Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

How Abnormal Is the Behaviour of Captive, Zoo-Living Chimpanzees?

Abstract

Background

Many captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) show a variety of serious behavioural abnormalities, some of which have been considered as possible signs of compromised mental health. The provision of environmental enrichments aimed at reducing the performance of abnormal behaviours is increasing the norm, with the housing of individuals in (semi-)natural social groups thought to be the most successful of these. Only a few quantitative studies of abnormal behaviour have been conducted, however, particularly for the captive population held in zoological collections. Consequently, a clear picture of the level of abnormal behaviour in zoo-living chimpanzees is lacking.

Methods

We present preliminary findings from a detailed observational study of the behaviour of 40 socially-housed zoo-living chimpanzees from six collections in the United States of America and the United Kingdom. We determined the prevalence, diversity, frequency, and duration of abnormal behaviour from 1200 hours of continuous behavioural data collected by focal animal sampling.

Results, Conclusion and Significance

Our overall finding was that abnormal behaviour was present in all sampled individuals across six independent groups of zoo-living chimpanzees, despite the differences between these groups in size, composition, housing, etc. We found substantial variation between individuals in the frequency and duration of abnormal behaviour, but all individuals engaged in at least some abnormal behaviour and variation across individuals could not be explained by sex, age, rearing history or background (defined as prior housing conditions). Our data support a conclusion that, while most behaviour of zoo-living chimpanzees is ‘normal’ in that it is typical of their wild counterparts, abnormal behaviour is endemic in this population despite enrichment efforts. We suggest there is an urgent need to understand how the chimpanzee mind copes with captivity, an issue with both scientific and welfare implications.

Introduction

Captive conditions are known to evoke abnormal behaviour patterns in a variety of species, including non-human primates [1], [2], [3], [4]. Many chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) kept in laboratory housing settings show a variety of serious behavioural abnormalities, such as repetitive rocking, drinking of urine, or self-mutilation [4], [5], [6], [7], [8], [9], [10], [11], [12]. Previous work indicates that various abnormal behaviour patterns also occur among chimpanzees held in zoological collections [13], [14], [15], [16], but detailed, quantitative studies on the zoo population are few [17].

Abnormal behaviours may indicate psychological suffering [18], but this is seldom considered directly (cf. Bradshaw and colleagues [11], [12]). Principles of evolutionary psychiatry [19] suggest that some abnormal behaviours may be symptomatic of underlying mental illness, a neglected area of research in great apes [17]. These possibilities raise serious ethical questions, particularly given the emotional and cognitive abilities of chimpanzees [20], [21], [22], [23], [24], although a consistent framework for the study of abnormal behaviour in apes has not yet been achieved [5], [17].

Social deprivation, and particularly maternal separation, have been suggested as causal factors in the development of abnormal behaviour in captive chimpanzees [15], [25], [26]: such events may be psychologically traumatic, or may deprive individuals of opportunities to learn appropriate behaviour. Persistent effects as a consequence of rearing have been found in some chimpanzees [16], but not all [27]. The impact of rearing history, while marked in younger chimpanzees, appears to wane as individuals age, at least in those re-socialised and housed with conspecifics [15], [27]. Such findings might suggest that social group housing, with the consequent opportunities for development of appropriate social relationships, will ameliorate the negative effects of early experiences [16], [28]. Social housing has been suggested as the most effective means of combating the occurrence and development of abnormal behaviours in primates [28], despite a lack of consensus over the causal roots of such behaviour [29], [30].

In this paper we present preliminary findings from a detailed, quantitative investigation of abnormal behaviour in captive (zoo-living) chimpanzees based on extensive and direct observation, comparable with that used for behavioural study of wild chimpanzees (for example: [31], [32], [33], [34]). We focus on adult and adolescent individuals housed in social groups, the context in which individuals should be least likely to display abnormal behaviour: our specific aim is to determine the level of abnormal behaviour in zoo-living, socially enriched, captive chimpanzees. We report detailed descriptive data on the occurrence of abnormal behaviour, and test whether performance of abnormal behaviour is related to social group size, rearing history or background (housing prior to entering the study group). As chimpanzee behaviour varies with age and sex, and some sex differences in abnormal behaviour have been reported [29], [35], we also test for the influence of these variables on the occurrence of abnormal behaviour.

