Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

A Trivers-Willard Effect in Contemporary Humans: Male-Biased Sex Ratios among Billionaires

A Trivers-Willard Effect in Contemporary Humans: Male-Biased Sex Ratios among Billionaires

  • Elissa Z. Cameron, 
  • Fredrik Dalerum
PLOS
x

Abstract

Background

Natural selection should favour the ability of mothers to adjust the sex ratio of offspring in relation to the offspring's potential reproductive success. In polygynous species, mothers in good condition would be advantaged by giving birth to more sons. While studies on mammals in general provide support for the hypothesis, studies on humans provide particularly inconsistent results, possibly because the assumptions of the model do not apply.

Methodology/Principal Findings

Here, we take a subset of humans in very good condition: the Forbe's billionaire list. First, we test if the assumptions of the model apply, and show that mothers leave more grandchildren through their sons than through their daughters. We then show that billionaires have 60% sons, which is significantly different from the general population, consistent with our hypothesis. However, women who themselves are billionaires have fewer sons than women having children with billionaires, suggesting that maternal testosterone does not explain the observed variation. Furthermore, paternal masculinity as indexed by achievement, could not explain the variation, since there was no variation in sex ratio between self-made or inherited billionaires.

Conclusions/Significance

Humans in the highest economic bracket leave more grandchildren through sons than through daughters. Therefore, adaptive variation in sex ratios is expected, and human mothers in the highest economic bracket do give birth to more sons, suggesting similar sex ratio manipulation as seen in other mammals.

Introduction

Natural selection should favour adaptive variation in offspring sex ratio if alterations maximise the offspring's potential reproductive success [Trivers-Willard hypothesis, TWH; 1]. For species where one sex has more variable reproductive success (males in polygynous species), the TWH predicts that 1) a mother with more resources to invest would be advantaged by producing a son, as a successful son would out-compete a successful daughter (constrained to a less variable reproductive rate), and 2) a mother with less resources to invest would be advantaged by producing a daughter, as her daughter would out-reproduce an unsuccessful son. Alternatively, if sons are more costly than daughters, only mothers in good condition could bear this cost [2].

For the predictions of the TWH to apply, the assumptions of the model must be met [3], [4]. Specifically, maternal condition should influence offspring condition, the offspring's condition should endure into adulthood, and any condition advantages should have a greater effect on the more reproductively variable sex [1], males in humans. The first two assumptions hold in humans. There is a strong association between birthweights of mothers and offspring [5], which seems to be largely environmentally determined [6]. Furthermore, birthweight is associated with survival and future reproductive success [7], [8], suggesting that condition advantages endure into adulthood. However, a greater condition advantage for sons than daughters has only been shown in one study on humans [9], and never in an industrialised population. To determine whether any sex ratio relationship is driven by success of resulting offspring in line with TWH [1], or simply occurs because sons cost more to raise [2], it is vital to determine if sons of mothers in good condition have higher reproductive success. However, in contemporary societies status can be negatively related to fertility [10]. Furthermore, most contemporary western societies have a monogamous social system, reducing differences in reproductive variance between sexes, although the change to monogamy appears to have been relatively recent [11].

Emerging evidence suggests that such an effect exists in mammals in relation to condition at conception [1], [12]. In humans, there has been extensive interest in sex ratio variation, but results have been inconsistent. While some studies in traditional societies support the TWH [e.g. 13], this is not so in industrialised humans [3], with most studies showing no difference [e.g. 14], [15], or small differences [e.g. 16]. Similar to studies in other mammals [12], studies on contemporary humans have used a variety of indices of condition with variable timing in the reproductive cycle, such as economic status [17], [18], female body shape [19] and size [20], [21], dominance, achievement or employment status [22], [23], and health [19]. In mammals, evidence suggests that sex ratios vary most consistently with condition around conception [12], [24], and physiological mechanisms may alter sex ratios during very early embryo development [12], [25], [26]. Recent evidence for sex ratio variation with diet at conception in humans [27] suggests similarity with other mammals. Furthermore, while the human birth sex ratio is male-biased [28] human in vitro raised embryos are more male-biased [e.g. 29], as in other mammal species [e.g.30] which has been attributed to glucose in the culture medium in cattle [25], [31]. Excess glucose kills female blastocysts but enhances male development [25]. However, alternate hypotheses in humans suggest that hormone levels [28], [32] or masculinity [22] are important regulators of human sex ratios. For example, levels of maternal testosterone are hypothesised to cause high-achieving women to give birth to more sons [23], [28]. Alternatively, relative ‘maleness’ in men has been proposed as an explanation for observed sex ratio biases by profession [22].

