Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Collective Response of Human Populations to Large-Scale Emergencies

  • James P. Bagrow ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: James P. Bagrow, Dashun Wang

    bagrowjp@gmail.com

    Affiliations Center for Complex Network Research, Department of Physics, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America, Center for Cancer Systems Biology, Dana-Farber Cancer Institute, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Dashun Wang ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: James P. Bagrow, Dashun Wang

    Affiliations Center for Complex Network Research, Department of Physics, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America, Center for Cancer Systems Biology, Dana-Farber Cancer Institute, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Albert-László Barabási

    Affiliations Center for Complex Network Research, Department of Physics, Northeastern University, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America, Center for Cancer Systems Biology, Dana-Farber Cancer Institute, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America, Department of Medicine, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, United States of America

Abstract

Despite recent advances in uncovering the quantitative features of stationary human activity patterns, many applications, from pandemic prediction to emergency response, require an understanding of how these patterns change when the population encounters unfamiliar conditions. To explore societal response to external perturbations we identified real-time changes in communication and mobility patterns in the vicinity of eight emergencies, such as bomb attacks and earthquakes, comparing these with eight non-emergencies, like concerts and sporting events. We find that communication spikes accompanying emergencies are both spatially and temporally localized, but information about emergencies spreads globally, resulting in communication avalanches that engage in a significant manner the social network of eyewitnesses. These results offer a quantitative view of behavioral changes in human activity under extreme conditions, with potential long-term impact on emergency detection and response.

Introduction

Current research on human dynamics is limited to data collected under normal and stationary circumstances [1], capturing the regular daily activity of individuals [2], [3], [4], [5], [6], [7], [8], [9], [10], [11], [12], [13], [14], [15]. Yet, there is exceptional need to understand how people change their behavior when exposed to rapidly changing or unfamiliar conditions [1], such as life-threatening epidemic outbreaks [4], [12], emergencies and traffic anomalies, as models based on stationary events are expected to break down under these circumstances. Such rapid changes in conditions are often caused by natural, technological or societal disasters, from hurricanes to violent conflicts [16]. The possibility to study such real time changes has emerged recently thanks to the widespread use of mobile phones, which track both user mobility [2], [3], [6], [17] and real-time communications along the links of the underlying social network [7], [18]. Here we take advantage of the fact that mobile phones act as in situ sensors at the site of an emergency, to study the real-time behavioral patterns of the local population under external perturbations caused by emergencies. Advances in this direction not only help redefine our understanding of information propagation [19] and cooperative human actions under externally induced perturbations, which is the main motivation of our work, but also offer a new perspective on panic [20], [21], [22], [23] and emergency protocols in a data-rich environment [24].

Our starting point is a country-wide mobile communications dataset, culled from the anonymized billing records of approximately ten million mobile phone subscribers of a mobile company which covers about one-fourth of subscribers in a country with close to full mobile penetration. It provides the time and duration of each mobile phone call [7], together with information on the tower that handled the call, thus capturing the real-time locations of the users [3], [6], [25] (Methods, File S1, Fig. A). To identify potential societal perturbations, we scanned media reports pertaining to the coverage area between January 2007 and January 2009 and developed a corpus of times and locations for eight societal, technological, and natural emergencies, ranging from bombings to a plane crash, earthquakes, floods and storms (Table 1). Approximately 30% of the events mentioned in the media occurred in locations with sparse cellular coverage or during times when few users are active (like very early in the morning). The remaining events do offer, however, a sufficiently diverse corpus to explore the generic vs. unique changes in the activity patterns in response to an emergency. Here we discuss four events, chosen for their diversity: (1) a bombing, resulting in several injuries (no fatalities); (2) a plane crash resulting in a significant number of fatalities; (3) an earthquake whose epicenter was outside our observation area but affected the observed population, causing mild damage but no casualties; and (4) a power outage (blackout) affecting a major metropolitan area ( File S1, Fig. B). To distinguish emergencies from other events that cause collective changes in human activity, we also explored eight planned events, such as sports games and a popular local sports race and several rock concerts. We discuss here in detail a cultural festival and a large pop music concert as non-emergency references (Table 1, see also File S1, Sec. B). The characteristics of the events not discussed here due to length limitations are provided in File S1, Sec. I for completeness and comparison.