Materials and Methods

Abnormal behaviours among captive animals are those that are atypical of wild-living individuals, whether absent or occurring only rarely. We used published sources on behaviour in chimpanzees [6], [9], [11], [15], [29], [36], [37], [38] to compile a list of abnormal behaviours seen in the captive population. As a consequence of our observations (see below), ten further behaviours were added to this list: bounce, groom stereotypically, groom stereotypically with object, move hand repetitively, rub hands repetitively, toss head, touch urine stream, poke anus, and walk on object; bite-hit-lick was added for one individual who performed these behaviours as an integrated pattern. We used the most comprehensive ethogram for wild chimpanzees available [38] to determine which of the behaviours have been recorded for wild chimpanzees, and for these, to confirm that they occurred only rarely. Our final list consisted of 37 behaviours (Table 1).

The first author collected observational data on 40 chimpanzees living in six accredited zoological institutions (accrediting agencies were the American Zoological Association – AZA - and the British and Irish Associations of Zoos and Aquariums - BIAZA). The chimpanzees were all housed in social groups, with one group from each of the six participating zoological gardens contributing to our dataset. We chose the zoos from a master list that gave no information about the chimpanzees' or the institutions' characteristics, housing, or husbandry. Once selected, the participating zoos provided information on the background of their chimpanzees. Zoos often provide a valuable retirement sanctuary for primates and as a result, the backgrounds of the chimpanzees in this study were varied, including “wild caught,” entertainment industry, pet trade, laboratories, other zoological gardens, and sometimes a combination of these. As only partial background information was available in several cases, we classified background as either: wild, laboratory, pet-trade/entertainment, or zoo.

Across the groups there were 17 male and 23 female chimpanzees. Median group size was 6.5 (range = 3–11). Groups were either single-male (two groups) or multi-male (four groups, one of which was male only) and included a wide range of ages (Table 2). For analysis, we assigned individuals to age categories: 8–15 yrs, 16–23 yrs, 24–31 yrs, 32–39 yrs, and 40+ yrs. Five of the six groups showed a mix of rearing styles (wild-born, mother-reared, hand-reared: reared by human care-giver(s) and bottle fed); the sixth consisted of individuals who were all hand-reared. All groups had access to multiple areas (both indoor and outdoor). Only one of the groups had access restricted to a single areas at any one time, typically outside during the day, and inside at night. All enclosures met AZA/BIAZA standards.

Data were collected from December 2008 to May 2010. Thirty hours of continuous focal sampling (a method of data collection in which all activities of a designated individual are recorded [39], [40]) were collected for each chimpanzee, yielding a total of 1200 observation hours (1800 min per individual). Data were collected using pen-and-paper from the public viewing areas at each zoo at distances ranging from 0.1 m (through glass) to about 10 m between observer and subject. Samples lasted 30 min and were collected in randomized rotation among members of each group, except for one zoo where reduced observability of subjects meant that samples lasted 20 min and could not be randomized at some times of the day. Once a focal sample began, recording was continuous unless a subject went out of sight; for every minute of such ‘bad observation’ one minute was added to the end of the sample.

From these data, we constructed four measures of abnormal behaviour: prevalence (proportion of individuals in a group who show a type of behaviour), diversity (number of types of behaviour), frequency (number of bouts of behaviour), and duration (amount of time spent performing a behaviour). Since our behavioural data were not normally distributed, we used nonparametric statistical tests [41]. All tests were two-tailed, and following convention, we set α at 0.05. Data were analyzed using SPSS Statistics 19 (SPSS Inc.).

Results

All 40 chimpanzees showed some abnormal behaviour. Across groups, the most prevalent behaviour (0.83) was eat faeces (Table 3; Figure 1). Six behaviours were present in all six groups (eat faeces, rock, groom stereotypically, pat genitals, regurgitate, fumble nipple) and a further two (pluck hair and hit self) were present in five of the six groups. Bite self was shown by eight individuals across four of the groups. Across groups and behaviours, the median prevalence was 0.1 (range: 0.03–0.83). The number of different abnormal behaviours displayed in each group varied from 15 to 23 (mean = 18) and was not correlated with the number of individuals in the group (r = 0.46; n = 6, p = 0.35).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Percentage of chimpanzees from six independent zoological collections displaying each of the indicated abnormal behaviours.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020101.g001

thumbnail
Table 3. Abnormal behaviours shown by zoo-living chimpanzees (n = 40).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020101.t003