We examined whether the assumptions of the TWH, apply to the super-rich, using published information on family size among billionaires. This enabled us to distinguish between biases resulting from the costs [2] or the advantages [1] of raising sons. We then compared the offspring sex ratio of billionaires with the global population, and use information on these billionaires to test whether maternal condition influences sex ratios. Lastly, we test whether work achievement, as an index of testosterone levels [following 23], or paternal masculinity [22] explain variation in sex ratios.

Materials and Methods

Information on US$ billionaires was extracted from the Forbes list (www.Forbes.com, 2008 list), including wealth, origin of wealth (self-made, inherited, and growing inheritance) residence and citizenship, and number of children (listed for 910 of 1046; 866 with at least one child; 71 female, 795 male). We then searched the billionaire's name online using Google, and used the resulting pages to determine the sex of children. Resulting pages included Wikipedia, bibliographic sites, company websites, and newspapers (particularly marriage, birth and death announcements). For sex ratio analysis we used those billionaires for whom we could ascertain the sex of every child (350 male billionaires, 49 female billionaires). We compared the sex ratio of billionaires with the population sex ratio using a chi-squared test. We then divided region of citizenship and residency into western Europe, eastern Europe, North America, South America, Asia, Africa, and Australasia, to control for cultural differences. Most billionaires were from monogamous societies, with fewer than 20 from Arab countries. Some Asian countries limit the number of children born, which could also influence results. We fitted a full model including interaction effects between gender and region of citizenship and gender and region of residency and the main effects, and used Akaike's Information Criterion [33] to select the most parsimonious model,. We square root transformed number of children to more closely approximate normality [34] and excluded two male billionaires that had grossly outlying number of offspring (37 and 61), since the rest of the population each had less than 20 children. We used a generalized linear logistic model with a logit link function [35] to test if offspring sex ratio differed with the source of wealth using the proportion of sons as the response variable, and gender and wealth-source as factorial predictors. Previous researchers have used work achievement as a proxy for testosterone levels [23]. We therefore reasoned that those billionaires who were either self-made or were growing their inheritance were high achieving (‘high work drive’), whereas those that had inherited their wealth were not as high achieving in employment (‘low work drive’). We fitted a model including all interaction effects and used AICs to select the minimal adequate model.

Results

To test the assumptions of the TWH, we used only those billionaires that had children, and found that male billionaires had significantly more children and a more variable number of children than female billionaires (men: 1 to 61 children, women: 1 to 7 children; F1, 869 = 4.36, P = 0.03; Fig. 1a). We identified 14 families where an original fortune had been made 2 generations ago, and where the fortune-maker (male in all cases) had at least one son and at least one daughter. This enabled us to trace the resulting grandchildren within a single family. The original fortune-maker left more grandchildren through his sons than through his daughters (mean of 1.33 more grandchildren, paired t-test: t13 = 3.09, P = 0.009; Fig. 1b). Furthermore, sons were, on average, richer than daughters within the same family (mean of 178 ranks higher on billionaire list; paired t-test; t13 = 2.45, P = 0.029), suggesting that sons received greater parental allocation. Therefore the assumptions of the model are met, and the TWH predicts that billionaires should give birth to more sons than daughters, and more sons than the non-billionaire population.

thumbnail
Figure 1. The sex bias in number of children born to billionaires by parental gender.