thumbnail
Table 1. Summary of the studied emergencies and non-emergencies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017680.t001

Results and Discussion

As shown in Fig. 1A, emergencies trigger a sharp spike in call activity (number of outgoing calls and text messages) in the physical proximity of the event, confirming that mobile phones act as sensitive local “sociometers” to external societal perturbations. The call volume starts decaying immediately after the emergency, suggesting that the urge to communicate is strongest right at the onset of the event. We see virtually no delay between the onset of the event and the jump in call volume for events that were directly witnessed by the local population, such as the bombing, the earthquake and the blackout. Brief delay is observed only for the plane crash, which took place in an unpopulated area and thus lacked eyewitnesses. In contrast, non-emergency events, like the festival and the concert in Fig. 1A, display a gradual increase in call activity, a noticeably different pattern from the “jump-decay” pattern observed for emergencies. See also File S1, Figs. I and J.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Call anomalies during emergencies.

A, The time dependence of call volume in the vicinity of four emergencies and two non-emergencies (See Table 1). B, The temporal behavior of the relative call volume of the events shown in A, where , Vevent is the call volume on the day of the event (shown in red in A), and is the average call volume during the same period of the week (the call volume during the previous week is shown in black in B). C, The relative change in the average number of calls placed per user () and the total number of users () making calls from the region indicates that the call anomaly is primarily due to a significant increase in the number of users that place calls during the events.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017680.g001

To compare the magnitude and duration of the observed call anomalies, in Fig. 1B we show the temporal evolution of the relative call volume as a function of time, where , Vevent is the call activity during the event and is the average call activity during the same time period of the week. As Fig. 1B indicates, the magnitude of correlates with our relative (and somewhat subjective) sense of the event's potential severity and unexpectedness: the bombing induces the largest change in call activity, followed by the plane crash; whereas the collective reaction to the earthquake and the blackout are somewhat weaker and comparable to each other. While the relative change was also significant for non-emergencies, the emergence of the call anomaly is rather gradual and spans seven or more hours, in contrast with the jump-decay pattern lasting only three to five hours for emergencies (Figs. 1B, File S1, Figs. I and J). As we show in Fig. 1C (see also File S1, Sec. C) the primary source of the observed call anomaly is a sudden increase of calls by individuals who would normally not use their phone during the emergency period, rather than increased call volume by those that are normally active in the area.

The temporally localized spike in call activity (Fig. 1A,B) raises an important question: is information about the events limited to the immediate vicinity of the emergency or do emergencies, often immediately covered by national media, lead to spatially extended changes in call activity [23]? We therefore inspected the change in call activity in the vicinity of the epicenter, finding that for the bombing, for example, the magnitude of the call anomaly is strongest near the event, and drops rapidly with the distance from the epicenter (Fig. 2A). To quantify this effect across all emergencies, we integrated the call volume over time in concentric shells of radius centered on the epicenter (Fig. 2B). The decay is approximately exponential, , allowing us to characterize the spatial extent of the reaction with a decay rate (Fig. 2C). The observed decay rates range from km (bombing) to 10 km (plane crash), indicating that the anomalous call activity is limited to the event's vicinity. An extended spatial range ( km) is seen only for the earthquake, lacking a narrowly defined epicenter. Meanwhile, a distinguishing pattern of non-emergencies is their highly localized nature: they are characterized by a decay rate of less than km, implying that the call anomaly was narrowly confined to the venue of the event. This systematic split in between the spatially extended emergencies and well-localized non-emergencies persists for all explored events (see Table 1, File S1, Fig. K).

thumbnail
Figure 2. The spatial impact of an emergency.

