nebouxii, an iteroparous long-lived tropical seabird with lengthy biparental care (up to six months; [47], [48]). In birds, recruitment has been considered a major component of offspring quality [13], and in the blue-footed booby the probability of recruitment varies considerably [49], [50], accounting for substantial variation in individual fitness. Also, age of male and female boobies influence reproductive success [25], [51], [52], with steady increase in the number of fledglings produced until roughly the tenth year of age, followed by progressive decline [51][53]. Development and survival of booby chicks before independence are affected by the amount of incubation, defence and food provided by both parents [54], [55]. In this study we examined the associations between parental age and probability of fledglings' recruitment to address four questions: (1) Does parental age influence the probability of recruitment of offspring? (2) Are parental age effects (if present) due to changes over individual lifetimes (i.e. within-individual heterogeneity) or differences in quality of individual parents (i.e. between-individual effects)? (3) Are parental effects related to previous reproductive effort? 4) Does offspring recruitment differ when parents are in their last reproductive event?

Materials and Methods

Ethics statement

The work met the Mexican legal requirements about animal welfare and long-term field work was annually supervised and approved by Dirección General de Vida Silvestre, Secretaría de Gestión para la Protección Ambiental (SEMANART permit numbers 517, 574, 5664, 10470, SGPA/DGVS/01323, SGPA/DGVS/3152, SGPA/DGVS/1543, SGPA/DGVS/0491, SGPA/DGVS/1547, SGPA/DGVS/10832, SGPA/DGVS/01916, SGPA/DGVS/00733, SGPA/DGVS/00357, SGPA/DGVS/00505, SGPA/DGVS/00091).

Study area

The study was carried out in the breeding colony of blue-footed boobies at Isla Isabel, Nayarit (21°52′N, 105°54′W), off the Pacific coast of Mexico. To evaluate the effects of parental age and previous reproductions (number of previous reproductive attempts) over the lifetime on recruitment of fledglings to the breeding colony, we used longitudinal data on the reproduction and offspring of two cohorts of fledglings (1988 and 1989) in a long-term study.

Long-term study

In every breeding season the reproductive performance of all breeders in two study areas (20,800 m2 and 6,089 m2, roughly 400 m apart) was recorded. In each nest laying, hatching and survival of eggs and chicks were registered every 3 days until most chicks reached age 30 days and thereafter every 6 days until age 70 d (at this age, males and females have reached between 98–100% of their skeletal growth, [49]). Chicks that hatched before the start of monitoring were aged by bill and ulna length [49]. Chicks were individually marked within 3 d of hatching with coloured leg bands, which were replaced by numbered plastic bands at age 7–10 days and by numbered steel bands at age 70 days. The band numbers of breeding birds were recorded and confirmed by independent readings on up to three days. Reproduction was monitored during roughly 5 months of every year, until the end of the colony's fledging period.

Blue-footed boobies are highly philopatric to their natal neighbourhoods (median natal dispersal distance: males, 24.1, females, 28.3 m) and show long term fidelity to their first breeding site. Since they apparently disperse from the study colony only rarely [56], [57], [58], the long-term study protocol allows us to record reproductive histories of nearly all recruits from the study areas. Survival of chicks was recorded until nearly the end of the growing period (70 d) but not until complete independence from parents. Blue-footed booby plumage development is completed at roughly 90 d, when fledglings are able to fly [48]. As in many species of seabirds, fledglings go through a transition period where parents feed them for approximately another 30 to 40 days whereas they develop foraging skills [47]. Hence, failure to recruit to the natal breeding population could occur because offspring died soon after we last registered them at age 70 d, subsequently during the transition to independence or during the first years of independent life. Additionally, some offspring could reach sexual maturity but fail to reproduce or disperse to another colony. We have no measure of how many offspring simply failed to reproduce, but dispersal from our study colony to other breeding colonies is low [49], [56], [57], [58]. During the growth period 52% of hatched chicks died but more than 95% of this mortality occurs before age 30 days (García Cerecedo MA, Saavedra Sordo MT and Drummond H, unpublished data), and fledglings are rarely found dead in the colony. In the year (1989) when we registered survival until plumage was completed only five out of a total of 1000 chicks died at age between 70 and 92 d. Thus, mortality during transition to independence and the first years after independence probably accounts for most of offspring failure to recruit.

Statistical analyses

The probability of recruitment during the first 6 years of life of fledglings from parents of cohorts 1988 and 1989 was analyzed. In this species, females breed for the first time at age two to eight (3.85±0.08) years and males at age one to ten (4.32±0.09) years, and more than 95% of male and female recruits bred for the first time during the first 6 years of life [49]. Individuals that were not recorded breeding during their first 6 years were assumed to have failed to recruit. In our study, most fledglings analyzed had a single parent from cohorts 1988 and 1989. Nevertheless, 215 out of 1601 fledglings had both parents in either cohort 1988 or cohort 1989. To avoid repetition of data, in these cases only a single parent, selected at random, was included in the analyses. If assortative mating by age occurs in a population, a similar pattern of aging effects in both sexes could arise from senescence in only one sex. However, this does not seem to be the case in the studied population, in a breeding season no correlation between ages of mates was found [53].

