Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Sleep Deprivation Influences Diurnal Variation of Human Time Perception with Prefrontal Activity Change: A Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Study

  • Takahiro Soshi,

    Affiliations Department of Psychophysiology, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, National Institute of Mental Health, Tokyo, Japan, Department of Adult Mental Health, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, National Institute of Mental Health, Tokyo, Japan

  • Kenichi Kuriyama ,

    kenichik@ncnp.go.jp

    Affiliations Department of Psychophysiology, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, National Institute of Mental Health, Tokyo, Japan, Department of Adult Mental Health, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, National Institute of Mental Health, Tokyo, Japan

  • Sayaka Aritake,

    Affiliation Department of Psychophysiology, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, National Institute of Mental Health, Tokyo, Japan

  • Minori Enomoto,

    Affiliation Department of Psychophysiology, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, National Institute of Mental Health, Tokyo, Japan

  • Akiko Hida,

    Affiliation Department of Psychophysiology, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, National Institute of Mental Health, Tokyo, Japan

  • Miyuki Tamura,

    Affiliation Department of Psychophysiology, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, National Institute of Mental Health, Tokyo, Japan

  • Yoshiharu Kim,

    Affiliation Department of Adult Mental Health, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, National Institute of Mental Health, Tokyo, Japan

  • Kazuo Mishima

    Affiliation Department of Psychophysiology, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, National Institute of Mental Health, Tokyo, Japan

Sleep Deprivation Influences Diurnal Variation of Human Time Perception with Prefrontal Activity Change: A Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Study

  • Takahiro Soshi, 
  • Kenichi Kuriyama, 
  • Sayaka Aritake, 
  • Minori Enomoto, 
  • Akiko Hida, 
  • Miyuki Tamura, 
  • Yoshiharu Kim, 
  • Kazuo Mishima
PLOS
x

Abstract

Human short-time perception shows diurnal variation. In general, short-time perception fluctuates in parallel with circadian clock parameters, while diurnal variation seems to be modulated by sleep deprivation per se. Functional imaging studies have reported that short-time perception recruits a neural network that includes subcortical structures, as well as cortical areas involving the prefrontal cortex (PFC). It has also been reported that the PFC is vulnerable to sleep deprivation, which has an influence on various cognitive functions. The present study is aimed at elucidating the influence of PFC vulnerability to sleep deprivation on short-time perception, using the optical imaging technique of functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Eighteen participants performed 10-s time production tasks before (at 21:00) and after (at 09:00) experimental nights both in sleep-controlled and sleep-deprived conditions in a 4-day laboratory-based crossover study. Compared to the sleep-controlled condition, one-night sleep deprivation induced a significant reduction in the produced time simultaneous with an increased hemodynamic response in the left PFC at 09:00. These results suggest that activation of the left PFC, which possibly reflects functional compensation under a sleep-deprived condition, is associated with alteration of short-time perception.

Introduction

Temporal perception is fundamental to environmental adaptation. Developing time management skills enables us not only to avoid life-threatening situations, but also to gain rewards and establish motor and cognitive skills. Higher organisms have at least two endogenous clock systems [1], [2]. One of these is a circadian pacemaker located in the suprachiasmatic nucleus of the hypothalamus [3], [4] which is driven by a self-sustaining oscillator with a period of about 24 h and provides the time of day as the hour hand of the clock [5]. Another is a stopwatch-like system which perceives brief temporal intervals as the minute hand of the clock [1], [6].

Most studies in human time perception refer to the issue that time perception is sensitive to the length of duration perceived, where the perception of a longer interval with a range of several minutes to hours (long-time perception) [7] shows different properties from the perception of a shorter interval with a range of seconds or within a minute (short-time perception), as well as from the perception of a sub-second interval [8][10]. Short-time perception robustly reflects the rate of the stopwatch-like system [8], [11], [12], being shortened or prolonged when the system “speeds up” or “slows down”, respectively [13]. When the stopwatch-like system speeds up, the duration of events may be felt as more extended than the real interval: That may equal to “time expansion”, which enables us to deal with more things or to perform more rapidly.

Neural correlates of the stopwatch-like system are suspected to include subcortical (cerebellum and basal ganglia) structures in addition to the right prefrontal cortex (PFC) [8], [9], [14], [15]. A three-stage model of temporal processing proposed by Pouthas et al. [9] comprises a first clock stage in which an endogenous clock system measures primitive timing intervals, a second storage stage in which measuring time is stored in short-term working memory and a third decision stage in which temporal judgment is finally made in reference to short- and long-term temporal memories. A similar model of temporal processing has been proposed by other researchers [16][18], and consensus has almost been reached that the cerebellum and the basal ganglia may contribute to a basic timing process that corresponds to the first stage of the three-stage model [19], [20]. The right PFC, on the other hand, is implicated in making judgments with working memory or attentional processes contributing to temporal processing [9], [21][23], although some researchers regard right PFC activity as constituting temporal processing per se [24], [25].

Biological rhythm studies have reported that short-time perception driven by the stopwatch-like system is not independent of the influence of the circadian pacemaker; short-time perception correlates with circadian markers such as core body temperature and melatonin, showing diurnal variation in consequence [7], [13], [26][28]. On the other hand, after sleep deprivation, there is less diurnal variation in short-time perception dissociating with endogenous circadian markers [27], [29], [30]; the biological stopwatch is “sped up” as a result [13].