Each of the 40 focal subjects displayed at least 2 abnormal behaviours (median: 5, range: 2–14). The number of different behaviours per individual (diversity) did not differ significantly across groups (Kruskal-Wallis H = 6.28, df = 5, p = 0.28). The lowest-scoring group had a median diversity of 4.5, and the highest-scoring group had a median diversity of 6.5. Similarly, the frequency of abnormal behaviour did not differ across groups (H = 5.75, df = 5, p = 0.33): group (C) with the lowest frequency had a median frequency of 0.6/hr, while group A had the highest median frequency of 3.2/hr (Table 4). The median total duration of abnormal behaviour likewise did not differ across groups (H = 1.28, df = 5, p = 0.94).

thumbnail
Table 4. Performance of 37 abnormal behaviour patterns by social group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020101.t004

In contrast, both frequency and duration of abnormal behaviours varied greatly between individuals. Across individuals, the median frequency was 1.45/hr but this ranged from 0.13/hr to 13.5/hr. The median total duration of abnormal behaviour was 79 min, or 4.4% of 30 hr observation time, but there was great individual variation (0.1–62.4% of activity: Table 4)

We found no effect of age or sex on the occurrence of abnormal behaviour whether measured by diversity (Sex: Mann-Whitney U = 187, n = 17, 23, p = 0.811; Age: Kruskal-Wallis H = 4.40, df = 4, p = 0.35), frequency (Sex: U = 212, p = 0.652; Age: H = 7.11, df = 4, 0.13) or duration (Sex: U = 147, p = 0.185; Age: H = 5.75, df = 4, p = 0.22). We also found no effect of rearing history (wild-born: n = 9, mother-reared: n = 17, hand-reared: n = 9) on the occurrence of abnormal behaviour, for diversity (H = 1.95, df = 2, p = 0.38), frequency (H = 2.46, df = 2, p = 0.29) or duration (H = 0.05, df = 2, p = 0.97); the ‘unknown’ group (n = 5) was omitted from this analysis. We also found no effect of background (wild, laboratory, pet-trade/entertainment, zoo) on diversity (H = 2.92, df = 3, p = 0.40), frequency (H = 0.99, df = 3, p = 0.81), or duration (H = 0.74, df = 3, p = 0.86).

Discussion

Our overall finding was that abnormal behaviour was present in all sampled individuals across six independent groups of zoo-living chimpanzees, despite the differences between these groups in size, composition, housing, etc. We found substantial variation between individuals in the frequency and duration of abnormal behaviour, but all individuals engaged in at least some abnormal behaviour, and variation across individuals could not be explained by sex, age, rearing history or background (defined as prior housing conditions).

Direct comparison of our results to earlier studies is complicated by differences in methods, particularly the measures used. Some studies coded only presence or absence for a behaviour, limiting any comparison to prevalence. On this measure, we found - across individuals and behaviours - a very similar level (0.10) to that (0.11; calculated from their Table 3) reported by Fritz et al. [10] (also Nash et al. [29]), although the most prevalent behaviour across these studies - eat faeces (coprophagy) – is performed by more individuals in our sample (prevalence: 0.85 vs. 0.49). Other studies present the fraction of activity budget spent in abnormal behaviour, calculated from durations or proportion of interval samples. By this measure, the individuals in our sample spent considerably more time behaving abnormally. Martin et al. [15] reported a median value of zero; four individuals who spent 5.6% of their time behaving abnormally were described as showing exceptionally high levels of abnormal behaviour. Similarly, Bloomsmith et al. [9] reported an average of only 0.3% (see also Hook et al. [4]). These figures are substantially lower than our average figure of 4.4%, and maximum of >60%.

Our study combined a relatively large sample of individuals across multiple populations with detailed, lengthy, direct, and continuous observations of behaviour. We found that some of the abnormal behaviours were performed for only short durations, or at low frequencies; many abnormal behaviours occurred rarely and only in limited contexts, and were often brief. Our choice of sampling method may have allowed us to record behaviours that might have been missed using other approaches. Continuous focal animal sampling allows the determination of prevalence, frequency, and duration, and so provides a more complete picture of the behaviour under investigation. For these reasons, this is the preferred sampling method for observational behavioural studies [39], [40] and we suggest future studies of abnormal behaviour should use this method when logistics allow.

