Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Mares Prefer the Voices of Highly Fertile Stallions

  • Alban Lemasson ,

    alban.lemasson@univ-rennes1.fr

    Affiliations Université de Rennes 1, Laboratoire d’éthologie animale et humaine, UMR 6552- C.N.R.S., Paimpont, France, Institut Universitaire de France, Paris, France

  • Kévin Remeuf,

    Affiliation Université de Rennes 1, Laboratoire d’éthologie animale et humaine, UMR 6552- C.N.R.S., Paimpont, France

  • Marie Trabalon,

    Affiliation Université de Rennes 1, Laboratoire d’éthologie animale et humaine, UMR 6552- C.N.R.S., Paimpont, France

  • Frédérique Cuir,

    Affiliation Haras du Pin, Le Pin-au-Haras, France

  • Martine Hausberger

    Affiliation C.N.R.S., Laboratoire d’éthologie animale et humaine, UMR 6552- Université de Rennes 1, Rennes, France

Abstract

We investigated the possibility that stallion whinnies, known to encode caller size, also encoded information about caller arousal and fertility, and the reactions of mares in relation to type of voice. Voice acoustic features are correlated with arousal and reproduction success, the lower-pitched the stallion’s voice, the slower his heart beat and the higher his fertility. Females from three study groups preferred playbacks of low-pitched voices. Hence, females are attracted by frequencies encoding for large male size, calmness and high fertility. More work is needed to explore the relative importance of morpho-physiological features. Assortative mating may be involved as large females preferred voices of larger stallions. Our study contributes to basic and applied ongoing research on mammal reproduction, and questions the mechanisms used by females to detect males’ fertility.

Introduction

Vocal communication is primordially important in the daily social life of a large number of mammal species. Just by hearing a given voice, some mammals, including humans, are able to assess their degree of familiarity with the caller (e.g. Campbell’s monkeys [1], Asian small-clawed otters [2]), its size (e.g. red deer [3], koalas [4], giant pandas [5]), and its identity (e.g. olive baboons [6], Atlantic walrus [7]). Auditory decoding capacities concerning internal characteristics of callers have also been demonstrated. For example, baboons [8] and spotted hyenas [9] encode acoustically the dominance rank of the caller. Humans can reliably estimate the age of a speaker just by his/her voice characteristics [10].

One of the main functions of vocal signals is to coordinate interactions between conspecifics. Among these interactions, exchange of vocal signals between conspecifics of opposite sexes is frequent and serves to attract new mates (e.g. mouse lemurs [11]), to strengthen male-female bonding (e.g. siamangs [12]), and to coordinate copulation or to inform about copulation success (e.g. Barbary macaques [13]). The application of the source—filter theory has enabled researchers to decompose the acoustic structure of vocal signals according to their mode of production and to predict the acoustic variation that is caused by anatomical or physiological attributes of the caller [14]. Hence, acoustic encoding of physiological characteristics of a potential sexual partner plays a role in the outcome of a given interaction. However, studies addressing this question remain limited, notably concerning mammals compared to birds and insects. For example, males discriminate the different stages of a female’s oestrous cycle by her voice (Barbary macaques [15], giant pandas [16], humans [17]). Hormonal, notably testosterone, levels, are correlated with frequency parameters (e.g. fundamental frequency, dominant frequency, frequency modulation rate) of male voices (e.g. humans [10, 18]), a trait that can impact females’ preference (e.g. giant pandas [19]). Frequency parameters predict reproduction success of the few species investigated (humans [20, 21], red deer [22], fallow deer [23]).