The present study aims at investigating neural responsibility for attenuation of the diurnal variation in short-time perception after sleep deprivation, utilizing the representative experimental paradigm of the time production (TP) method. The hypothesis is that an attenuated variation of short-time perception is associated with an alteration in PFC activity as a consequence of sleep deprivation. With regard to neural vulnerability to sleep deprivation, the PFC shows complicated hemodynamic or metabolic patterns during cognitive performance after sleep deprivation [31][33]. In this state, the PFC shows hypoactivity during working memory or arithmetic tasks [34], [35] but hyperactivity during verbal learning or attention-loaded tasks, as well as verbal working memory tasks [36][38]. Although hemodynamic or metabolic responses to sleep deprivation on various cognitive tasks may be influenced by the task difficulty and weight of contribution from PFC activity [37], the neural vulnerability of the PFC to sleep deprivation must have consistent repercussions on cognitive performance. Taken together, the vulnerability of the PFC to sleep deprivation presumably influences short-time perception.

We utilize the brain imaging method of functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) for the experimental purpose. fNIRS is a noninvasive optical imaging technique to measure changes of cerebral blood flow and volume through fluctuations in local glucose and oxygen coupled with neural electric activity [39], [40]. It is well suited to sleep deprivation studies because it neither produces high noise levels on scanning nor requires that participants severely restrict their body movement, and thus is unlikely to seriously influence the sleep-deprived condition. It is also suitable for monitoring the participants' condition while they are performing tasks.

Results

Sleep Data

Nocturnal sleep duration on the interval day between the first and the second experiments for two different sleep schedules [sleep-controlled (SC) - sleep-deprived (SD), and SD - SC] was not significantly different [SC-SD (8.01±0.96 h) vs. SD-SC (8.70±1.72 h); t(1,16) = 1.102, p = 0.287]. Sleep duration between the first and the second TP sessions during the participants' stay in the laboratory also did not differ significantly between the two sleep schedules [SC-SD (6.40±0.18 h) vs. SD-SC (6.63±0.11 h); t(1,16) = 1.145, p = 0.269].

Time Production Data

Produced time in the SC condition was 11.16±0.27 s on day 1 and 11.60±0.33 s on day 2, and in the SD condition was 11.14±0.24 s on day 1 and 10.94±0.35 s on day 2 (Fig. 1).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Mean interval variation in SC and SD conditions (n = 18).

Black filled triangles with error bars (left: day 1, right: day 2) and the linking line indicate mean produced intervals and the trend of fluctuation in the SC condition, respectively. Gray filled circles with error bars (left: day 1, right: day 2) and the linking line show mean produced intervals and the trend of variation in the SD condition, respectively. Solid curves connecting two markers indicate significant differences (p<0.05).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0008395.g001

A significant interaction of condition × day was obtained [F(1,16) = 9.888, p = 0.006]. Pair-wise comparisons of two levels within the factor of day in each condition revealed that the produced time on day 2 was significantly longer than that on day 1 in the SC condition [F(1,17) = 4.575, p = 0.047]. In the SD condition, on the other hand, the produced time on day 2 was not significantly different from that on day 1 [F(1,17) = 0.753, p = 0.398]. Produced time on day 2 in the SD condition, relative to that in the SC condition, was significantly attenuated [F(1,17) = 6.123, p = 0.024].

There was no significant interaction of the between-subject factor of schedule [condition × schedule: F(1,16) = 0.254, p = 0.621; condition × day × schedule: F(1,16) = 0.755, p = 0.398]. These findings taken together with those for sleep data indicate that time production performance was not influenced by sleep schedule.

fNIRS Data

We conducted statistical analyses for fNIRS data utilizing mean values of changes in oxygenated hemoglobin (oxy-Hb) concentration. The overall ANOVA for the midline area did not show any significant difference [condition: F(1,13) = 0.322, p = 0.580; condition × day: F(1,13) = 1.715, p = 0.213; condition × channel: F(3,39) = 0.458, p = 0.713; condition × day × channel: F(3,39) = 1.777, p = 0.167]. The overall ANOVA for the lateral site showed only a significant interaction for condition × day × hemisphere [F(1,13) = 5.373, p = 0.037], suggesting that PFC activity in the SD condition, compared with that in the SC condition, was more enhanced in the left hemisphere on day 2 (Fig. 2B). Subsequent planed ANOVAs confirmed that there was a significant difference between conditions on day 2 [condition × hemisphere: F(1,26) = 9.049, p = 0.006], but not on day 1 [condition × hemisphere: F(1,26) = 0.024, p = 0.879]. Additional ANOVAs showed that the more enhanced PFC activation in the SD condition on day 2 appeared in the left hemisphere [F(1,52) = 6.142, p = 0.017], but not in the right hemisphere [F(1,52) = 0.225, p = 0.637].

thumbnail
Figure 2. Time course of grand averaged oxygenated hemoglobin (oxy-Hb) concentration changes during 10-s time production trial in the left anterior prefrontal (LAPFC) (n = 14).

Comparison of oxy-Hb concentration changes between the SC (green line) and the SD (red line) conditions on (A) day 1 and (B) day 2 in the LAPFC (chs.17, 21, 22). Scores of 0, 11 and 27 below the waveform box indicate the onset of the trial, the end of the trial, and the finish of the post-trial rest, respectively. Oxy-Hb changes are given as z-scores. Any significant differences in oxy-Hb concentration changes between the SC and the SD conditions in the right hemisphere except for in the left hemisphere are not observed on day 2. Marked differences in oxy-Hb concentration changes between conditions are observed in the LAPFC on day 2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0008395.g002

Enhanced oxy-Hb concentration changes on day 2 in the SD condition, compared with those in the SC condition, were observed in the left anterior PFC (LAPFC) region of interest (ROI) including chs. 17, 21 and 22 (see Fig. 3A). Comparing the mean change in concentration in the SD condition with that in the SC condition, a significant effect was observed in the overall ROI [t(1,13) = 2.810, p = 0.015 (p<0.0375: adjusted α level under the control of the false discovery rate; FDR)]. Each channel in the ROI also showed a significant difference [ch.22: t(1,13) = 3.066, p = 0.009 (p<0.0125: FDR); ch.17: t(1,13) = 2.872, p = 0.013 (p<0.025: FDR)] or trend [ch.21: t(1,13) = 2.007, p = 0.066 (p<0.05: FDR)]. A significant positive correlation was observed between produced time and change in oxy-Hb concentration in the overall ROI on day 2 in the SD condition [r = 0.535, p = 0.049; Fig. 3B].

thumbnail
Figure 3. Change in hemodynamic response and functional correlation in prefrontal cortex (PFC) activity after sleep deprivation.