(A) Number of children to female (71) and male (795) billionaires, and (B) the number of children to daughters and sons of an original billionaire (14), showing means±1 SE.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004195.g001

Billionaires for whom we could locate information on all of their children had more sons than daughters, and significantly more sons than the general population (population: 51% sons, [28], billionaires 60% sons; χ2 = 37.57, DF = 1, P<0.0001; Fig. 2). Therefore, mothers in the highest economic bracket had more sons than expected from the population average.

thumbnail
Figure 2. A comparison of sex ratios of children born to billionaires compared to the general population.

Offspring sex ratio for the global population (Grant 1998), all billionaires for whom the sex of all children could be traced (399), as well as female billionaires (49) and females married to billionaires (350), showing means±1 SE.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004195.g002

When we separated the billionaires by the sex of the listed billionaire we found that women who had children with billionaires had a more male biased offspring sex ratio than female who were themselves billionaires (χ2 = 4.32, DF = 1, P = 0.04 Fig. 2). While the sex ratio of children to women who were themselves billionaires did not differ from the global population (53% male, χ2 = 0.12, DF = 1, P = 0.73), women who had children with billionaires gave birth to significantly more sons (65% sons, χ2 = 80.13, DF = 1, P<0.0001).

The source and current status of wealth (inherited only vs self-made and growing inheritance) did not influence sex ratios of children fathered by billionaires (inherited only (60% sons) vs self-made (61% sons) and growing inheritance (65% sons) Fig. 3). However, among the women who were themselves billionaires, those that were self-made (52% sons) or were growing their inheritance (39% sons) had significantly fewer sons than those that inherited their wealth (56% sons, χ2 = 4.32, DF = 1, P = 0.04, Fig 3).

thumbnail
Figure 3. Offspring sex ratio for female billionaires and females married to billionaires in relation to work-drive or achievement where self-made and growing inheritance are considered high work drive as a proxy for high testosterone, showing means±1 SE.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004195.g003

Discussion

Among all billionaires we show that male billionaires produce more children than female billionaires. Furthermore, when we investigated the grandchildren of an original fortune-maker, we found that he had more grandchildren through his sons than through his daughters, supporting the assumption that reproductive success would be enhanced by giving birth to more sons than daughters. Finally, parental allocation by the fortune-maker seemed to be higher into their sons than their daughters since sons ranked higher up the billionaire list than daughters. This is the first study to show that males have greater reproductive success in a sample from a contemporary, industrialized, monogamous society, and it supports previous research showing that high-income men have more children than either high-income women or low income men [36]. It also means that the assumptions of the TWH apply in this subset of society. Therefore, any differences in the production of sons and daughters are unlikely to be explained solely by differences in the cost of producing sons and daughters [2], [37], even though human sons seem to be more costly to produce [38]. Rather, our results suggest that there is a benefit to producing sons for billionaires [1]. In line with our predictions, billionaires gave birth to significantly more sons (60%) than the rest of the population (51%), and this was particularly marked for male billionaires (65% sons).

Hypotheses to explain biased sex ratios in humans include those relating levels of testosterone to offspring sex ratios [39], [32]. For example, Grant argues that maternal testosterone determines offspring sex ratios [28], and that testosterone levels are indicated by work achievement [23]. Other studies have confirmed a link between testosterone and work achievement [39], and people with entrepreneurial tendencies have higher levels of testosterone [40]. Contrary to expectations based on previous findings [23], women who were themselves billionaires, indicating high motivation, had fewer sons, and this was most marked among self-made women and those that were working and expanding their inheritance. Therefore, work achievement, was not a strong predictor of offspring sex ratios, suggesting that either maternal testosterone was not associated with sex ratio, or, more likely, that work achievement is a poor index of testosterone. Recent studies on other mammals have suggested that maternal testosterone levels are related to offspring sex ratios [41], [42]. However, theory also suggests that particularly high achieving women may have more influence over the future success of their daughters than their sons, or daughters may inherit aspects of their mother's rank [43], as seen in other primate species [44].