Horses are interesting models in this perspective for several reasons. First, male-female interactions play a key role in their social system. Horse populations are structured by long-lasting social and breeding relationships in polygynous groups [24]. The basic social unit of free-ranging horses is the harem composed mostly of two to six individuals, including adults (1–2 males, 2–3 females) who have established stable bonds, and juveniles (under 3 years old) [25, 26]. Physiologically, sexual maturity is reached at around 18 and 20 months old respectively in males and females [25, 26]. Juveniles of both sexes socially disperse, and adult males stay all year long with the females. Females express mate choice at two levels. Females emigrate from their natal group when they reach sexual maturity, dispersal peaking during the sexual receptivity season [27]. Females can create their own harem with a bachelor male or immigrate into an already formed harem [27]. Late female dispersal occurs sometimes when the stallion of a harem dies or when he is displaced [24]. Alternatively, females can accompany the migrating male. During their lifetime, females are not all equally receptive, older and more experienced mares are more receptive [28]. Authors have hypothesized that females’ libido and reproductive efficiency are related to the time spent in contact with a male [29]. Przewalski stallions can sometimes be rejected by mares that are dominant over them and hence fail to mate [30]. In the wild thus, females do express clear choices by first approaching a preferred stallion and then integrating his family group: visual attention, approach and proximity seeking are therefore key elements in the process of mate choice. In captivity, when presented a choice of males, females prefer some males to others [31, 32], but the question of how the choice is made remains open. Second, horses’ vocal repertoire includes a loud and long-distance call, named whinny, used for distant communication (up to 1 km) when visual and olfactory signals are not reliable. Whinnies are notably exchanged between or within sexes when spatially or visually separated [25, 36]. Whinnies encode caller’s sex, size and potentially dominance status [33]. Playback experiments showed that horses use whinnies to decode their degree of familiarity with the caller [33], as well as its identity [34]. Third, these large mammals can be handled easily by human experimenters enabling the collection of a variety of physiological parameters. Fourth, understanding the mechanisms facilitating the control of the reproduction of this domestic species is of primary importance for stud farms and breeders [35].

This study asked whether stallion voices encode their physiological characteristics, and whether mares display preferences for certain types of voice. We investigated relationships between acoustic characteristics, notably frequency-related parameters that have been showed to be crucial [36], and physiological features (i.e. heart beat, hormonal profile, sperm quality and reproductive success) known to be involved in arousal (i.e. motivational and emotional states) and fertility (Study 1). We hypothesized that if, as predicted, pitch is of primal importance, females’ auditory preferences can be evaluated by playback experiments (Study 2—Experiment 1). As pitch is already known to encode stallion size [33], we evaluated the influence of females’ size on the strength of the above preference (Study 2—Experiment 2). This relationship is predicted by the hypothesis of phenotypic assortative mating [37], a nonrandom mating pattern in which individuals with similar phenotypes mate with one another more frequently. Assortative mating has been proposed as an adaptive process to favour local populations with local adaptations to environmental conditions. Wild populations of horses do show local adaptations in terms of morphology and size therefore this possibility seemed worth testing. Moreover, experience may play a role and domestic mares are generally mated with a male of assorted breed hence size. Here, we predicted that taller mares should be more attracted by tall stallions than smaller mares.

Study 1: Acoustic Encoding of Physiological Features by Stallions

Materials and methods

Ethics statements.

Studies 1 and 2 were performed in accordance with the routine procedures in the facilities where stallions are handled daily and regularly submitted to blood sampling. All procedures other than recordings and observations were performed by farm staff under the supervision of the local veterinarian. Moreover, experiments (in studies 1 and 2) complied with the current French (Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique) regulations governing the care and use of research animals and have been approved by the French National Institute for Horse and Horse riding (Institut français du cheval et de l’équitation). Experiments were performed in accordance to the 2010/63/EU European Communities Council Directive. The stud farm staff was responsible for all animal husbandry and care. As our experiments were based on non-invasive observations no ethical approval was required.

Subjects.

In February 2012, we recorded the calls and heart beats of 15 breeding stallions (S1–S15) of various breeds and ages, housed on three different French stud farms (Table 1). These stallions were housed individually in 3m x 3m stalls, with a natural photoperiod, and were moved to paddocks for one or two hours each day. Horses were fed hay and pellets twice a day. Water was provided ad libitum. These stallions were handled every year for sperm collection.

Acoustic recordings and measurements.