(A) Topographic image representing increased activation in the PFC on day 2 in the SD condition (n = 14). The image is constructed by using subtracted values between the SC and the SD conditions (SD minus SC). The area of dotted white circles emphasizes the left anterior PFC (LAPFC) including chs. 17, 21 and 22 for region of interest (ROI) analysis. (B) Correlation between normalized produced intervals and oxy-Hb concentration changes in the LAPFC ROI on day 2 in the SD condition (n = 14). Functional connectivity in changes of short-time perception with LAPFC activity under sustained wakefulness is suggested by a correlation coefficient (r) of 0.535 with statistical significance (p<0.05).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0008395.g003

Discussion

Sleep is believed to be a neural state during which both consolidation of memories and homeostatic preservations are taking place [41][44]. Sleep deprivation, even for the course of an extended active period of the day, eliminates these effects, and possibly results in a deterioration in cognitive activity [13], [30], [45].

The present study demonstrated that sleep deprivation modulates short-time perception behavior, as was suggested by earlier studies [27], [30]. It has been shown previously that a short-time perception profile exhibits diurnal variation, reaching a peak (the longest produced time) around 09:00 and a nadir (the shortest produced time) around 21:00 with a regular sleep-wake cycle under experimental conditions [13]. Here, we measured produced time around both these expected peak and nadir periods in order to obtain approximate peak-to-peak amplitude of the diurnal variation of short-time perception. As a result, we found that the produced time at 09:00 was significantly attenuated after sleep deprivation compared with that after physiological sleep. It was unlikely that the alteration in properties of time perception after sleep deprivation was caused by circadian phase shifting (different peak times) of short-time perception rhythms between the SC and SD sessions because all participants were exposed to similar time cues such as dim light (<100 lux) and prohibited from physical exercise throughout the experiments, and the produced time levels around 21:00 were in fact identical between the two sessions. This seems to indicate that sleep deprivation per se modulated short-time perception. Given that produced time represents the rate of the biological stopwatch-like system [1], the attenuation of produced time observed on day 2 in the SD condition might be associated with the stopwatch-like system that runs faster [13].

A functional correlation of increased activation of the LAPFC after sleep deprivation with short-time perception behavior was observed in the present study, although unlike in previous studies, we failed to detect any significant changes in right PFC activity [8], [9], [14], [15]. This discrepancy in the localized PFC activation findings between the present and previous studies is possibly attributable to experimental settings, tasks used and imaging modalities [9], [14], [24], [46]. It has also been argued that increased activation of the PFC after sleep deprivation is associated with neural compensation for cognitive function [36], [38], [47], and the LAPFC activity observed in the present study should also be argued in the context of neural compensation: the LAPFC activity likely contributed to the third decision stage of temporal judgment in the three-stage model [9], with adaptive neural activity in the SD condition, although the produced time on day 2 in the SD condition was different from that in the SC condition.

It remains open as to whether or not a subcortical network including the cerebellum and the basal ganglia, assumed to be associated with timing processing, contributes to attenuating the diurnal fluctuation of short-time perception. Sustained wakefulness affects dopaminergic neuronal activity related with cognitive function as well as temporal function [48][50]. If alteration of subcortical activities might be caused by sleep deprivation, attenuation of short-time perception probably reflects subcortical vulnerability, and the change in PFC activity is possibly a by-product. Further studies are warranted to elucidate the processes involved.

As has been reported, short-time perception is modulated by affective states [51], an intense fever [52], [53] and psychotic disorders [54], [55]. Diurnal variation of short-time perception associated with the rest-activity cycle is expedient for adapting in daily life, similarly to the vegetative functions in blood circulation [56], aspiration [57], assimilation [58] or incretion [59], [60]. These functions robustly synchronize to diurnal variation oscillated by a circadian pacemaker, and simultaneously prepare for desynchronization from diurnal variation under stressful conditions. Temporarily collapsing the diurnal variation of short-time perception may be an important function for surviving a crisis, such as in a situation with an acute emergency. Time perception in humans, as well as in other organisms, should be fundamentally synchronized to the physical state for constant adaptation to daily lifestyle regularity. However, once we are placed under stress, time perception must desynchronize from regular physical homeostasis and be shortened enabling time expansion and assumedly allowing us to adopt suitable strategies for coping with a stressful environment by doing or thinking of things more rapidly. Serious consideration of an adaptive nature of the human PFC function [61], [62] suggests that the PFC might play a switch-like role in short-time perception as a situation demands, to meet the demands for adaptation.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Eighteen healthy right-handed males (mean age 22.4 yr; range 20–28 yr) participated in laboratory-based experiments in a 4-day protocol. A psychiatrist conducted a medical examination to confirm that none of the participants had a history of neurological or psychiatric disorder or abused psychoactive drugs. Participants were asked to avoid intake of caffeine, nicotine and alcohol for one week prior to and during the experiment. For one week before the experiment, they were also required to keep a regular sleep-wake habit in which they slept from around 00:00 and awoke at around 08:00, as confirmed by a sleep log and an activity recorder (Actiwatch-L, Mini-Mitter Co., Inc. Bend, OR). The mean habitual sleep duration was 6.5±0.1 h, and habitual sleep onset and wake times were 00:34±00:13 and 07:22±00:10, respectively. All procedures for the study were carried out in accordance with the principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki. The experimental protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Japan. All participants gave written informed consent to take part in the study.