The linear and quadratic effects of parental age on fledgling recruitment, including the identity of the parent as a random factor, were analysed using Generalized Linear Mixed Models (GLMMs) with Binomial error distribution, a logit link function (GLIMMIX) [59] and the Satterthwaite approximation for the denominator degrees of freedom [60]. We included parental sex and its interactions with the linear and quadratic effects of parental age to test for differences between maternal age and paternal age on the probability of offspring recruitment. To assess whether decline in the probability of offspring recruitment at older parental ages was significant, General Additive Models (GAMs) were used to determine parental age at onset of senescence (the predicted peak by the GAM). The GAM models were fitted with binomial error distribution, a logit link function, and spline smoothers selected by generalized cross validation (GAM procedure in SAS) [59]. Then, a GLMM (as described above, but excluding the quadratic effects) was adjusted to data from parental age at the onset of senescence (as predicted by the GAM) onward.

To probe the potential mechanisms underlying observed patterns of senescence [39], we performed an additional GLMM as described above but which included as covariates: recruiting age of parent, parental age at last observed reproduction (an estimation of the individual reproductive lifespan), number of previous breeding attempts by parent, fledgling's hatch date (number of days between laying of the egg the fledgling hatched from and the laying of the earliest clutch in the same year) to test for effects of early conditions on offspring recruitment, parental cohort (1988, 1989) and the annual proportion of recruitment from each cohort included in our analyses (1991 to 2003) to control for differences in recruitment due to annual variations. Parental age at last reproduction was analysed up to 2005, i.e. maximum ages 17 and 16 for individuals from cohorts 1988 and 1989, respectively. This is an appropriate estimation of age at last reproduction because roughly only 5% of individuals are still alive at ages 17 [53]. Parental age at last reproduction was included in the model to account for selective disappearance from the breeding population and differences in senescence patterns between parents of different lifespan [39]. To evaluate the potential of terminal effects, we included a two-level variable for parents' last reproductive event (last or not). The identity of the parent was included as random effect. Initially all explanatory variables and their two-way interactions, and the quadratic terms for parental age, parental age at last reproduction and previous breeding attempts were fitted in the model. To test for difference in the effects of maternal and paternal age on offspring recruitment, two and three-way interactions with sex were also included. Nonsignificant terms were dropped sequentially starting with three-way interactions until the minimal adequate model was obtained. Interactions between parental age (linear and quadratic) and parental age at last reproduction tested whether age effects differ between birds of different lifespan while the interactions between parental age and parents' last reproductive event tested the age-specific effect of terminal effects. Statistical significance of random effects was assessed using the restricted likelihood ratio test as the change in −2 log-likelihood of the model with and without the individual effect. This difference is distributed as χ2 [60].

Results

Parent age had a significant quadratic effect on the probability of offspring recruitment (GLMM, parental age, F1,1601 = 33.47, P<0.0001; parental age2, F1,1601 = 31.18, P<0.0001; Figure 1), and there was no evidence that maternal and paternal age effects differ (parental age*sex, F1,1598 = 0.11, P = 0.74, parental age2*sex, F1,1600 = 0.02, P = 0.89). The age spline in GAM was also significant (P = 0.0001), revealing that fledgling recruitment increased steadily up to parent age of five years, followed by a plateau and an abrupt decline after parent age of 12 years. In the analysis of parents of age 12 and older, there was a significant negative linear effect of parent age on recruitment (estimate, −1.20, F1,152 = 13.81, P<0.001).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Parental age effects on fledgling recruitment.

Circles represent the proportion of fledglings that recruited from each parental age class. Numbers next to circles are sample sizes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027245.g001

The additional model to test potential mechanism (see methods) confirmed a quadratic effect of parental age on offspring recruitment (Table 1). In this model, the interaction of parental age2 and parental age of last reproduction (parental longevity) on fledgling recruitment was significant (Table 1). Thus, fledglings from long-lived parents had higher recruitment probability and a lesser quadratic effect of parental age compared to fledglings from shorter-lived middle aged parents (Figure 2).

thumbnail
Figure 2. Effect of age of last reproduction (PALR) and age of parents on fledgling recruitment.

Estimated surface was calculated from the minimal adequate model reported in table 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027245.g002

thumbnail
Table 1. Effect of parental age and life history traits on fledgling recruitment during the first 6 years of life.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027245.t001

We also found evidence of terminal effects, especially in offspring of young parents (Parental last reproduction*Parental age, Table 1; Figure 3). Fledglings from young parents (<10 years) in a terminal reproduction had lower recruitment probability compared to fledglings from non-terminal parents (Figure 3b), but this effect was not found in old parents (Figure 3b). Also, the interaction of number of parental previous breeding attempts and fledgling hatch date was significant (Table 1). The probability of fledgling recruitment decreased with fledgling hatch date for parents with low experience, but not for high experienced parents (Figure 4). There was no evidence that maternal and paternal age effects differ (sex and its second- and third-order interactions with main effects, P>0.11). Offspring recruitment covaried with cohort recruitment from population (Table 1), but the effects of parental cohort and parent recruiting age were not significant (P>0.63).

thumbnail
Figure 3. Effect of parental last reproduction (terminal event) on the relationship between parental age and fledgling recruitment.

a) Estimated curves calculated from the minimal adequate model reported in table 1. b) Proportion of fledglings that recruited from total fledglings produced (numbers of total fledglings above bars) by age classes from parents in their last reproduction (black bars) or in a non-terminal event (white bars).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027245.g003

thumbnail
Figure 4. Effect of hatching date and parents' previous breeding experience on fledgling recruitment.