Stallions’ whinnies were recorded with a directional microphone (K6/ME66 Sennheiser) connected to a digital portable recorder (PMD661 Marantz, sampling rate 44100Hz, resolution 16 bits). Each stallion was led outdoors twice by one caretaker for 10 min recordings, on different days, in a random order. During a recording session, the male stood 50m from his stall and another caretaker led a familiar mare (the same for all males on a stud farm) keeping 10–20m away. The experimenter (K.R.) stood 5m in front of the male with the recording apparatus. All males were recorded in the same context (excitement-frustration) to avoid situation-specific acoustic variations. A total of 178 whinnies (12 ± 6 per subject, range: 10–18) were recorded and used for the subsequent analyses.

Spectrograms were extracted using ANA software [38] with Linux (FFT size 256) for subsequent acoustic measurements. A typical whinny is composed of three parts [33]: a first loud, relatively flat and tonal part (named here Part 1 or ‘Introduction’), a second relatively atonal part, more or less rhythmical, including long modulated units (Part 2, ‘Climax’) and a third very soft and rhythmical part with very short units (Part 3, ‘End’). For comparative reasons, we measured the same 12 acoustic parameters as Lemasson et al. [33] and averaged the data per stallion (Fig. 1): F01 (fundamental frequency of the Introduction), Fmax1, 2, 3, T (dominant frequency, i.e. frequency value with the maximal intensity, of Parts 1, 2, 3 and entire call, measured on the intensity spectrum), Fb2 (basic frequency of the Climax, i.e. first frequency reinforced, because the fundamental frequency is not always identifiable), D1, 2, 3, T, U2 (durations of Parts 1, 2, 3, entire call and Part 2 units), NbU2 (number of units in Part 2).

thumbnail
Fig 1. A spectrogram of a whinny showing acoustic measures taken.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118468.g001

Heart beat and hormonal measuring.

Before being led outdoors for acoustic recording, subjects were equipped with a heart rate monitor for horses (Polar Horse Trainer S810i). With this equipment, the experimenter could record heart beat within a 5 second window during which a whinny was emitted (HB5s) and during the 10 min of the recording session (HB10m). HB5s and HB10m were averaged per male for subsequent comparisons.

The same week, a caretaker (the same for all stallions at each location) familiar to the horses, collected two blood samples per handled subject, on two consecutive days, using 4ml blood collection tubes BD Vacutainer Lithium Heparin 68. Samples were collected from the jugular vein between 9 and 10 am, within 1–2 minutes, when stallions were calm and still in stalls. The blood sampling site was first swabbed with alcohol. Only one 4ml blood tube was collected per horse and per day to sample the minimum necessary for analyses. Plasma was immediately separated by centrifugation at 4°C (15 min at 3000 rpm) and the aliquot extracts were stored at-36°C until assayed (cortisol and testosterone, as we were interested in respectively emotion- and reproduction-related hormones). Hormone concentrations were measured without an extraction procedure, using a commercially available EIA kit and performed according to the manufacturer’s guidelines (Assay Designs Inc., USA). The concentrations of hormones in plasma samples were calculated from a standard curve and expressed in pg/ml. The two concentrations for each hormone were averaged per subject. The intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation were below 5.4% and 10.9% respectively for cortisol, 7.8% and 12.6% respectively for testosterone.

Reproductive success and semen features.

Data on previous breeding records for the different stallions were made available by the National studs. Hence, we could calculate two reproductive success scores for all stallions [35]: Insemination Success (IS = number of embryos alive after 16 days / number of artificial inseminations) and Survival Success (SS = number of pregnant mares at the end of the gestation / number of embryos alive after 16 days). This was based on 294 ± 176 artificial inseminations per stallion performed between 2007 and 2012.

Spermograms by stud farms between 2006 and 2012 for all stallions (except horse # S1 for which spectrograms were not available) enabled the scoring of sperm mobility at 0h (M0), 24h (M24) and 48h (M48) after ejaculation, as well as the percentage of spermatozoa presenting physical abnormalities in the mid-piece in particular (SpM), or in all parts (SpO). Spermograms were done using Palmer and Fauquenot’s protocol [39].