Experimental Protocol

All participants performed a 10-s TP task in the SC and SD conditions, scheduled in random order with a 1-day interval (Fig. 4). On the first day (day 1) in both the SC and SD conditions, participants arrived at the laboratory at about 18:00. fNIRS probe and optodes were attached to the surface of the scalp approximately one hour before the start of the TP session (at 20:00). The session, which lasted approximately 15 min, generally started around 21:00–22:00. After the end of the session, in the SC condition, the participants rested without sleep and exercise until 00:00 and then stayed in bed under complete darkness (<0.1 lux) until 08:00 on the second day (day 2). In contrast, in the SD condition, participants were asked to stay awake quietly under room light (100 lux) until 08:00 the next morning. During the nighttime period of enforced wakefulness, researchers monitored the participants' status via a video monitoring system. On day 2, the TP session started again around 09:00–10:00. All the experiments were performed at the time isolation facility of the National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry. The ambient temperature and humidity in the time isolation facility were maintained at 24.0±0.5°C and 60.0±5%, respectively, and temporal information was not provided to participants throughout the experimental schedule.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Experimental protocol.

Time production sessions (TPS) were conducted twice at around 21:00 on day 1 and about 09:00 on day 2 in the sleep-controlled (SC) and the sleep-deprived (SD) settings. The two conditions were randomly scheduled with a 1-day interval. Each TPS included 15 trials, each of which participants started and ended with their own button presses. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) data were recorded continuously through the sessions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0008395.g004

Time Production Task

TP tasks were arranged in an event-related design for detecting the hemodynamic response for a single trial. It has been confirmed that output of the task shows diurnal variation correlated with core body temperature or melatonin under a constant condition [7], [13], [26], [28]. It shortens from morning into night, and is prolonged again from night to the next morning [7], [13], [26], [29].On the basis of our previous findings [26], [27], TP sessions in the present study were conducted between 21:00–22:00 on day 1 and 09:00–10:00 on day 2, corresponding to the expected nadir and the peak periods of the diurnal variation of TP in the present participants with a regular sleep-wake cycle, respectively.

Each TP session consisted of 15 trials. The inter-trial interval was about 30 s. Participants were asked to produce a 10-s interval and to begin and end each trial by pressing a key button [13]. Duration from the first to the second button presses was counted as the produced time. Participants were given no feedback about accuracy in the trials.

fNIRS Data Acquisition

Regional hemodynamic changes in brain tissue were monitored throughout the TP sessions by a continuous wave-type fNIRS system (OMM3000; Shimazu Co., Tokyo, Japan), which outputs near-infrared light at three wavelengths (780, 805 and 830 nm). All transmitted intensities of the three wavelengths were recorded every 130 ms at 22 channels in order to estimate concentration changes in oxy-Hb, deoxygenated hemoglobin and total hemoglobin, on the basis of the modified Beer-Lambert equation as a function of light absorbance of Hb and pathlength. A 3×5 optode probe was utilized, in which light detectors and emitters were alternately positioned at an equal distance of 3 cm. Based on the international 10–20 system [63], the lower central edge of each probe was suited above Fpz, along the reference curve of T3 - Fpz - T4. The 22 channels (see Fig. 5) probably covered the middle and superior PFC regions (BA9, 46, 10) [64].

thumbnail
Figure 5. Approximate positions of 22 channels in fNIRS superimposed on the 3D head model.

The 22 measuring channels were produced by optodes placed equidistantly over the prefrontal cortex (PFC) area. The lower line of the 3×5 optode probe was positioned along the reference curve linking T3, T4 and Fpz.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0008395.g005

fNIRS Data Analysis

Oxy-Hb data was chosen to examine event-related response in the PFC since it is an optimal index for changes of regional cerebral blood flow [65]. Data from 4 participants who were recruited to the experiment in the early stage were excluded from fNIRS analyses because they were monitored with a distinct optode probe. We applied a high-pass filter to raw data, re-sampled at 10 Hz, using the low-cutoff frequency of 0.05 Hz. Smoothing was performed by the moving average method (a boxcar filter) with a sliding time window of 1.1 s. Data were converted into z-scores by the normalization process [(XiM)/SD; where Xi is the value at each time point, M is the mean of all points and SD is the standard deviation of all points] [66]. Concentration changes time-locked to trial onset were extracted from 5 s before to 27 s after the onset, covering a mean produced time of about 11 s and a mean rest interval of about 16 s. A total of 15 epochs were obtained for each experimental day (day 1 or day 2) in each condition (SC or SD). Before individual averaging, baselines were corrected with mean z-scores of 5 s before trial onset. Grand averaged concentration changes in the LAPFC region, based on statistical analyses, were superimposed (Fig. 2), and subtracted values were utilized for reconstructing a topographic image (Fig. 3A).