Estimated surface was calculated from the minimal adequate model reported in table 1. Hatch date was expressed as the difference in days between the laying date of the egg the fledgling was born from and the laying date of the earliest clutch in the same year.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027245.g004

Discussion

We found that offspring recruitment probability varies over the life of parents but, interestingly, these effects were modulated by parental life-history, suggesting individual heterogeneity in parental effects. We did not detect any differences between mothers and fathers in the effects of parental age on the probability of offspring recruitment. Overall, fledgling recruitment was highest when either parent was roughly 6 to 12 years old, while younger and older parents produced fledglings less likely to recruit. The decline in recruitment of fledglings produced by senescent parents was confirmed by a posteriori analysis on the data subset after age 12 years, the parental onset of senescence. In the blue-footed booby reproductive success varies with age, and males and females between roughly 8 to 10 years of age produce more fledglings than younger or older parents [25], [51], [53]. Hence, assuming recruits of different aged parents enjoyed similar reproductive success, our long term study suggests that some age classes contribute significantly more to the gene pool than others by producing more fledglings with higher probability of recruiting to the breeding population, thus affecting the strength of selection across parental life.

Effects of parental age on offspring recruitment varied with parental age at last reproduction, an estimation of the parent's reproductive lifespan. Probability of recruitment of offspring from parents of shorter reproductive lifespan was low at young parental ages increasing up to the age of roughly 8 years and then decreasing. In contrast, probability of recruitment of offspring from parents of longer reproductive lifespan was relatively high at young parental ages, increased slightly up to the age of roughly 8 to 9 years, then declined slightly with advancing age. The recruitment enhancement until parental age 8 years may be attributed to the combined effects of individual improvement and selective disappearance of low quality individuals. For parents that differ in their lifespan, reports of age-dependent variation in reproductive traits underlying recruitment have been mixed [11], [21], [38]. Parents' reproductive lifespan influenced offspring birth weight, but not calving date in red deer [11], improved laying date and clutch size in mute swans [38], but did not influence clutch size, hatching success or fledging success in great tits [21]. Similar to what we found, reproductive lifespan of female great tits was positively correlated to annual recruit production, however, contrary to our study, lifespan was not related to senescence in recruitment success [21]. In the blue-footed booby, age at last reproduction of males and females was unrelated to age-dependent variation in laying date, brood size, and breeding success [53]. Nevertheless, we found that parental senescence in offspring recruitment varied with parental lifespan (i.e. between individual heterogeneity). Importantly, the effects of parental age on offspring recruitment were not related to selective disappearance of individuals that invested heavily early in the life. On the contrary, we found that parents with long lifespans produce offspring with higher recruitment probability than those produced by parents with short lifespans. Thus, despite parents of different reproductive lifespans made a similar breeding effort up to fledging [53], parents with long lifespans produced offspring more likely to recruit at younger and older parental ages, compared to parents with short lifespans. Additionally, senescence patterns (onset and slope) in offspring recruitment were more pronounced in parents with intermediate lifespans than parents with long lifespans. Thus, parental quality seems to affect offspring recruitment throughout parental life. Overall, these results suggest that age effects on the probability of offspring recruitment can be attributed to changes within individual parents over their lifetimes (improvement followed by senescence), but largely determined by individual quality (reflected by reproductive lifespan).

In our study, terminal illness, was found in short-lived but not on long-lived parents (i.e. age-dependent), suggesting also heterogeneity in relation to lifespan. Thus, results suggest that for younger birds, terminal illness probably account for their lower breeding success. Fledglings from young parents (<10 years) in a terminal reproduction had lower recruitment compared to fledglings from non-terminal parents, indicating abrupt phenotypic deterioration in the last year of the life. For older parents, the adjusted curves suggest greater investment during a terminal event than in a non-terminal event, but the difference was not significant. Our results suggest that a gradual age-dependent senescence in long-lived individuals, rather than an abrupt decline in performance in their last reproduction. A steep decrease in reproduction in late-life has been found in several bird species [42], [43]. Here, we found that terminal illness may occur in some age classes but not in others, highlighting that terminal effect may be affected by individual quality. Short-lived parents are probably more constrained, due to their physiological condition or their experience/skills, than long-lived parents.

In the blue-footed booby, the probability of recruitment decreases with laying date [49] and old males and females tend to lay late in the season [53]. In birds, recruitment varies among cohorts [61], [62] and within a cohort, early fledglings are often raised in good breeding conditions [63][65]; and early incorporation to the breeding population confers advantages throughout life [66][68]. Interestingly, in the present study besides the effects of parental age, recruitment decreased with fledgling hatch date for parents with low experience, but not for those with high experience. More experienced parents, those with more than eight breeding events, were able to produce high-quality offspring with high probability of recruiting into the breeding population even late in the season, when environmental conditions are poor. Furthermore, more experienced parents are also parents with longer reproductive lifespans suggesting that individual quality and breeding experience may have additive effects on offspring recruitment. These results highlight how parental effects such as breeding experience can modify chick rearing conditions and overcome the negative effects of late hatching.

We found no effect of parental age at first reproduction, a life history trait that is expected to influence senescence, on the recruitment of offspring from parents of different ages. In the blue-footed booby there is evidence that early onset of a male's first reproduction accelerates, to some extent, reproductive senescence and decreases lifespan [53]. Nevertheless, early onset of reproduction by males and females did not prejudice the recruitment of the offspring they produced at advanced ages, as found in studies of reproductive senescence in the red deer and the red-billed chough [11], [13].