A, Maps of total anomalous call activity (activity during the event minus expected normal activity) for two-hour periods before (), during (), and after () the bombing. The color code corresponds to the total change , where the sum runs over the particular time period. B, Changes in call volume in regions at various distances from the event epicenter. Note that the peak of the call volume anomaly for the bombing within the observed km region is delayed by approximately 10 minutes compared to the km epicenter region. No call anomaly is observed for km. The earthquake covers a large spatial range so we instead choose three event regions A–C, at distances of 310 km, 340 km, and 425 km from the seismic epicenter (which was outside the studied region). C, To measure the distance dependence of the anomaly, we computed the total anomalous call volume in B before () and after () each event as a function of the distance , revealing approximately exponential decay, . Non-emergencies are spatially localized, with km.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017680.g002

Despite the clear temporal and spatial localization of anomalous call activity during emergencies, one expects some degree of information propagation beyond the eyewitness population [26]. We therefore identified the individuals located within the event region , as well as a group consisting of individuals outside the event region but who receive calls from the group during the event, a group that receive calls from , and so on. We see that the individuals engage their social network within minutes, and that the , , and occasionally even the group show an anomalous call pattern immediately after the anomaly (Fig. 3A). This effect is quantified in Fig. 3B, where we show the increase in call volume for each group as a function of their social network based distance from the epicenter (for example, the social distance of the group is 2, being two links away from the group), indicating that the bombing and plane crash show strong, immediate social propagation up to the third and second neighbors of the eyewitness population, respectively. The earthquake and blackout, less threatening emergencies, show little propagation beyond the immediate social links of and social propagation is virtually absent in non-emergencies.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Social characteristics of information cascades.

A, Changes in call volume for users directly affected by the event (), users that receive calls from but are not near the event (), users contacted by but not in or (), etc. For the bombing and plane crash, populations respond very rapidly, within minutes. B, The total amount of anomalous call activity in A before (during ) and after (during ) the event for each user group quantifies the impact on the social network. We see that information propagates deeply into the social network for the bombing and plane crash. C, Top panel: the contact network formed between affected users during the bombing. Bottom panel: the call pattern between users that are active during the emergency during the previous week, indicating that the information cascade observed during the bombing is out of the ordinary. D, The distribution of shortest paths within the contact network quantifies the anomalous information cascade induced by the bombing.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017680.g003

The nature of the information cascade behind the results shown in Fig. 3A,B is illustrated in Fig. 3C, where we show the individual calls between users active during the bombing. In contrast with the information cascade triggered by the emergencies witnessed by the users, there are practically no calls between the same individuals during the previous week. To quantify the magnitude of the information cascade we measured the length of the paths emanating from the users, finding them to be considerably longer during the emergency (Fig. 3D), compared to five non-emergency periods, demonstrating that the information cascade penetrates deep into the social network, a pattern that is absent during normal activity [27]. See also File S1, Figs. E, F, G, H, L, M, N, and O, and Table A.

The existence of such prominent information cascades raises tantalizing questions about who contributes to information propagation about the emergency. Using self-reported gender information available for most users (see File S1), we find that during emergencies female users are more likely to make a call than expected based on their normal call patterns. This gender discrepancy holds for the (eyewitness) and groups, but is absent for non-emergency events (see File S1, Sec. E, Fig. C). We also separated the total call activity of and individuals into voice and text messages (including SMS and MMS). For most events (the earthquake and blackout being the only exceptions), the voice/text ratios follow the normal patterns ( File S1, Fig. D), indicating that users continue to rely on their preferred means of communication during an emergency.

The patterns identified discussed above allow us to dissect complex events, such as an explosion in an urban area preceded by an evacuation starting approximately one hour before the blast. While a call volume anomaly emerges right at the start of the evacuation, it levels off and the jump-decay pattern characteristic of an emergency does not appear until the real explosion (Fig. 4A). The spatial extent of the evacuation response is significantly smaller than the one observed during the event ( for the evacuation compared with for the explosion, see Fig. 4B). During the evacuation, social propagation is limited to the and groups only (Fig. 4C,D) while after the explosion we observe a communication cascade that activates the users as well. The lack of strong propagation during evacuation indicates that individuals tend to be reactive rather than proactive and that a real emergency is necessary to initiate a communication cascade that effectively spreads emergency information.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Analyzing a composite event (evacuation preceding an explosion).