Statistical Analysis

To examine whether the order of the sleep conditions [SC-SD (n = 7) vs. SD-SC (n = 11)] influenced outcomes of time production performance, we compared nocturnal sleep duration in both sleep schedules during the 1-day interval between the first and the second experiments and the sleep duration between the first and the second TP sessions during the participants' stay in the laboratory. We also analyzed the time production data of 18 participants using three-way ANOVA including two within-subject factors of condition (SC and SD) and day (day 1 and day 2), and a between-subject factor of schedule (SC-SD and SD-SC). If significant interactions were obtained, follow-up ANOVAs were conducted.

fNIRS data of 14 participants were tested separately for the lateral and the midline sites by overall ANOVA including within-subject factors of condition, day, hemisphere (left, right) and channel [9 chs: left, 1, 2, 5, 6, 10, 14, 15, 19, 20; right (the corresponding order): 4, 3, 9, 8, 13, 18, 17, 22, 21] for the lateral site, or factors of condition, day and channel (4 chs: 7, 11, 12, 16) for the midline area, utilizing mean z-scores of all temporal points (0–27 s). When the overall ANOVA reached statistical significance of the interaction coefficients, planed ANOVAs were subsequently performed with pooling error terms across the entire analyses, for the purpose to examine our assumption that sleep deprivation modulates the PFC activities on the time production trial. If degrees of freedom exceeded one degree, Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied on the basis of the sphericity test. Depending on the topographic image reconstructed with subtracted values of changes in oxy-Hb concentration (SD minus SC; Fig. 3A), and a confirmed methodology for fNIRS probe placement [64], we defined the LAPFC, including chs. 17, 21 and 22, which corresponds to the medial dorsal part of the PFC (Brodmann's area 10) [64], as the ROI, and further examined the influence of sleep-deprivation on neural response in the LAPFC. Paired t-tests were performed for the ROI and the three constituent channels, with α levels adjusted by Benjamini and Hochberg's method to control the FDR [67]: H(i) denotes the null hypothesis based on the corresponding p-value P(i), i = 1, 2, …, m. P(i) is increasingly ordered such as P(1)<P(2)<…<P(m). The FDR control procedure starts with i = m, and continues to compare P(i) with α(i/m) until P(i) satisfy the constraint P(i)<or = α(i/m). The relation between produced times standardized within each participant and oxy-Hb concentration changes in the ROI on day 2 in the SD condition was tested with Pearson's product-moment correlation coefficient to examine functional connectivity between short TP and neural response of the LAPFC.

Outcomes of the tests for produced time and fNIRS data were considered significant at level α if p<0.05. Results are shown as mean and standard errors (SEs). Statistical analysis was performed by SPSS 12.0 J (SPSS Japan Inc., Tokyo).

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to S. Higuchi, H. Suzuki and M. Abe for valuable comments and discussions during the preparation of the experiment and the paper.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: TS KK SA ME KM. Performed the experiments: TS KK SA ME AH MT KM. Analyzed the data: TS KK KM. Wrote the paper: TS KK KM. Contributed to discussion: TS, KK, YK, KM. Supervised the entire project: KM.