The mechanisms underlying differences in the probability of recruitment of offspring from young and old parents are probably different, but depend on other life history traits and individual heterogeneity. Low recruitment of fledglings from younger parents is probably due to constraint or restraint of inexperienced breeders [33], [69][71]. Old parents may also restrain their reproduction in accordance with their individual levels of somatic damage to increase the number of future reproductive events [72]. The fact that variation in recruitment of offspring from younger and older parents was related to individual reproductive lifespan suggests that age-intrinsic factors such as egg quality, rearing capacity [25], and germline deterioration [17], [28], [73][75] differ among individuals of different age and quality. In the blue-footed booby older mothers produce smaller eggs and provide lower quality care [25]. Furthermore, older males carry greater loads of premutagenic DNA damages in the sperm [27]. Hence, progeny of blue-footed booby senescent males are probably at higher risk of genetic disorders. Further work will need to investigate the possible physiological and epigenetic effects associated with age and reproductive lifespan and its transmission across generations.

In conclusion, in this long-lived wild vertebrate parental aging appears to affect the viability of offspring. The diverse effects of aging on reproductive performance recently demonstrated in wild populations [10], [40] oblige us to acknowledge the importance of senescence in nature [37], [76], [77]. Our results suggest that senescence is not the result of terminal illness but a gradual process, although short lived parents showed deterioration (as revealed by offspring recruitment) in their last reproductive event. Parental individual heterogeneity influenced recruitment success, with long-lived (i.e. high-quality) breeders performing better than short-lived breeders at younger ages and suffering from lower rates of senescence. Hence, intrinsic-individual factors and probably other life-history traits influence the rate of senescence. Here, we showed that long-lived individuals contributed considerably more to the next generation. Our study highlights that effects of parental age and parental quality are evolutionarily important and should be further incorporated into theoretical models of the evolution of senescence and the dynamics of age-structured populations (see also [78]). Further studies should address which proximate mechanisms are involved in variation of offspring recruitment with parental quality and age.

Acknowledgments

We thank C. Rodríguez and J. L. Osorno, and numerous volunteers who made indispensable contributions to recording and managing the longitudinal data. We wish to thank M.L. Leonard and two anonymous referees for their constructive revision and comments to this article. Permissions for fieldwork were granted by Secretaría del Medio Ambiente y Recursos Naturales, and logistical support was provided by the Armada de México, the staff of the Parque Nacional Isla Isabel, and many fishermen of San Blas and Camichin.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: RT AV. Performed the experiments: HD RT. Analyzed the data: AV RT. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: HD RT AV. Wrote the paper: RT AV.