A, Call activity increases during the evacuation () but levels off after the initial warning, until the explosion at causes a much larger increase in call activity. B, Spatially, the evacuation causes a sharply localized activity spike ( km), but the explosion increases the spatial extent dramatically ( km). C–D, The evacuation only activates the (eyewitness) and groups, meaning that information fails to propagate significantly beyond the initial group and their immediate ties. However, the blast not only leads to a further increase in call activity in the and groups, but also triggers the second neighbors .

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017680.g004

The results of Figs. 14 not only indicate that the collective response of the population to an emergency follows reproducible patterns common across diverse events, but they also document subtle differences between emergencies and non-emergencies. We therefore identified four variables that take different characteristic values for emergencies and non-emergencies: (i) the midpoint fraction , where and are the times when the anomalous activity begins and ends, respectively, and is the time when half of the total anomalous call volume has occurred; (ii) the spatial decay rate capturing the extent of the event; (iii) the relative size of each information cascade, representing the ratio between the number of users in the event cascade and the cascade tracked during normal periods; (iv) the probability for users to contact existing friends (instead of placing calls to strangers).

In Fig. 5 we show these variables for all 16 events, finding systematic differences between emergencies and non-emergencies. As the figure indicates, a multidimensional variable, relying on the documented changes in human activity, can be used to automatically distinguish emergency situations from non-emergency induced anomalies. Such a variable could also help real-time monitoring of emergencies [24], from information about the size of the affected population, to the timeline of the events, and could help identify mobile phone users capable of offering immediate, actionable information, potentially aiding search and rescue.

thumbnail
Figure 5. Systematic response mechanisms during emergencies.

A, The midpoint fraction quantifying the onset speed of anomalous call activity (a lower indicates a faster onset). Emergencies display a more abrupt call anomaly than non-emergencies, which feature gradual buildups of anomalous call activity. B, The spatial extent of the events, quantified by , indicates that non-emergency events are far more centrally localized than unexpected emergencies. C, The relative cascade size , where is the number of users in the social cascade. D, , where is the probability of calling an acquaintance and is the standard deviation of .

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017680.g005

Rapidly-evolving events such as those studied throughout this work require dynamical data with ultra-high temporal and spatial resolution and high coverage. Although the populations affected by emergencies are quite large, occasionally reaching thousands of users, due to the demonstrated localized nature of the anomaly, this size is still small in comparison to other proxy studies of human dynamics, which can exploit the activity patterns of millions of internet users or webpages [13], [14], [15], [27]. Meanwhile, emergencies occur over very short timespans, a few hours at most, whereas much current work on human dynamics relies on longitudinal datasets covering months or even years of activity for the same users (e.g. [3], [6], [9]), integrating out transient events and noise. But in the case of emergencies, such transient events are precisely what we wish to quantify. Given the short duration and spatially localized nature of these events, it is vital to have extremely high coverage of the entire system, to maximize the availability of critical information during an event. To push human dynamics research into such fast-moving events requires new tools and datasets capable of extracting signals from limited data. We believe that our research offers a first step in this direction.

In summary, similar to how biologists use drugs to perturb the state of a cell to better understand the collective behavior of living systems, we used emergencies as external societal perturbations, helping us uncover generic changes in the spatial, temporal and social activity patterns of the human population. Starting from a large-scale, country-wide mobile phone dataset, we used news reports to gather a corpus of sixteen major events, eight unplanned emergencies and eight scheduled activities. Studying the call activity patterns of users in the vicinity of these events, we found that unusual activity rapidly spikes for emergencies in contrast with non-emergencies induced anomalies that build up gradually before the event; that the call patterns during emergencies are exponentially localized regardless of event details; and that affected users will only invoke the social network to propagate information under the most extreme circumstances. When this social propagation does occur, however, it takes place in a very rapid and efficient manner, so that users three or even four degrees from eyewitnesses can learn of the emergency within minutes.