So how abnormal is the behaviour of captive chimpanzees? In 1023 hours of focal animal sampling of wild chimpanzees in Uganda, none of the abnormal behaviours listed in this study were observed (Newton-Fisher, unpublished data). In contrast, seventeen of 37 behaviours we detailed in this study have been reported to occur (albeit rarely) in wild chimpanzees [20], [38]. The most prevalent form of ‘abnormal’ behaviour in this and other studies of captive populations – coprophagy – has been reported from at least six wild populations [42] and it may also be transmitted by social learning [29]. If this – or other – behaviour is ‘abnormal’ in captive chimpanzees, it may be the rate at which it is performed rather than simple occurrence that deviates from the behaviour of wild chimpanzees.

A formal comparison of rates and durations of these behaviour patterns between captive and wild populations is difficult: few sufficiently detailed quantitative data are available from wild populations, as might be expected for behaviours that are notably rare. The published literature on abnormal behaviour in wild chimpanzees is sparse [20], [42], [43] and rates of abnormality comparable to those that we describe here have never been reported. The hypothetical average individual in our sample of captive chimpanzees had a repertoire of five abnormal behaviours, performed some kind of abnormal behaviour once every forty minutes or so (although in real individuals, performance was more clumped than this figure implies) and spent between four and five percent of their activity in abnormal behaviours. Some individuals were far more extreme (over 60% of activity), and all individuals showed at least 2 different types of abnormal behaviour (cf. [29]).

Our data support a conclusion that, while most behaviour of captive chimpanzees is ‘normal’ in the sense that it is behaviour seen in their wild counterparts, abnormal behaviour is endemic in captivity. For some individuals it may dominate much of their activity, but for the rest it is a persistent element of their everyday behaviour despite living in social groups in enriched environments. Note that we do not address here whether captive chimpanzees use their normal behaviour in a manner atypical of wild chimpanzees, which may be a further dimension to ‘abnormal’ behaviour [44], [45].

In the wild, chimpanzee communities have a fluid social structure in which individuals are free to choose associates, mates, and ranging area (although all of these are subject to competitive effects). Similarly, they choose when, where, and what to eat; their natural diets include many species of flora and fauna, and they use a large variety of foraging, food processing, and hunting methods. Their daily activities vary accordingly, and they range widely over varied landscapes and habitat types [20], [46], [47], [48], [49]. In comparison, zoo-living chimpanzees have little opportunity to adjust association patterns, occupy restricted and barren spaces compared to the natural habitat, and have large parts of their lives substantially managed by humans [14]. Controlled diets and provisioned feeding contrast radically with the ever-changing foraging and decision-making processes of daily life in the wild.

Providing captive apes with more naturalistic enclosures, unpredictable feeding schedules and extractive foraging opportunities, as well as the opportunity to interact with conspecifics through housing in social groups, all appear to decrease the performance of abnormal behaviours [9], [13], [45], [50], [51]. Even the best zoo environments (which includes all zoos in this study) are limited, however, in what they can provide [13]. Some abnormal behaviours persist despite interventions to ‘naturalise’ the captive conditions and we suggest that captivity itself may be fundamental as a causal factor in the presence of persistent, low-level, abnormal behaviour (and potentially more extreme levels in some individuals). The cognitive and behavioural challenges in captivity are fewer than in the wild – stressful and dangerous place that it may be – and many normal behaviours and normal development are precluded. While extreme levels of abnormal behaviour may be explicable by individuals' particular histories, the pattern of low-level, pervasive abnormal behaviour shown by this study suggests that chimpanzee minds struggle to cope with conditions of captivity, despite the best efforts of those charged with their care.

From the perspective of human psychiatry, some of the behavioural abnormalities demonstrated by chimpanzees might be seen as symptoms of compromised mental health, i.e. mental illness, if they were observed in human primates [11], [17]. As with humans, chimpanzees are self-aware [52] sentient, emotional creatures [20], [53] with the potential to suffer [20], [54]. The apparently pervasive nature of abnormal behaviour, and its persistence in the face of environmental enrichment and social group housing, raises the concern that at least some examples of such behaviour are indicative of possible mental health problems [5], [11], [17].

Future research should address preventative or remedial actions, whether intervention is best aimed at the environment and/or the individual, and how to best monitor recovery [7]. More critically, however, we need to understand how the chimpanzee mind copes with captivity, an issue with both scientific [55] and welfare implications that will impact potential discussions concerning whether such species should be kept in captivity at all.