References

  1. 1. Mousseau TA, Fox C (1998) Maternal effects as adaptations. Oxford: Oxford University Press. TA MousseauC. Fox1998Maternal effects as adaptationsOxfordOxford University Press
  2. 2. Lynch M, Walsh B (1998) Genetics and analysis of quantitative traits. Sunderland: Sinauer. M. LynchB. Walsh1998Genetics and analysis of quantitative traitsSunderlandSinauer
  3. 3. West-Eberhard MJ (2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press. MJ West-Eberhard2003Developmental plasticity and evolutionOxfordOxford University Press
  4. 4. Clutton-Brock TH (1984) Reproductive effort and terminal investment in iteroparous animals. Am Nat 123: 212–229.TH Clutton-Brock1984Reproductive effort and terminal investment in iteroparous animals.Am Nat123212229
  5. 5. Bennett PM, Owens IPF (2002) Evolutionary Ecology of Birds: Life histories, mating systems and extinction. Oxford: Oxford University Press. PM BennettIPF Owens2002Evolutionary Ecology of Birds: Life histories, mating systems and extinctionOxfordOxford University Press
  6. 6. Hamilton WD (1966) Molding of senescence by natural selection. J Theor Biol 12: 12–45.WD Hamilton1966Molding of senescence by natural selection.J Theor Biol121245
  7. 7. Charlesworth B (1994) Evolution in age-structured populations. 2nd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. B. Charlesworth1994Evolution in age-structured populations. 2nd editionCambridgeCambridge University Press
  8. 8. Baudisch A (2005) Hamilton's indicators of the force of selection. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102: 8263–8268.A. Baudisch2005Hamilton's indicators of the force of selection.Proc Natl Acad Sci USA10282638268
  9. 9. Catry P, Phillips RA, Phalan B, Croxall JP (2006) Senescence effects in an extremely long-lived bird: the grey-headed albatross Thallassarche chrysostoma. Proc R Soc B 273: 1625–1630.P. CatryRA PhillipsB. PhalanJP Croxall2006Senescence effects in an extremely long-lived bird: the grey-headed albatross Thallassarche chrysostoma.Proc R Soc B27316251630
  10. 10. Jones OR, Gaillard JM, Tuljapurkar S, Alho JS, Armitage KB, et al. (2008) Senescence rates are determined by ranking on the fast-slow life-history continuum. Ecol Lett 11: 664–673.OR JonesJM GaillardS. TuljapurkarJS AlhoKB Armitage2008Senescence rates are determined by ranking on the fast-slow life-history continuum.Ecol Lett11664673
  11. 11. Nussey DH, Kruuk LEB, Donald A, Fowlie M, Clutton-Brock TH (2006) The rate of senescence in maternal performance increases with early-life fecundity in red deer. Ecol Lett 9: 1342–1350.DH NusseyLEB KruukA. DonaldM. FowlieTH Clutton-Brock2006The rate of senescence in maternal performance increases with early-life fecundity in red deer.Ecol Lett913421350
  12. 12. Sharp SP, Clutton-Brock TH (2010) Reproductive senescence in a cooperatively breeding mammal. J Anim Ecol 79: 176–183.SP SharpTH Clutton-Brock2010Reproductive senescence in a cooperatively breeding mammal.J Anim Ecol79176183
  13. 13. Reid JM, Bignal EM, Bignal S, McCracken DI, Bogdanova MI, et al. (2010) Parent age, lifespan and offspring survival: structured variation in life history in a wild population. J Anim Ecol 79: 851–862.JM ReidEM BignalS. BignalDI McCrackenMI Bogdanova2010Parent age, lifespan and offspring survival: structured variation in life history in a wild population.J Anim Ecol79851862
  14. 14. Nikola T, Darka E, Vesna S (2004) The short-term and long-term effects of parental age in the bean weevil (Acanthoscelides obtectus). Evol Ecol 18: 187–201.T. NikolaE. DarkaS. Vesna2004The short-term and long-term effects of parental age in the bean weevil (Acanthoscelides obtectus).Evol Ecol18187201
  15. 15. Yılmaz M, Özsoy ÆED, Bozcuk ÆAN (2008) Maternal age effects on longevity in Drosophila melanogaster populations of different origin. Biogerontology 9: 163–168.M. YılmazÆED ÖzsoyÆAN Bozcuk2008Maternal age effects on longevity in Drosophila melanogaster populations of different origin.Biogerontology9163168
  16. 16. Priest NK, Mackowiak B, Promislow DEL (2002) The role of parental age effects on the evolution of aging. Evolution 56: 927–935.NK PriestB. MackowiakDEL Promislow2002The role of parental age effects on the evolution of aging.Evolution56927935
  17. 17. Kern S, Ackerman M, Stearns SC, Kawecki TJ (2001) Decline in offspring viability as a manifestation of aging in Drosophila melanogaster. Evolution 55: 1822–1831.S. KernM. AckermanSC StearnsTJ Kawecki2001Decline in offspring viability as a manifestation of aging in Drosophila melanogaster.Evolution5518221831
  18. 18. Moore PJ, Harris WE (2003) Is a decline in offspring quality a necessary consequence of maternal age? Proc R Soc B 270: S192–S194.PJ MooreWE Harris2003Is a decline in offspring quality a necessary consequence of maternal age?Proc R Soc B270S192S194
  19. 19. Partridge L, Gems D (2007) Benchmarks for ageing studies. Nature 450: 165–167.L. PartridgeD. Gems2007Benchmarks for ageing studies.Nature450165167
  20. 20. Descamps S, Boutin S, Berteaux D, Gaillard JM (2008) Age-specific variation in survival, reproductive success and offspring quality in red squirrels: evidence of senescence. Oikos 117: 1406–1416.S. DescampsS. BoutinD. BerteauxJM Gaillard2008Age-specific variation in survival, reproductive success and offspring quality in red squirrels: evidence of senescence.Oikos11714061416