These results not only deepen our fundamental understanding of human dynamics, but could also improve emergency response. Indeed, while aid organizations increasingly use the distributed, real-time communication tools of the 21st century, much disaster research continues to rely on low-throughput, post-event data, such as questionnaires, eyewitness reports [28], [29], and communication records between first responders or relief organizations [30]. The emergency situations explored here indicate that, thanks to the pervasive use of mobile phones, collective changes in human activity patterns can be captured in an objective manner, even at surprisingly short time-scales, opening a new window on this neglected chapter of human dynamics.

Materials and Methods

Dataset

We use a set of anonymized billing records from a western european mobile phone service provider [7], [3], [6]. The records cover approximately 10M subscribers within a single country over 3 years of activity. Each billing record, for voice and text services, contains the unique identifiers of the caller placing the call and the callee receiving the call; an identifier for the cellular antenna (tower) that handled the call; and the date and time when the call was placed. Coupled with a dataset describing the locations (latitude and longitude) of cellular towers, we have the approximate location of the caller when placing the call. For full details, see File S1, Sec. A.

Identifying events

To find an event in the mobile phone data, we need to determine its time and location. We have used online news aggregators, particularly the local news.google.com service to search for news stories covering the country and time frame of the dataset. Keywords such as ‘storm’, ‘emergency’, ‘concert’, etc. were used to find potential news stories. Important events such as bombings and earthquakes are prominently covered in the media and are easy to find. Study of these reports, which often included photographs of the affected area, typically yields precise times and locations for the events. Reports would occasionally conflict about specific details, but this was rare. We take the reported start time of the event as .

To identify the beginning and ending of an event, and , we adopt the following procedure. First, identify the event region (a rough estimate is sufficient) and scan all its calls during a large time period covering the event (e.g., a full day), giving . Then, scan calls for a number of “normal” periods, those modulo one week from the event period, exploiting the weekly periodicity of . These normal periods' time series are averaged to give . (To smooth time series, we typically bin them into 5–10 minute intervals.) The standard deviation as a function of time is then used to compute . Finally, we define the interval as the longest contiguous run of time intervals where , for some fixed cutoff . We chose for all events.

For full details, see File S1, Sec. B.

Supporting Information

Acknowledgments

The authors thank A. Pawling, F. Simini, M. C. González, S. Lehmann, R. Menezes, N. Blumm, C. Song, J. P. Huang, Y.-Y. Ahn, P. Wang, R. Crane, D. Sornette, and D. Lazer for many useful discussions.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: JPB DW ALB. Performed the experiments: JPB DW. Analyzed the data: JPB DW. Wrote the paper: JPB DW ALB.