Previous studies have suggested a similar hormonal or masculinity effect in males, whereby high achieving males give birth to more sons, either because of hormone levels [32] or brain masculinity [22], both of which may be indexed by work achievement. However, there was no difference in sex ratio between inherited or high work-drive male billionaires, suggesting that male work achievement does not explain the observed sex ratio bias.

Finally, if sex ratio variation arose due to variation in maternal condition, we would predict that all mothers in the sample were in good condition, and no difference in sex ratio between the groups would be predicted. This was confirmed for all categories except the high-achieving billionairesses, although this may have been due to the small sample size in this category.

Therefore, contemporary humans in the highest economic bracket show a significant sex ratio bias in favour of sons as predicted by the TWH, and these sons leave more grandchildren for the parents than daughters. It seems likely that similar physiological mechanisms operate in human females as in other mammalian species [12], [26], since sex ratio variation seems to arise around conception in relation to maternal condition [27].

Acknowledgments

We thank the University of Pretoria Research Development Programme for funding. The manuscript was improved by comments from J.M. Gaillard and an anonymous reviewer.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: EC. Performed the experiments: EC. Analyzed the data: EC FD. Wrote the paper: EC FD.

References

  1. 1. Trivers RL, Willard D (1973) Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring. Science 179: 90–92.RL TriversD. Willard1973Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring.Science1799092
  2. 2. Myers JH (1978) Sex-Ratio adjustment under food stress - maximization of quality or numbers of offspring. Am Nat 112: 381–388.JH Myers1978Sex-Ratio adjustment under food stress - maximization of quality or numbers of offspring.Am Nat112381388
  3. 3. Lazurus J (2002) Human sex ratios: adaptations and mechanisms, problems and prospects. In: Hardy I, editor. Sex ratios: concepts and research methods. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 287–311.J. Lazurus2002Human sex ratios: adaptations and mechanisms, problems and prospects.I. HardySex ratios: concepts and research methodsCambridgeCambridge University Press287311
  4. 4. Hewison AJM, Gaillard JM (1999) Successful sons or advantaged daughters? The Trivers-Willard model and sex biased maternal investment in ungulates. Tr Ecol Evol 14: 229–234.AJM HewisonJM Gaillard1999Successful sons or advantaged daughters? The Trivers-Willard model and sex biased maternal investment in ungulates.Tr Ecol Evol14229234
  5. 5. Stein AD, Lumey LH (2000) The relationship between maternal and offspring birth weights after maternal prenatal famine exposure: The Dutch Famine Birth Cohort Study. Human Biol 72: 641–654.AD SteinLH Lumey2000The relationship between maternal and offspring birth weights after maternal prenatal famine exposure: The Dutch Famine Birth Cohort Study.Human Biol72641654
  6. 6. Brooks AA, Johnson MR, Steer PJ, Pawson ME, Abdalla HI (1995) Birth weight: nature or nurture? Early Hum Dev 42: 29–35.AA BrooksMR JohnsonPJ SteerME PawsonHI Abdalla1995Birth weight: nature or nurture?Early Hum Dev422935
  7. 7. Barker DJP (1998) Mother, Babies and Health in Later Life. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone. DJP Barker1998Mother, Babies and Health in Later LifeEdinburghChurchill Livingstone