Statistical analyses.

Given our small sample sizes (N = 14 for sperm-related variables and N = 15 for all other cases), we used nonparametric Spearman tests to assess correlations between physiological and acoustical parameters. Statistics were run on Statistica 8.0 (Statsoft) with a significance threshold of 0.05.

Results

Physiological and reproductive profiles differed among stallions (Table 2). However, reproductive success was consistent between insemination (IS) and survival success (SS) data (Table 3). Similarly, different measures of sperm mobility (M0, M24, M48 altogether), of spermatozoon abnormality (SpM with SpO) and of heart rates (HB5s with HB10m) were positively correlated. Heart beat rates were negatively correlated with one measure of fertility (HB10m/SS). Sperm mobility and spermatozoon abnormalities (M0/SpM, M0/SpO, M48/SpM, M48/SpO) were negatively correlated. However, hormonal levels (testosterone and cortisol) were not correlated with any other parameter.

thumbnail
Table 2. Acoustic (a) and physiological (b) traits of stallions (mean ± SE) (N = 15 stallions).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118468.t002

Acoustic parameters were correlated with heart beat rates and fertility, but with no other physiological parameter (Table 4). The most important parameter appeared to be the pitch of the stallion’s voice. The lower-pitched the stallion’s voice, the slower its heart beat (positive correlations: FmaxT/HB5s, Fmax2/HB5s, F01/HB10m, Fb2/HB10m; Fig. 2a), and the higher its fertility (negative correlations: FmaxT/IS and SS, F01/IS, Fmax1/IS and SS, Fmax2/IS and SS; Fig. 2b). One temporal acoustic parameter was also correlated with physiology: the higher the number of repeated units in the climax (Fig. 3), the slower the heart beat (NBU2/HB5s).

thumbnail
Table 4. Relationships between acoustic and physiological features.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118468.t004

thumbnail
Fig 2. Correlation between voice frequencies in relation to (a) heart beat rates (P = 0.03) and (b) fertility scores (P = 0.002).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118468.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Heart beat rates in relation to number of repeated units in the climax of the whinny.

Example of three spectrograms (X axis = duration, Y axis = frequency): (1) NBU2 = 4—HB5s = 174 beats per minute (bpm), (2) NBU2 = 8—HB5s = 112 bpm, (3) NBU2 = 15—HB5s = 73 bpm.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118468.g003

Study 2: Auditory Preference of Mares

Materials and methods

Subjects.

Playback subjects were 40 adult (7 to 27 years old) mares. Group A (N = 14, 16.5 +/- 4.8 years old) and B (N = 15, 16 +/- 5.6 year old) females of similar height (165 +/- 5 cm), were subjects of Experiment 1 and were housed at ‘Haras National du Pin’ (National stud, France). Sizes of group C (N = 11, Age 11.5 +/- 1.9) females were very heterogeneous (155 +/- 11 cm tall). They were the Experiment 2 subjects and were housed at Ploermel riding school (France) (Table 5). The group A and B mares had known one another for more than 3 years and were housed outdoors in their respective enclosures (300m2), with an ad libitum access to shelters and water. The group C mares were housed individually in their own 12m2 stalls of the riding school. All mares were fed hay and pellets once a day. Water was available ad libitum. All females were unfamiliar to the stallions (housed in a different stud) used in these experiments. All group A and B females have been inseminated at least once. Fourteen of them had experienced giving birth. We have no information concerning the experience of females from the three groups of natural covering by stallions.

Experiment 1.

Prior to playback experiments, group A and B mares had been hormonally treated with Regumate (altrenogest) to try to synchronize their cycles and to increase chances to be in estrus at the time of experiments. The day before and the day after Experiment 1, each female was presented to a stallion, separated by a wooden board, to assess their sexual receptivity following the Asa’s protocol [40]. Females that were receptive before and after playbacks were considered (in this study) to be in oestrus state (N = 11), whereas females that were not receptive before and after playbacks were considered in anoestrus state (N = 6). All other females presented an unstable status (receptive either before or after the experiment) and were thus not included in this comparison.