References

  1. 1. Morell V (1996) Setting a biological stopwatch. Science 271: 905–906.V. Morell1996Setting a biological stopwatch.Science271905906
  2. 2. Ruan GX, Allen GC, Yamazaki S, McMahon DG (2008) An autonomous circadian clock in the inner mouse retina regulated by dopamine and GABA. PLoS Biol 6: 2249–2265.GX RuanGC AllenS. YamazakiDG McMahon2008An autonomous circadian clock in the inner mouse retina regulated by dopamine and GABA.PLoS Biol622492265
  3. 3. Inoue ST, Kawamura H (1979) Persistence of circadian rhythmicity in a mammalian hypothalamic ‘island’ containing the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 76: 5962–5966.ST InoueH. Kawamura1979Persistence of circadian rhythmicity in a mammalian hypothalamic ‘island’ containing the suprachiasmatic nucleus.Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A7659625966
  4. 4. Moore RY, Silver R (1998) Suprachiasmatic nucleus organization. Chronobiol Int 15: 475–487.RY MooreR. Silver1998Suprachiasmatic nucleus organization.Chronobiol Int15475487
  5. 5. Moore-Ede MC, Czeisler CA, Richardson GS (1983) Circadian time-keeping in health and disease. Part 1. Basic properties of circadian pacemakers. N Eng J Med 309: 469–476.MC Moore-EdeCA CzeislerGS Richardson1983Circadian time-keeping in health and disease. Part 1. Basic properties of circadian pacemakers.N Eng J Med309469476
  6. 6. Gibbon J, Church RM (1981) Time left: linear versus logarithmic subjective time. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 7: 87–107.J. GibbonRM Church1981Time left: linear versus logarithmic subjective time.J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process787107
  7. 7. Aschoff J (1998) Human perception of short and long time intervals: its correlation with body temperature and the duration of wake time. J Biol Rhythms 13: 437–442.J. Aschoff1998Human perception of short and long time intervals: its correlation with body temperature and the duration of wake time.J Biol Rhythms13437442
  8. 8. Lewis PA, Miall RC (2003) Brain activation patterns during measurement of sub- and supra-second intervals. Neuropsychologia 41: 1583–1592.PA LewisRC Miall2003Brain activation patterns during measurement of sub- and supra-second intervals.Neuropsychologia4115831592
  9. 9. Pouthas V, Geroge N, Poline JB, Pfeuty M, VandeMoorteele PF, et al. (2005) Neural network involved in time perception: an fMRI study comparing long and short interval estimation. Hum Brain Mapp 25: 433–441.V. PouthasN. GerogeJB PolineM. PfeutyPF VandeMoorteele2005Neural network involved in time perception: an fMRI study comparing long and short interval estimation.Hum Brain Mapp25433441
  10. 10. Rammsayer TH (2006) Effects of pharmacologically induced changes in NMDA receptor activity on human timing and sensorimotor performance. Brain Res 1073–1074: 407–416.TH Rammsayer2006Effects of pharmacologically induced changes in NMDA receptor activity on human timing and sensorimotor performance.Brain Res1073–1074407416
  11. 11. Mattel MS, Meck WH (2004) Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence detection of oscillatory processes. Cogn Brain Res 58: 119–124.MS MattelWH Meck2004Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence detection of oscillatory processes.Cogn Brain Res58119124
  12. 12. Rammsayer T, Ulrich R (2005) No evidence for qualitative differences in the processing of short and long temporal intervals. Acta Psychol 120: 141–171.T. RammsayerR. Ulrich2005No evidence for qualitative differences in the processing of short and long temporal intervals.Acta Psychol120141171
  13. 13. Kuriyama K, Uchiyama M, Suzuki H, Tagaya H, Ozaki A, et al. (2005) Diurnal fluctuation of time perception under 30-h sustained wakefulness. Neurosci Res 53: 123–128.K. KuriyamaM. UchiyamaH. SuzukiH. TagayaA. Ozaki2005Diurnal fluctuation of time perception under 30-h sustained wakefulness.Neurosci Res53123128
  14. 14. Maquet P, Lejeune H, Pouthas V, Bonnet M, Casini L, et al. (1996) Brain activation induced by estimation of duration: a PET study. Neuroimage 3: 119–126.P. MaquetH. LejeuneV. PouthasM. BonnetL. Casini1996Brain activation induced by estimation of duration: a PET study.Neuroimage3119126
  15. 15. Rao S, Mayer AR, Harrington DL (2001) The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing. Nat Neurosci 4: 317–323.S. RaoAR MayerDL Harrington2001The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing.Nat Neurosci4317323
  16. 16. Treisman M (1963) Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval implications for a model of the ‘internal clock.’ Psychol Monogr 77 (13): 1–31.M. Treisman1963Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval implications for a model of the ‘internal clock.’Psychol Monogr77 (13)131
  17. 17. Ivry RB, Spencer RM (2004) The neural representation of time. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14: 225–32.RB IvryRM Spencer2004The neural representation of time.Curr Opin Neurobiol1422532
  18. 18. Zakay D, Block RA (2004) Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: an executive-control perspective. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 64: 319–28.D. ZakayRA Block2004Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: an executive-control perspective.Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars)6431928
  19. 19. Casini L, Ivry RB (1999) Effects of divided attention on temporal processing in patients with lesions of the cerebellum or frontal lobe. Neuropsychology 13: 10–21.L. CasiniRB Ivry1999Effects of divided attention on temporal processing in patients with lesions of the cerebellum or frontal lobe.Neuropsychology131021
  20. 20. Lewis PA, Miall RC (2002) Brain activity during non-automatic motor production of discrete multi-second intervals. Neuroreport 13: 1731–1735.PA LewisRC Miall2002Brain activity during non-automatic motor production of discrete multi-second intervals.Neuroreport1317311735
  21. 21. Harrington DL, Haaland KY, Knight RT (1998) Cortical networks underlying mechanisms of time perception. J Neurosci 18: 1085–1095.DL HarringtonKY HaalandRT Knight1998Cortical networks underlying mechanisms of time perception.J Neurosci1810851095
  22. 22. Macar F, Lejeune H, Bonnet M, Ferrara A, Pouthas V, et al. (2002) Activation of the supplementary motor area and of attentional networks during temporal processing. Exp Brain Res 142: 475–485.F. MacarH. LejeuneM. BonnetA. FerraraV. Pouthas2002Activation of the supplementary motor area and of attentional networks during temporal processing.Exp Brain Res142475485