  8. 8. Phillips DIW, Handelsman DJ, Eriksson JG, Forsén T, Osmond C, et al. (2001) Prenatal growth and subsequent marital status: longitudinal study. Br Med J 322: 771.DIW PhillipsDJ HandelsmanJG ErikssonT. ForsénC. Osmond2001Prenatal growth and subsequent marital status: longitudinal study.Br Med J322771
  9. 9. Borgerhoff Mulder M (1998) Brothers and sisters-how sibling interactions affect optimal parental allocations. Human Nature 9: 119–161.M. Borgerhoff Mulder1998Brothers and sisters-how sibling interactions affect optimal parental allocations.Human Nature9119161
  10. 10. Potts M (1997) Sex and the birth rate: Human biology, demographic change, and access to fertility-regulation methods. Pop Dev Rev 23: 1–39.M. Potts1997Sex and the birth rate: Human biology, demographic change, and access to fertility-regulation methods.Pop Dev Rev23139
  11. 11. Dupanloup I, Pereira L, Bertorell G, Calafell F, Prata MJ, et al. (2003) A recent shift from polygyny to monogamy in humans is suggested by the analysis of worldwide Y-chromosome diversity. J Mol Evol 57: 85–97.I. DupanloupL. PereiraG. BertorellF. CalafellMJ Prata2003A recent shift from polygyny to monogamy in humans is suggested by the analysis of worldwide Y-chromosome diversity.J Mol Evol578597
  12. 12. Cameron EZ (2004) Facultative adjustment of mammalian sex ratios in support of the Trivers-Willard hypothesis: evidence for a mechanism. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 271: 1723–1728.EZ Cameron2004Facultative adjustment of mammalian sex ratios in support of the Trivers-Willard hypothesis: evidence for a mechanism.Proc Roy Soc Lond B27117231728
  13. 13. Gibson MA, Mace R (2003) Strong mothers bear more sons in rural Ethiopia. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 270: S108–S109.MA GibsonR. Mace2003Strong mothers bear more sons in rural Ethiopia.Proc Roy Soc Lond B270S108S109
  14. 14. Freese J, Powell B (1999) Sociobiology, status, and parental investment in sons and daughters: testing the Trivers-Willard hypothesis. Am J Sociol 106: 1704–1743.J. FreeseB. Powell1999Sociobiology, status, and parental investment in sons and daughters: testing the Trivers-Willard hypothesis.Am J Sociol10617041743
  15. 15. Keller MC, Nesse RM, Hofferth S (2001) The Trivers-Willard hypothesis of parental investment: no effect in the contemporary United States. Evol Hum Behav 22: 343–360.MC KellerRM NesseS. Hofferth2001The Trivers-Willard hypothesis of parental investment: no effect in the contemporary United States.Evol Hum Behav22343360
  16. 16. Almond D, Edlund L (2007) Trivers-Willard at birth and one year: evidence from US natality data 1983–2001. Proc Roy Soc B 274: 2491–2496.D. AlmondL. Edlund2007Trivers-Willard at birth and one year: evidence from US natality data 1983–2001.Proc Roy Soc B27424912496
  17. 17. Kaňková Š, Šulc J, Nouzová K, Fajfrlík K (2007) Women infected with parasite Toxoplasma have more sons. Naturwissenschaften 94: 122–127.Š. KaňkováJ. ŠulcK. NouzováK. Fajfrlík2007Women infected with parasite Toxoplasma have more sons.Naturwissenschaften94122127
  18. 18. Catalano RA (2003) Sex ratios in the two Germanies: A test of the economic stress hypothesis. Hum Reprod 18: 1972–1975.RA Catalano2003Sex ratios in the two Germanies: A test of the economic stress hypothesis.Hum Reprod1819721975
  19. 19. Tovee MJ, Brown JE, Jacobs D (2001) Maternal waist-to-hip does not predict child gender. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 268: 1007–1010.MJ ToveeJE BrownD. Jacobs2001Maternal waist-to-hip does not predict child gender.Proc Roy Soc Lond B26810071010
  20. 20. Kanazawa S (2005) Big and tall parents have more sons: further generalizations of the Trivers–Willard hypothesis. J Theor Biol 235: 583–590.S. Kanazawa2005Big and tall parents have more sons: further generalizations of the Trivers–Willard hypothesis.J Theor Biol235583590