  21. 21. Bouwhuis S, Sheldon BC, Verhulst S, Charmantier A (2009) Great tits growing old: selective disappearance and the partitioning of senescence to stages within the breeding cycle. Proc R Soc B 276: 2769–2777.S. BouwhuisBC SheldonS. VerhulstA. Charmantier2009Great tits growing old: selective disappearance and the partitioning of senescence to stages within the breeding cycle.Proc R Soc B27627692777
  22. 22. Bouwhuis S, Charmantier A, Verhulst S, Sheldon BC (2010) Trans-generational effects on ageing in a wild bird population. J Evol Biol 23: 636–642.S. BouwhuisA. CharmantierS. VerhulstBC Sheldon2010Trans-generational effects on ageing in a wild bird population.J Evol Biol23636642
  23. 23. Fitzgerald C, Zimon AE, Jones EE (1998) Aging and reproductive potential in women. Yale J Biol Med 71: 367–381.C. FitzgeraldAE ZimonEE Jones1998Aging and reproductive potential in women.Yale J Biol Med71367381
  24. 24. Velando A, Torres R, Alonso-Alvarez C (2008) Avoiding bad genes: oxidatively damaged DNA in germ line and mate choice. BioEssays 30: 11–12.A. VelandoR. TorresC. Alonso-Alvarez2008Avoiding bad genes: oxidatively damaged DNA in germ line and mate choice.BioEssays301112
  25. 25. Beamonte-Barrientos R, Velando A, Drummond H, Torres R (2010) Senescence of maternal effects: aging influences egg quality and rearing capacities of a long-lived bird. Am Nat 175: 469–480.R. Beamonte-BarrientosA. VelandoH. DrummondR. Torres2010Senescence of maternal effects: aging influences egg quality and rearing capacities of a long-lived bird.Am Nat175469480
  26. 26. Møller AP, Mousseau TA, Rudolfsen G, Balbontín J, Marzal A, et al. (2009) Senescent sperm performance in old male birds. J Evol Biol 22: 334–344.AP MøllerTA MousseauG. RudolfsenJ. BalbontínA. Marzal2009Senescent sperm performance in old male birds.J Evol Biol22334344
  27. 27. Velando A, Noguera JC, Drummond H, Torres R (2011) Senescent males carry premutagenic lesions in sperm. J Evol Biol 24: 693–697.A. VelandoJC NogueraH. DrummondR. Torres2011Senescent males carry premutagenic lesions in sperm.J Evol Biol24693697
  28. 28. Pizzari T, Dean R, Pacey A, Moore H, Bonsall MB (2008) The evolutionary ecology of pre- and post-meiotic sperm senescence. Trends Ecol Evol 23: 131–140.T. PizzariR. DeanA. PaceyH. MooreMB Bonsall2008The evolutionary ecology of pre- and post-meiotic sperm senescence.Trends Ecol Evol23131140
  29. 29. White J, Wagner RH, Helfenstein F, Hatch SA, Mulard H, et al. (2008) Multiple deleterious effects of experimentally aged sperm in a monogamous bird. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105: 13947–13952.J. WhiteRH WagnerF. HelfensteinSA HatchH. Mulard2008Multiple deleterious effects of experimentally aged sperm in a monogamous bird.Proc Natl Acad Sci USA1051394713952
  30. 30. Cam E, Monnat JY (2000) Apparent inferiority of first time breeders in the Kittiwake: the role of heterogeneity among age classes. J Anim Ecol 69: 380–394.E. CamJY Monnat2000Apparent inferiority of first time breeders in the Kittiwake: the role of heterogeneity among age classes.J Anim Ecol69380394
  31. 31. Barbraud C, Weimerskirch H (2005) Environmental conditions and breeding experience affect costs of reproduction in blue petrels. Ecology 86: 682–692.C. BarbraudH. Weimerskirch2005Environmental conditions and breeding experience affect costs of reproduction in blue petrels.Ecology86682692
  32. 32. Sanz-Aguilar A, Tavecchia G, Pradel R, Mínguez E, Oro D (2008) The cost of reproduction and experience-dependent vital rates in a small petrel. Ecology 89: 3195–3203.A. Sanz-AguilarG. TavecchiaR. PradelE. MínguezD. Oro2008The cost of reproduction and experience-dependent vital rates in a small petrel.Ecology8931953203
  33. 33. Curio E (1983) Why do young birds reproduce less well? Ibis 125: 400–404.E. Curio1983Why do young birds reproduce less well?Ibis125400404
  34. 34. Nol E, Smith JNM (1987) Effects of age and breeding experience on seasonal reproductive success in the song sparrow. J Anim Ecol 56: 301–313.E. NolJNM Smith1987Effects of age and breeding experience on seasonal reproductive success in the song sparrow.J Anim Ecol56301313
  35. 35. Komdeur J (1996) Influence of age on reproductive performance in the Seychelles warbler. Behav Ecol 7: 417–425.J. Komdeur1996Influence of age on reproductive performance in the Seychelles warbler.Behav Ecol7417425
  36. 36. Pärt T (2001) Experimental evidence of environmental effects on age-specific reproductive success: the importance of resource quality. Proc R Soc B 268: 2267–2271.T. Pärt2001Experimental evidence of environmental effects on age-specific reproductive success: the importance of resource quality.Proc R Soc B26822672271
  37. 37. Kirkwood TBL, Austad SN (2000) Why do we age? Nature 408: 233–238.TBL KirkwoodSN Austad2000Why do we age?Nature408233238
  38. 38. McCleerly RH, Perrins CM, Sheldon BC, Charmantier A (2008) Age-specific reproduction in a long-lived species: the combined effects of senescence and individual quality. Proc R Soc B 275: 963–970.RH McCleerlyCM PerrinsBC SheldonA. Charmantier2008Age-specific reproduction in a long-lived species: the combined effects of senescence and individual quality.Proc R Soc B275963970
  39. 39. van de Pol M, Verhulst S (2006) Age-dependent traits: a new statistical model to separate within- and between individual effects. Am Nat 167: 766–773.M. van de PolS. Verhulst2006Age-dependent traits: a new statistical model to separate within- and between individual effects.Am Nat167766773
  40. 40. Nussey D, Coulson JC, Festa-Bianchet M, Gaillard J-M (2008) Measuring senescence in wild animal populations: towards a longitudinal approach. Funct Ecol 22: 393–406.D. NusseyJC CoulsonM. Festa-BianchetJ-M Gaillard2008Measuring senescence in wild animal populations: towards a longitudinal approach.Funct Ecol22393406