  23. 23. Livesey AC, Wall MB, Smith AT (2007) Time perception: manipulation of task difficulty dissociates clock functions from other cognitive demands. Neuropsychologia 45: 321–331.AC LiveseyMB WallAT Smith2007Time perception: manipulation of task difficulty dissociates clock functions from other cognitive demands.Neuropsychologia45321331
  24. 24. Smith A, Lidzba K, Taylor E, Rubia K (2003) A right hemispheric frontocerebellar network for time discrimination of several hundreds of milliseconds. Neuroimage 20: 344–350.A. SmithK. LidzbaE. TaylorK. Rubia2003A right hemispheric frontocerebellar network for time discrimination of several hundreds of milliseconds.Neuroimage20344350
  25. 25. Rubia K, Smith A (2004) The neural correlates of cognitive time management: a review. Acta Neurobiol Exp 64: 329–340.K. RubiaA. Smith2004The neural correlates of cognitive time management: a review.Acta Neurobiol Exp64329340
  26. 26. Ashoff J, Daan S (1997) Human time perception in temporal isolation: effects of illumination intensity. Choronobiol Int 14: 585–596.J. AshoffS. Daan1997Human time perception in temporal isolation: effects of illumination intensity.Choronobiol Int14585596
  27. 27. Miró E, Cano MC, Espinosa-Fernández L, Buela-Casal G (2003) Time estimation during prolonged sleep deprivation and its relation to activation measures. Hum Factors 45: 148–159.E. MiróMC CanoL. Espinosa-FernándezG. Buela-Casal2003Time estimation during prolonged sleep deprivation and its relation to activation measures.Hum Factors45148159
  28. 28. Kuriyama K, Uchiyama M, Suzuki H, Tagaya H, Ozaki A, et al. (2003) Circadian fluctuation of time perception in healthy human subjects. Neurosci Res 46: 23–31.K. KuriyamaM. UchiyamaH. SuzukiH. TagayaA. Ozaki2003Circadian fluctuation of time perception in healthy human subjects.Neurosci Res462331
  29. 29. Pöppel E, Giedke H (1970) Diurnal variation of time perception. Psychol Forsch 34: 182–198.E. PöppelH. Giedke1970Diurnal variation of time perception.Psychol Forsch34182198
  30. 30. Aschoff J (1988) Circadian timing. Ann N Y Acad Sci 423: 442–468.J. Aschoff1988Circadian timing.Ann N Y Acad Sci423442468
  31. 31. Horne JA (1993) Human sleep, sleep loss and behavior: implication for the prefrontal cortex and psychiatric disorder. B J Psychiatry 162: 413–419.JA Horne1993Human sleep, sleep loss and behavior: implication for the prefrontal cortex and psychiatric disorder.B J Psychiatry162413419
  32. 32. Drummond SPA, Brown GG (2001) The effects of total sleep deprivation on cerebral responses to cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacology 25(5): supplS68–73.SPA DrummondGG Brown2001The effects of total sleep deprivation on cerebral responses to cognitive performance.Neuropsychopharmacology255supplS6873
  33. 33. Durmer JS, Dinges DF (2005) Neurocognitive consequence of sleep deprivation. Semin Neurol 25: 117–129.JS DurmerDF Dinges2005Neurocognitive consequence of sleep deprivation.Semin Neurol25117129
  34. 34. Drummond SPA, Brown GG, Stricker JL, Buxton RB, Wong EC, et al. (2000b) Sleep deprivation-induced reduction in cortical functional response to serial subtraction. Neuroreport 10: 3745–3748.SPA DrummondGG BrownJL StrickerRB BuxtonEC Wong2000bSleep deprivation-induced reduction in cortical functional response to serial subtraction.Neuroreport1037453748
  35. 35. Mu Q, Nahas Z, Johnson KA, Yamanaka K, Mishory A, et al. (2005) Decreased cortical activation response to verbal working memory following sleep deprivation. Sleep 28: 55–67.Q. MuZ. NahasKA JohnsonK. YamanakaA. Mishory2005Decreased cortical activation response to verbal working memory following sleep deprivation.Sleep285567
  36. 36. Drummond SPA, Brown GG, Gillin JC, Stricker JL, Wong EC, et al. (2000a) Altered brain response to verbal learning following sleep deprivation. Nature 403: 655–657.SPA DrummondGG BrownJC GillinJL StrickerEC Wong2000aAltered brain response to verbal learning following sleep deprivation.Nature403655657
  37. 37. Drummond SPA, Gillin JC, Brown GG (2001) Increased cerebral response during a divided attention task following sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res 10: 85–92.SPA DrummondJC GillinGG Brown2001Increased cerebral response during a divided attention task following sleep deprivation.J Sleep Res108592
  38. 38. Chee MW, Choo WC (2004) Functional imaging of working memory after 24 hr of total sleep deprivation. J Neurosci 24: 4560–4567.MW CheeWC Choo2004Functional imaging of working memory after 24 hr of total sleep deprivation.J Neurosci2445604567
  39. 39. Hoshi Y, Tamura M (1993) Dynamic multichannel near-infrared optical imaging of human brain activity. J Appl Physiol 75: 1842–1846.Y. HoshiM. Tamura1993Dynamic multichannel near-infrared optical imaging of human brain activity.J Appl Physiol7518421846
  40. 40. Villringer A, Planck J, Hock C, Schleinkofer L, Dirnagl U (1993) Near infrared spectroscopy (NIRS): a new tool to study hemodynamic changes during activation of brain function in human adults. Neurosci Let 154: 101–104.A. VillringerJ. PlanckC. HockL. SchleinkoferU. Dirnagl1993Near infrared spectroscopy (NIRS): a new tool to study hemodynamic changes during activation of brain function in human adults.Neurosci Let154101104
  41. 41. Tononi G, Cirelli C (2006) Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep Med Rev 10: 49–62.G. TononiC. Cirelli2006Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis.Sleep Med Rev104962
  42. 42. Walker MP, Stickgold R (2004) Sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation. Neuron 44: 121–133.MP WalkerR. Stickgold2004Sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation.Neuron44121133
  43. 43. Kuriyama K, Stickgold R, Walker SP (2004) Sleep-dependent learning and motor skill complexity. Lean Mem 11: 705–713.K. KuriyamaR. StickgoldSP Walker2004Sleep-dependent learning and motor skill complexity.Lean Mem11705713
  44. 44. Kuriyama K, Mishima K, Suzuki H, Aritake S, Uchiyama M (2008) Sleep accelerates the improvement in working memory performance. J Neurosci 28: 10145–10150.K. KuriyamaK. MishimaH. SuzukiS. AritakeM. Uchiyama2008Sleep accelerates the improvement in working memory performance.J Neurosci281014510150