  21. 21. Helle S (2008) Height, weight, body mass index and offspring sex at birth in contemporary Finnish women. J Theor Biol 252: 773–775.S. Helle2008Height, weight, body mass index and offspring sex at birth in contemporary Finnish women.J Theor Biol252773775
  22. 22. Kanazawa S, Vandermassen G (2005) Engineers have more sons, nurses have more daughters: an evolutionary psychological extension of Baron-Cohen's extreme brain theory of autism and its empirical implications. J Theor Biol 233: 589–599.S. KanazawaG. Vandermassen2005Engineers have more sons, nurses have more daughters: an evolutionary psychological extension of Baron-Cohen's extreme brain theory of autism and its empirical implications.J Theor Biol233589599
  23. 23. Grant VJ, Yang S (2003) Achieving women and declining sex ratios. Hum Biol 75: 917–927.VJ GrantS. Yang2003Achieving women and declining sex ratios.Hum Biol75917927
  24. 24. Sheldon BC, West SA (2004) Maternal dominance, maternal condition, and offspring sex ratio in ungulate mammals. Am Nat 163: 40–54.BC SheldonSA West2004Maternal dominance, maternal condition, and offspring sex ratio in ungulate mammals.Am Nat1634054
  25. 25. Larson MA, Kimura K, Kubisch HM, Roberts RM (2001) Sexual dimorphism among bovine embryos in their ability to make the transition to expanded blastocyst and in the expression of the signaling molecule IFN-tau. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 98: 9677–9682.MA LarsonK. KimuraHM KubischRM Roberts2001Sexual dimorphism among bovine embryos in their ability to make the transition to expanded blastocyst and in the expression of the signaling molecule IFN-tau.Proc Nat Acad Sci USA9896779682
  26. 26. Cameron EZ, Lemons P, Bateman PW, Bennett NC (2008) Experimental alteration of litter sex ratios in a mammal. Proc Roy Soc B 275: 323–327.EZ CameronP. LemonsPW BatemanNC Bennett2008Experimental alteration of litter sex ratios in a mammal.Proc Roy Soc B275323327
  27. 27. Mathews F, Johnson PJ, Neil A (2008) You are what your mother eats: evidence for maternal preconception diet influencing foetal sex in humans. Proc Roy Soc B 275: 1661–1668.F. MathewsPJ JohnsonA. Neil2008You are what your mother eats: evidence for maternal preconception diet influencing foetal sex in humans.Proc Roy Soc B27516611668
  28. 28. Grant VJ (1998) Maternal personality, evolution and the sex ratio. London: Routledge. VJ Grant1998Maternal personality, evolution and the sex ratioLondonRoutledge
  29. 29. Luna M, Duke M, Copperman A, Grunfeld L, Sandler B, et al. (2007) Blastocyst embryo transfer is associated with a sex-ratio imbalance in favour of male offspring. Fert Steril 87: 519–523.M. LunaM. DukeA. CoppermanL. GrunfeldB. Sandler2007Blastocyst embryo transfer is associated with a sex-ratio imbalance in favour of male offspring.Fert Steril87519523
  30. 30. Gutiérrez-Adán A, Behboodi E, Anderson GB, Medrano JF, Murrary JD (1996) Relationship between stage of development and sex of bovine IVM-IVF embryos cultured in vitro versus in the sheep oviduct. Theriogenol 16: 515–525.A. Gutiérrez-AdánE. BehboodiGB AndersonJF MedranoJD Murrary1996Relationship between stage of development and sex of bovine IVM-IVF embryos cultured in vitro versus in the sheep oviduct.Theriogenol16515525
  31. 31. Gutiérrez-Adán A, Granados J, Pintado B, de la Fuente J (2001) Influence of glucose on the sex ratio of bovine IM/IVF embryos cultured in vitro. Reprod Fert Dev 13: 361–365.A. Gutiérrez-AdánJ. GranadosB. PintadoJ. de la Fuente2001Influence of glucose on the sex ratio of bovine IM/IVF embryos cultured in vitro.Reprod Fert Dev13361365