  41. 41. Reid JM, Bignal EM, McCracken DI, Monaghan P (2003) Age-specific reproductive performance in red-billed choughs Pyrrhocorax pyrrhocorax: patterns and processes in a natural population. J Anim Ecol 72: 765–776.JM ReidEM BignalDI McCrackenP. Monaghan2003Age-specific reproductive performance in red-billed choughs Pyrrhocorax pyrrhocorax: patterns and processes in a natural population.J Anim Ecol72765776
  42. 42. Coulson JC, Fairweather JA (2001) Reduced reproductive performance prior to death in the black-legged kittiwake: senescence or terminal illness? J Avian Biol 32: 146–152.JC CoulsonJA Fairweather2001Reduced reproductive performance prior to death in the black-legged kittiwake: senescence or terminal illness?J Avian Biol32146152
  43. 43. Rattiste K (2004) Reproductive success in presenescent common gulls (Larus canus): the importance of the last year of life. Proc R Soc B 271: 2059–2064.K. Rattiste2004Reproductive success in presenescent common gulls (Larus canus): the importance of the last year of life.Proc R Soc B27120592064
  44. 44. Ricklefs RE (2008) The evolution of senescence from a comparative perspective. Funct Ecol 22: 379–392.RE Ricklefs2008The evolution of senescence from a comparative perspective.Funct Ecol22379392
  45. 45. Williams GC (1966) Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton: Princeton University Press. GC Williams1966Adaptation and natural selectionPrincetonPrinceton University Press
  46. 46. Clutton-Brock TH (1984) Reproductive effort and terminal investment in iteroparous animals. Am Nat 123: 212–229.TH Clutton-Brock1984Reproductive effort and terminal investment in iteroparous animals.Am Nat123212229
  47. 47. Nelson B (1978) The Sulidae: Gannets and Boobies. Oxford: Oxford University Press. B. Nelson1978The Sulidae: Gannets and BoobiesOxfordOxford University Press
  48. 48. Torres R, Drummond H (1999) Does large size make daughters of the blue-footed booby more expensive than sons? J Anim Ecol 68: 1–10.R. TorresH. Drummond1999Does large size make daughters of the blue-footed booby more expensive than sons?J Anim Ecol68110
  49. 49. Drummond H, Torres R, Krishnan VV (2003) Buffered development: resilience after aggressive subordination in infancy. Am Nat 161: 794–807.H. DrummondR. TorresVV Krishnan2003Buffered development: resilience after aggressive subordination in infancy.Am Nat161794807
  50. 50. Oro D, Torres R, Rodríguez C, Drummond H (2010) Climatic influence on demographic parameters of a tropical seabird varies with age and sex. Ecology 91: 1205–1214.D. OroR. TorresC. RodríguezH. Drummond2010Climatic influence on demographic parameters of a tropical seabird varies with age and sex.Ecology9112051214
  51. 51. Velando A, Drummond H, Torres R (2006) Senescent birds redouble reproductive effort when ill: confirmation of the terminal investment hypothesis. Proc R Soc B 273: 1443–1448.A. VelandoH. DrummondR. Torres2006Senescent birds redouble reproductive effort when ill: confirmation of the terminal investment hypothesis.Proc R Soc B27314431448
  52. 52. Torres R, Velando A (2007) Male reproductive senescence: the price of immune-induced oxidative damage on sexual attractiveness in the blue-footed booby. J Anim Ecol 76: 1161–1168.R. TorresA. Velando2007Male reproductive senescence: the price of immune-induced oxidative damage on sexual attractiveness in the blue-footed booby.J Anim Ecol7611611168
  53. 53. Kim S-Y, Velando A, Torres R, Drummond H (2011) Effects of recruiting age on senescence, lifespan and lifetime reproductive success in a long-lived seabird. Oecologia 166: 615–626.S-Y KimA. VelandoR. TorresH. Drummond2011Effects of recruiting age on senescence, lifespan and lifetime reproductive success in a long-lived seabird.Oecologia166615626
  54. 54. Guerra M, Drummond H (1995) Reversed sexual size dimorphism and parental care: minimal division of labour in the blue-footed booby. Behaviour 132: 479–496.M. GuerraH. Drummond1995Reversed sexual size dimorphism and parental care: minimal division of labour in the blue-footed booby.Behaviour132479496
  55. 55. Velando A, Alonso-Alvarez C (2003) Differential body condition regulation by males and females in response to experimental manipulations of brood size and parental effort in the blue-footed Booby. J Anim Ecol 72: 846–856.A. VelandoC. Alonso-Alvarez2003Differential body condition regulation by males and females in response to experimental manipulations of brood size and parental effort in the blue-footed Booby.J Anim Ecol72846856
  56. 56. Osorio-Beristain M, Drummond H (1993) Natal dispersal and deferred breeding in the blue-footed booby. Auk 110: 234–239.M. Osorio-BeristainH. Drummond1993Natal dispersal and deferred breeding in the blue-footed booby.Auk110234239
  57. 57. Kim S-Y, Torres R, Domínguez C, Drummond H (2007) Lifetime philopatry in the blue-footed booby: a longitudinal study. Behav Ecol 18: 1132–1138.S-Y KimR. TorresC. DomínguezH. Drummond2007Lifetime philopatry in the blue-footed booby: a longitudinal study.Behav Ecol1811321138
  58. 58. Kim S-Y, Torres R, Drummond H (2009) Simultaneous positive and negative density-dependent dispersal in a colonial bird species. Ecology 90: 230–239.S-Y KimR. TorresH. Drummond2009Simultaneous positive and negative density-dependent dispersal in a colonial bird species.Ecology90230239
  59. 59. SAS Institute (1999) SAS Online Documentation, Version 8. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc. SAS Institute1999SAS Online Documentation, Version 8Cary, NCSAS Institute Inc