Acknowledgments

We thank all the participating zoological gardens for their help and support throughout the project, especially during data collection. BIAZA gave approval to the project and a letter of support, for which we are very grateful. Mr. E. Thetford gave inspiration at the start of the study, and L.P. Birkett thanks him very much for that.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: LPB NNF. Analyzed the data: LPB NNF. Wrote the paper: LPB NNF.

References

  1. 1. Young RJ (2003) Environmental enrichment for captive animals. Oxford: Blackwell Science Ltd. 240 p.
  2. 2. Mason G, Ruson J, editors. (2006) Stereotypic animal behaviour: fundamentals and applications to welfare. 2nd ed. Wallingford: CABI. 384 p.
  3. 3. Poole J, Granli P (2008) Mind and movement: Meeting the interests of elephants. In: Forthman DL, Kane LF, Hancocks D, Waldau PF, editors. An elephant in the room: The science and well-being of elephants in captivity. North Grafton, MA: Tufts University. pp. 2–21.
  4. 4. Hook MA, Lambeth SP, Perlman JE, Stavisky R, Bloomsmith MA, et al. (2002) Inter-group variation in abnormal behavior in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 76: 165–176.
  5. 5. Brüne M, Brüne-Cohrs U, McGrew WC (2004) Psychiatric treatment for great apes? Science 306: 2039–2039.
  6. 6. Walsh S, Bramblett CA, Alford PL (1982) A vocabulary of abnormal behaviors in restrictively reared chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology 3: 315–319.
  7. 7. Bourgeois SR, Vazquez M, Brasky K (2007) Combination therapy reduces self-injurious behavior in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes troglodytes): A case report. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 10: 123–140.
  8. 8. Baker KC, Easley SP (1996) An analysis of regurgitation and reingestion in captive chimpanzees. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 49: 403–415.
  9. 9. Bloomsmith MA, Lambeth SP (1995) Effects of predictable versus unpredictable feeding schedules on chimpanzee behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 44: 65–74.
  10. 10. Fritz J, Maki S, Nash LT, Martin T, Matevia M (1992) The relationship between forage material and levels of coprophagy in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Zoo Biology 11: 313–318.
  11. 11. Bradshaw GA, Capaldo T, Lindner L, Grow G (2008) Building an inner sanctuary: complex PTSD in chimpanzees. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation 9: 9–34.
  12. 12. Bradshaw GA, Capaldo T, Lindner L, Grow G (2009) Developmental context effects on bicultural posttrauma self repair in chimpanzees. Developmental Psychology 45: 1376–1388.
  13. 13. Clarke AS, Juno CJ, Maple TL (1982) Behavioral effects of a change in the physical environment: a pilot study of captive chimpanzees. Zoo Biology 1: 371–380.
  14. 14. Hosey GR, Skyner LJ (2007) Self-injurious behavior in zoo primates. International Journal of Primatology 28: 1431–1437.
  15. 15. Martin JE (2002) Early life experiences: Activity levels and abnormal behaviours in resocialised chimpanzees. Animal Welfare 11: 419–436.
  16. 16. Murray LE (1998) The effects of group structure and rearing strategy on personality in Chimpanzees Pan troglodytes at Chester, London ZSL and Twycross Zoos. International Zoo Yearbook 36: 97–108.
  17. 17. Brüne M, Brüne-Cohrs U, McGrew WC, Preuschoft S (2006) Psychopathology in great apes: concepts, treatment options and possible homologies to human psychiatric disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 30: 1246–1259.
  18. 18. Rollin B (2006) Science and ethics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. 304 p.
  19. 19. Brüne M (2008) Textbook of evolutionary psychiatry. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. 380 p.
  20. 20. Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behaviour. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press. 673 p.
  21. 21. de Waal FBM (1989) Food sharing and reciprocal oligations among chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution 18: 433–459.
  22. 22. Inoue S, Matsuzawa T (2007) Working memory of numerals in chimpanzees. Current Biology 17: R1004–1005.
  23. 23. Searle J (2002) Consciousness and language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. 280 p.
  24. 24. Mcgrew WC (1981) Social and cognitive capabilities of non-human primates: Lessons from the wild to captivity. International Journal for the Study of Animal Problems 2: 138–149.
  25. 25. Davenport RK (1979) Some behavioral disturbances of great apes in captivity. In: Hamburg D, McCown E, editors. The great apes. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin Cummings. pp. 341–357.