References

  1. 1. Vespignani A (2009) Predicting the Behavior of Techno-Social Systems. Science 325: 425–428.
  2. 2. Brockmann D, Hufnagel L, Geisel T (2006) The scaling laws of human travel. Nature 439: 462–465.
  3. 3. González MC, Hidalgo CA, Barabási AL (2008) Understanding individual human mobility patterns. Nature 453: 779–782.
  4. 4. Eubank S, Guclu H, Kumar VSA, Marathe MV, Srinivasan A, et al. (2004) Modelling disease outbreaks in realistic urban social networks. Nature 429: 180–184.
  5. 5. Lazer D, Pentland A, Adamic L, Aral S, Barabási AL, et al. (2009) Computational Social Science. Science 323: 721–723.
  6. 6. Song C, Qu Z, Blumm N, Barabási AL (2010) Limits of Predictability in Human Mobility. Science 327: 1018–1021.
  7. 7. Onnela JP, Saramäki J, Hyvönen J, Szabó G, Lazer D, et al. (2007) Structure and tie strengths in mobile communication networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104: 7332–7336.
  8. 8. Gabrielli A, Caldarelli G (2009) Invasion percolation and the time scaling behavior of a queuing model of human dynamics. Journal of Statistical Mechanics: Theory and Experiment 2009: P02046.
  9. 9. Rybski D, Buldyrev SV, Havlin S, Liljeros F, Makse HA (2009) Scaling laws of human interaction activity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106: 12640–12645.
  10. 10. Singer HM, Singer I, Herrmann HJ (2009) Agent-based model for friendship in social networks. Phys Rev E 80: 026113.
  11. 11. Hufnagel L, Brockmann D, Geisel T (2004) Forecast and control of epidemics in a globalized world. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101: 15124–15129.
  12. 12. Colizza V, Barrat A, Barthélemy M, Vespignani A (2006) The role of the airline transportation network in the prediction and predictability of global epidemics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103: 2015–2020.
  13. 13. Wu F, Huberman B (2007) Novelty and collective attention Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104: 17599–17601.
  14. 14. Gonçalves B, Ramasco J (2008) Human dynamics revealed through Web analytics. Phys Rev E 78: 026123.
  15. 15. Ratkiewicz J, Fortunato S, Flammini A, Menczer F, Vespignani A (2010) Characterizing and Modeling the Dynamics of Online Popularity. Phys Rev Lett 105: 158701.
  16. 16. Bohorquez J, Gourley S, Dixon A, Spagat M, Johnson N (2009) Common ecology quantifies human insurgency. Nature 462: 911–914.
  17. 17. Balcan D, Colizza V, Gonçalves B, Hu H, Ramasco JJ, et al. (2009) Multiscale mobility networks and the spatial spreading of infectious diseases. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106: 21484–21489.
  18. 18. Caldarelli G (2007) Scale-Free Networks. Oxford University Press.
  19. 19. Centola D (2010) The Spread of Behavior in an Online Social Network Experiment. Science 329: 1194–1197.
  20. 20. Helbing D, Farkas I, Vicsek T (2000) Simulating dynamical features of escape panic. Nature 407: 487–490.
  21. 21. Kaplan EH, Craft DL, Wein LM (2002) Emergency response to a smallpox attack: The case for mass vaccination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 99: 10935–10940.
  22. 22. Helbing D, Johansson A, Mathiesen J, Jensen MH, Hansen A (2006) Analytical approach to continuous and intermittent bottleneck flows. Phys Rev Lett 97: 168001.
  23. 23. Petrescu-Prahova M, Butts CT (2008) Emergent coordinators in the World Trade Center disaster. International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters 28: 133–168.
  24. 24. Kapoor A, Eagle N, Horvitz E (2010) People, quakes, and communications: Inferences from call dynamics about a seismic event and its influences on a population. Proceedings of AAAI Artificial Intelligence for Development (AI-D'10). pp. 51–56.
  25. 25. Lambiotte R, Blondel VD, de Kerchove C, Huens E, Prieur C, et al. (2008) Geographical dispersal of mobile communication networks. Physica A 387: 5317–5325.
  26. 26. Crane R, Sornette D (2008) Robust dynamic classes revealed by measuring the response function of a social system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105: 15649–15653.
  27. 27. Onnela JP, Reed-Tsochas F (2010) Spontaneous emergence of social influence in online systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
  28. 28. Sheetz S, Kavanaugh AL, Quek F, Kim BJ, Lu SC (2010) The expectation of connectedness and cell phone use in crises. Journal of Emergency Management.
  29. 29. Rodriguez H, Quarantelli EL, Dynes R, editors. (2006) Handbook of Disaster Research. Springer, 1st edition.
  30. 30. Lind BE, Tirado M, Butts CT, Petrescu-Prahova M (2008) Brokerage roles in disaster response: Organizational mediation in the wake of Hurricane Katrina. International Journal of Emergency Management 5: 75–99.