  32. 32. James WH (1996) Evidence that mammalian sex ratios at birth are partially controlled by parental hormone levels at the time of conception. J Theo Biol 180: 271–286.WH James1996Evidence that mammalian sex ratios at birth are partially controlled by parental hormone levels at the time of conception.J Theo Biol180271286
  33. 33. Akaike H (1974) New look at statistical-model identification. IEEE Trans Autom Contr 19: 716–723.H. Akaike1974New look at statistical-model identification.IEEE Trans Autom Contr19716723
  34. 34. Bartlett MS (1936) Some notes on insecticide tests in the laboratory and in the field. J Roy Stat Soc Suppl 3: 185–194.MS Bartlett1936Some notes on insecticide tests in the laboratory and in the field.J Roy Stat Soc Suppl3185194
  35. 35. Vittinghof E, Glidden DV, Shiboski SC, McCullough CE (2005) Regression methods in biostatistics linear, logistic, survival and repeated measures models. New York: Springer-Verlag. E. VittinghofDV GliddenSC ShiboskiCE McCullough2005Regression methods in biostatistics linear, logistic, survival and repeated measures modelsNew YorkSpringer-Verlag
  36. 36. Hopcraft RL (2006) Sex, status and reproductive success in the contemporary United States. Evol Hum Behav 27: 104–120.RL Hopcraft2006Sex, status and reproductive success in the contemporary United States.Evol Hum Behav27104120
  37. 37. Gomendio M, Clutton-Brock TH, Albon SD, Guinness FE, Simpson MJ (1990) Mammalian sex ratios and variation in costs of rearing sons and daughters. Nature 343: 261–263.M. GomendioTH Clutton-BrockSD AlbonFE GuinnessMJ Simpson1990Mammalian sex ratios and variation in costs of rearing sons and daughters.Nature343261263
  38. 38. Rickard IJ, Russell AF, Lummaa V (2007) Producing sons reduces lifetime reproductive success of subsequent offspring in pre-industrial Finns. Proc Roy Soc B 274: 2981–2988.IJ RickardAF RussellV. Lummaa2007Producing sons reduces lifetime reproductive success of subsequent offspring in pre-industrial Finns.Proc Roy Soc B27429812988
  39. 39. Al-ayadhi LY (2004) Sex hormones, personality characters and professional status among Saudi females. Saudi Med J 25: 711–716.LY Al-ayadhi2004Sex hormones, personality characters and professional status among Saudi females.Saudi Med J25711716
  40. 40. White RE, Thornhill S, Hampson E (2006) Entrepreneurs and evolutionary biology: the relationahips between testosterone and new venture creation. Organiz. RE WhiteS. ThornhillE. Hampson2006Entrepreneurs and evolutionary biology: the relationahips between testosterone and new venture creation.Organiz
  41. 41. Grant VJ, Irwin RJ, Standley NT, Shelling AN, Chamley LW (2008) Sex of bovine embryos may be related to mother's preovulatory follicular testosterone. Biol Reprod 78: 812–815.VJ GrantRJ IrwinNT StandleyAN ShellingLW Chamley2008Sex of bovine embryos may be related to mother's preovulatory follicular testosterone.Biol Reprod78812815
  42. 42. Helle S, Laaksonen T, Adamsson A, Paranki J, Huitu O (2008) Female field voles with high testosterone and glucose levels produce male-biased litters. Anim Behav 75: 1031–1039.S. HelleT. LaaksonenA. AdamssonJ. ParankiO. Huitu2008Female field voles with high testosterone and glucose levels produce male-biased litters.Anim Behav7510311039
  43. 43. Leimar O (1996) Life-history analysis of the Trivers and Willard sex-ratio problem. Behav Ecol 7: 316–325.O. Leimar1996Life-history analysis of the Trivers and Willard sex-ratio problem.Behav Ecol7316325
  44. 44. Nevison C (1997) Social rank and birth sex ratios in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Appl Anim Behav Sci 5: 285–291.C. Nevison1997Social rank and birth sex ratios in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta).Appl Anim Behav Sci5285291