  60. 60. Littell RC, Milliken GA, Stroup WW, Wolfinger RD, Schabenberger O (2006) SAS for mixed models. 2nd edition. Cary: SAS Institute Inc. RC LittellGA MillikenWW StroupRD WolfingerO. Schabenberger2006SAS for mixed models. 2nd editionCarySAS Institute Inc
  61. 61. Thompson PM, Ollason JC (2001) Lagged effects of ocean climate change on fulmar population dynamics. Nature 413: 417–420.PM ThompsonJC Ollason2001Lagged effects of ocean climate change on fulmar population dynamics.Nature413417420
  62. 62. Crespin L, Harris MP, Lebreton JD, Frederiksen M, Wanless S (2006) Recruitment to a seabird population depends on environmental factors and on population size. J Anim Ecol 75: 228–238.L. CrespinMP HarrisJD LebretonM. FrederiksenS. Wanless2006Recruitment to a seabird population depends on environmental factors and on population size.J Anim Ecol75228238
  63. 63. Perrins CM (1970) The timing of birds' breeding seasons. Ibis 112: 242–255.CM Perrins1970The timing of birds' breeding seasons.Ibis112242255
  64. 64. Daan S, Dijkstra C, Tinbergen JM (1990) Family planning in the kestrel (Falco tinnunculus): the ultimate control of variation in laying date and clutch size. Behaviour 114: 83–116.S. DaanC. DijkstraJM Tinbergen1990Family planning in the kestrel (Falco tinnunculus): the ultimate control of variation in laying date and clutch size.Behaviour11483116
  65. 65. Caro SP, Charmantier A, Lambrechts MM, Blondel J, Balthazart J, et al. (2009) Local adaptation of timing of reproduction: females are in the driver's seat. Funct Ecol 23: 172–179.SP CaroA. CharmantierMM LambrechtsJ. BlondelJ. Balthazart2009Local adaptation of timing of reproduction: females are in the driver's seat.Funct Ecol23172179
  66. 66. Nilsson J-A (1999) Fitness consequences of timing of reproduction. J-A Nilsson1999Fitness consequences of timing of reproduction.In: Proc. 22 Int. Ornithol. Congr., Durban (eds NJ Adams and RH Slotow), pp. 234–247. Johannesburg, South Africa: Birdlife South Africa.
  67. 67. Velando A (2000) The importance of the hatching date on dominance of young shags. Animl Behav 60: 181–185.A. Velando2000The importance of the hatching date on dominance of young shags.Animl Behav60181185
  68. 68. Aguirre JI, Vergara P (2007) Younger, weaker white stork (Ciconia ciconia) nestlings become the best breeders. Evol Ecol Res 9: 355–364.JI AguirreP. Vergara2007Younger, weaker white stork (Ciconia ciconia) nestlings become the best breeders.Evol Ecol Res9355364
  69. 69. Reed TE, Kruuk LEB, Wanless S, Frederiksen M, Cunningham EJA, et al. (2008) Reproductive senescence in a long-lived seabird: rates of decline in late-life performance are associated with varying costs of early reproduction. Am Nat 171: E89–E101.TE ReedLEB KruukS. WanlessM. FrederiksenEJA Cunningham2008Reproductive senescence in a long-lived seabird: rates of decline in late-life performance are associated with varying costs of early reproduction.Am Nat171E89E101
  70. 70. Daunt F, Wanless S, Harris MP, Monaghan P (1999) Experimental evidence that age-specific reproductive success is independent of environmental effects. Proc R Soc B 266: 1489–1493.F. DauntS. WanlessMP HarrisP. Monaghan1999Experimental evidence that age-specific reproductive success is independent of environmental effects.Proc R Soc B26614891493
  71. 71. Angelier F, Weimerskirch H, Dano S, Chastel O (2007) Age, experience and reproductive performance in a long-lived bird: a hormonal perspective. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61: 611–621.F. AngelierH. WeimerskirchS. DanoO. Chastel2007Age, experience and reproductive performance in a long-lived bird: a hormonal perspective.Behav Ecol Sociobiol61611621
  72. 72. McNamara JM, Houston AI, Barta Z, Scheuerlein A, Fromhage L (2009) Deterioration, death and the evolution of reproductive restraint in late life. Proc R Soc B 276: 4061–4066.JM McNamaraAI HoustonZ. BartaA. ScheuerleinL. Fromhage2009Deterioration, death and the evolution of reproductive restraint in late life.Proc R Soc B27640614066
  73. 73. Waskar M, Li Y, Tower J (2005) Stem cell aging in the Drosophila ovary. Age 27: 201–212.M. WaskarY. LiJ. Tower2005Stem cell aging in the Drosophila ovary.Age27201212
  74. 74. Hansen TF, Price DK (1995) Good genes and old age: Do old mates provide superior genes? J Evol Biol 8: 759–778.TF HansenDK Price1995Good genes and old age: Do old mates provide superior genes?J Evol Biol8759778
  75. 75. Wyrobek AJ, Eskenazi B, Young S, Arnheim N, Tiemann-Boege I, et al. (2006) Advancing age has differential effects on DNA damage, chromatin integrity, gene mutations, and aneuploidies in sperm. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103: 9601–9606.AJ WyrobekB. EskenaziS. YoungN. ArnheimI. Tiemann-Boege2006Advancing age has differential effects on DNA damage, chromatin integrity, gene mutations, and aneuploidies in sperm.Proc Natl Acad Sci USA10396019606
  76. 76. Rose MR (1991) Evolutionary biology of ageing. Oxford: Oxford University Press. MR Rose1991Evolutionary biology of ageingOxfordOxford University Press
  77. 77. Hayflic L (2000) The future of ageing. Nature 408: 267–269.L. Hayflic2000The future of ageing.Nature408267269
  78. 78. Kim S-Y, Drummond H, Torres R, Velando A (2011) Evolvability of an avian life-history trait declines with father's age. J Evol Biol 24: 295–302.S-Y KimH. DrummondR. TorresA. Velando2011Evolvability of an avian life-history trait declines with father's age.J Evol Biol24295302