  45. 45. Aschoff J (1985) On the perception of time during prolonged temporal isolation. Human Neurobiol 4: 41–52.J. Aschoff1985On the perception of time during prolonged temporal isolation.Human Neurobiol44152
  46. 46. Coull JT, Nobre AC (2008) Dissociating explicit timing from temporal expectation with fMRI. Curr Opin Neurobiol 18: 137–144.JT CoullAC Nobre2008Dissociating explicit timing from temporal expectation with fMRI.Curr Opin Neurobiol18137144
  47. 47. Drummond SPA, Meloy MJ, Yanagi MA, Orff HJ, Brown GG (2005) Compensatory recruitment after sleep deprivation and the relationship with performance. Psychiatry Res 140: 211–223.SPA DrummondMJ MeloyMA YanagiHJ OrffGG Brown2005Compensatory recruitment after sleep deprivation and the relationship with performance.Psychiatry Res140211223
  48. 48. Rammsayer TH, Vogel WH (1992) Pharmacologic properties of the internal clock underlying time perception in humans. Neuropsychobiology 26 (1–2): 71–80.TH RammsayerWH Vogel1992Pharmacologic properties of the internal clock underlying time perception in humans.Neuropsychobiology26 (1–2)7180
  49. 49. Meck WH, Penny TB, Pouthas V (2008) Cortico-striatal representation of time in animals and humans. Curr Opin Neurobiol 18: 145–152.WH MeckTB PennyV. Pouthas2008Cortico-striatal representation of time in animals and humans.Curr Opin Neurobiol18145152
  50. 50. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, et al. (2008) Sleep deprivation decreases binding of [11C]Raclopride to dopamine D2/D3 receptors in the human brain. J Neurosci 28: 8454–8461.ND VolkowGJ WangF. TelangJS FowlerJ. Logan2008Sleep deprivation decreases binding of [11C]Raclopride to dopamine D2/D3 receptors in the human brain.J Neurosci2884548461
  51. 51. Angrilli A, Cherubini P, Pavese A, Mangfredini S (1997) The influence of affective factors on time perception. Percept Psychophys 59: 972–982.A. AngrilliP. CherubiniA. PaveseS. Mangfredini1997The influence of affective factors on time perception.Percept Psychophys59972982
  52. 52. Hoagland H (1933) The physiological control of judgments of duration: evidence for a chemical clock. J Gen Psychol 9: 267–287.H. Hoagland1933The physiological control of judgments of duration: evidence for a chemical clock.J Gen Psychol9267287
  53. 53. Kleber RJ, Lhamon WT, Goldstone S (1963) Hyperthermia, hyperthyroidism, and time judgment. J Comp Physiol Psychol 56: 362–365.RJ KleberWT LhamonS. Goldstone1963Hyperthermia, hyperthyroidism, and time judgment.J Comp Physiol Psychol56362365
  54. 54. Tysk L (1984) Time perception and affective disorders. Percept Mot Skills 58: 455–464.L. Tysk1984Time perception and affective disorders.Percept Mot Skills58455464
  55. 55. Bschor T, Ising M, Bauer M, Lewitzka U, Skerstupeit M, et al. (2004) Time experience and time judgment in major depression, mania and healthy subjects: a controlled study of 93 subjects. Acta Psychatr Scand 109: 222–229.T. BschorM. IsingM. BauerU. LewitzkaM. Skerstupeit2004Time experience and time judgment in major depression, mania and healthy subjects: a controlled study of 93 subjects.Acta Psychatr Scand109222229
  56. 56. Kraft M, Martin RJ (1995) Chronobiology and chronotherapy in medicine. Dis Mon 41: 501–75.M. KraftRJ Martin1995Chronobiology and chronotherapy in medicine.Dis Mon4150175
  57. 57. Jindal SK, Aggarwal AN, Gupta D (2002) Diurnal variability of peak expiratory flow. J Asthma 39: 363–73.SK JindalAN AggarwalD. Gupta2002Diurnal variability of peak expiratory flow.J Asthma3936373
  58. 58. Davidson AJ, Castañón-Cervantes O, Stephan FK (2004) Daily oscillations in liver function: diurnal vs circadian rhythmicity. Liver Int 24: 179–86.AJ DavidsonO. Castañón-CervantesFK Stephan2004Daily oscillations in liver function: diurnal vs circadian rhythmicity.Liver Int2417986
  59. 59. Boyar RM, Witkin M, Carruth A, Ramsey J (1979) Circadian cortisol secretory rhythms in Cushing's disease. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 48: 760–5.RM BoyarM. WitkinA. CarruthJ. Ramsey1979Circadian cortisol secretory rhythms in Cushing's disease.J Clin Endocrinol Metab487605
  60. 60. Fevre M, Segel T, Marks JF, Boyar RM (1978) LH and melatonin secretion patterns in pubertal boys. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 47: 1383–6.M. FevreT. SegelJF MarksRM Boyar1978LH and melatonin secretion patterns in pubertal boys.J Clin Endocrinol Metab4713836
  61. 61. Duncan J (2001) An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 2: 820–829.J. Duncan2001An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex.Nat Rev Neurosci2820829
  62. 62. Miller EK, Cohen JD (2001) An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci 24: 167–202.EK MillerJD Cohen2001An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function.Annu Rev Neurosci24167202
  63. 63. Jasper HH (1958) The ten-twenty electrode system of international federation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 86: 7–14.HH Jasper1958The ten-twenty electrode system of international federation.Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol86714
  64. 64. Okamoto M, Dan H, Sakamoto K, Takeo K, Shimizu K, et al. (2004) Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10–20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping. Neuroimage 21: 99–111.M. OkamotoH. DanK. SakamotoK. TakeoK. Shimizu2004Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10–20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping.Neuroimage2199111
  65. 65. Hoshi Y, Kobayashi N, Tamura M (2001) Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: a study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model. J Appl Physiol 90: 1657–1662.Y. HoshiN. KobayashiM. Tamura2001Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: a study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model.J Appl Physiol9016571662
  66. 66. Matsuda G, Hiraki K (2006) Sustained decrease in oxygenated hemoglobin during video games in the dorsal prefrontal cortex: a NIRS study of children. Neuroimage 29: 706–11.G. MatsudaK. Hiraki2006Sustained decrease in oxygenated hemoglobin during video games in the dorsal prefrontal cortex: a NIRS study of children.Neuroimage2970611
  67. 67. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Statist Soc B 57: 289–300.Y. BenjaminiY. Hochberg1995Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing.J R Statist Soc B57289300