  26. 26. Davenport RK Jr, Menzel EW Jr (1963) Stereotyped behavior of the infant chimpanzee. Archives of General Psychiatry 8: 99–104.
  27. 27. Martin JE (2005) The influence of rearing on personality ratings of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 90: 167–181.
  28. 28. Lutz CK, Novak MA (2005) Environmental enrichment for nonhuman primates: theory and application. ILAR Journal 46: 178–191.
  29. 29. Nash LT, Fritz J, Alford PA, Brent L (1999) Variables influencing the origins of diverse abnormal behaviors in a large sample of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Americal Journal of Primatology 48: 15–29.
  30. 30. Hosey GR (2005) How does the zoo environment affect the behaviour of captive primates? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 90: 107–129.
  31. 31. Newton-Fisher NE (2004) Hierarchy and social status in Budongo chimpanzees. Primates 45: 81–87.
  32. 32. Newton-Fisher NE, Lee PC (2011) Grooming reciprocity in wild male chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 81: 439–446.
  33. 33. Muller MN (2002) Agonistic relations among Kanyawara chimpanzees. In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF, editors. Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 112–124.
  34. 34. Wrangham RW (1986) Ecology and social relationships in two species of chimpanzee. In: Rubenstein DI, Wrangham RW, editors. Ecology and social evolution: Birds and mammals. Princeton, USA: Princeton University Press. pp. 352–378.
  35. 35. Fritz J, Nash LT, Alford PL, Bowen JA (1992) Abnormal behaviors, with a special focus on rocking, and reproductive competence in a large sample of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology 27: 161–176.
  36. 36. Pederson AK, King JE, Landau VL (2005) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) personality predicts behavior. Journal of Research in Personality 39: 534–549.
  37. 37. Nishida T, Kano T, Goodall J, McGrew WC, Nakamura M (1999) Ethogram and ethnography of Mahale chimpanzees. Anthropological Science 107: 141–188.
  38. 38. Nishida T, Zamma K, Matsusaka T, Inaba A, McGrew WC (2010) Chimpanzee behavior in the wild: An audio-visual encyclopedia. Tokyo: Springer. 247 p.
  39. 39. Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behaviour: Sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227–265.
  40. 40. Martin P, Bateson P (2007) Measuring behaviour: An introductory guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 176 p.
  41. 41. Gibbon JD (1997) Nonparametric methods for quantitative analysis. New York: American Sciences Press. 537 p.
  42. 42. Payne CLR, Webster TH, Hunt KD (2008) Coprophagy by the semi-habituated chimpanzees of Semliki, Uganda. Pan Africa News 15: 29–32.
  43. 43. Hiraiwa-Hasegawa M, Hasegawa T (1988) A case of offspring desertion by a female chimpanzee and the behavioral-changes of the abandoned offspring. Primates 29: 319–330.
  44. 44. Paquette D, Paquette J (1988) Use of novel objects to enhance environments of captive chimpanzees. Zoo Biology 7: 15–23.
  45. 45. Celli ML, Tomonaga M, Udono T, Teramoto M, Nagano K (2003) Tool use task as environmental enrichment for captive chimpanzees. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 81: 171–182.
  46. 46. Boesch C, Boesch-Achermann H (2000) The chimpanzees of the Taï forest. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 316 p.
  47. 47. Nishida T (1990) The chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains: sexual and life history strategies. Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press. 328 p.
  48. 48. McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Nishida T (1996) Great ape societies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 352 p.
  49. 49. Reynolds V (2005) The chimpanzees of the Budongo forest: Ecology, behaviour and conservation. New York: Oxford University Press. xv, 297 p.
  50. 50. Bloomstrand M, Riddle K, Alford P, Maple TL (1986) Objective evaluation of a behavioral enrichment device for captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Zoo Biology 5: 293–300.
  51. 51. Lukas D, Reynolds V, Boesch C, Vigilant L (2005) To what extent does living in a group mean living with kin? Molecular Ecology 14: 2181–2196.
  52. 52. Gallup GG (1970) Chimpanzees: Self recognition. Science 167: 86–87.
  53. 53. Parr LA, Waller BM (2006) Understanding chimpanzee facial expression: Insights into the evolution of communication. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 1: 221–228.
  54. 54. Rollin B (1998) 330 p. The unheeded cry: Animal consciousness, animal pain, and science.
  55. 55. Garner JP (2005) Stereotypies and other abnormal repetitive behaviors: Potential impact on validity, reliability, and replicability of scientific outcomes ILAR Journal 46: 106–117.