Playback experiments aimed at evaluating mares’ preferred pitch of a male voice. Each study group heard the whinnies of one pair of stallions (Group A—stallions S10 and S5, Group B—stallions S3 and S6). Stallions S10 and S3 had low pitched voices (their mean FmaxT were respectively 676 and 948 Hz) and stallions S5 and S6 had high pitched voices (their mean FmaxT were respectively 2007 and 1388 Hz).

Four whinny exemplars per stallion were used in the experiment. These exemplars were selected so that their FmaxT values were close to the corresponding stallion’s mean. During a given playback trial, a single exemplar of two whinnies (one Low pitched and one High pitched, see Fig. 4 and S1S4 Sounds for examples) were broadcast simultaneously. Whinnies were paired so that their pitches differed but their durations matched (1948 +/- 109 ms). All whinnies were homogenized in amplitude so that they reached 70db in the centre of the experimental setup, a sound level matching natural conditions [33].

thumbnail
Fig 4. Acoustic stimuli spectrograms (a) low pitch and (b) high pitch.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118468.g004

The experimental setup was a visually enclosed rectangle (3m x 10m) with a border of piled straw bales (3m high) (Fig. 5). The rectangle was divided virtually into three parts: a 2m-long central zone where females were placed, and two 4m-long lateral corridors. We placed a loudspeaker (JLH-2002 Sanha) at the end of each corridor, one for the low pitched stimulus and the other one for the high pitched stimulus. Loudspeakers were monitored wireless with a M51V Asus computer. The experimenter (K.R.) stayed still in the central zone filming the subject with a HDR-XRI55E Sony camera.

Each subject was familiarized with the setup the week preceding the experiment (i.e. females were walked individually all along the setup and then were freed for exploration twice for five minutes each time). The experiment lasted 4 consecutive days for each group (A: 16–19 April 2012, B: 23–26 April 2012), totalizing 8 trials per subject (one every morning between 8 and 11 am and one every afternoon between 2 and 5 pm). During a trial, a caretaker led a subject into the central zone, waited one or two minutes for the mare to calm down, adjusted her body position and freed the mare. The experimenter played the sounds when the mare’s body was strictly perpendicular to both loudspeakers with the mare facing the exit door and then filmed the female’s response for 5 minutes. Between two consecutive trials, a caretaker removed any dung and sprayed Desogerme (ethanol) to neutralize as much as possible the odours in the setup. A different pair of whinnies was played each day. The high pitched stimulus was broadcast from one side (randomly chosen) in the morning and from the other side in the afternoon so that each subject heard the same whinny from the left and the right on the same day.

The video recordings allowed quantification (by K. R.) of several behavioural items using the focal sampling method. We calculated the occurrence frequencies of ‘turn head towards High/Low pitched side’ (orientation of the head by more than 45° towards the loudspeaker) and of ‘turn body towards High/Low pitched side’. We recorded the total duration spent in each corridor of the setup as well as the first head orientation towards each loudspeaker. Data were coded blind as videos were scored in a random order with the sound off.

Experiment 2.

The group C mares were not treated hormonally (and presented no clear sign of estrus) and each female could hear the same voices of the two stallions (S5 and S10) used in Experiment 1. These females were tested in a familiar 40m x 20m arena. Females were led to the arena and positioned in the centre following exactly the same procedure as in Experiment 1. Each mare was tested twice, at a 4-day interval. Tests were performed at 5–6 pm or 8–9 pm in a random order outside the riding centre’s activities. The same behaviours as in Experiment 1 were quantified for each 5-minute trial. Group C, including tall and short females, was suitable to test the assortative mating hypothesis. A standard criterion discriminating tall from small mares is withers height: above 151cm mares are called tall (French National Studbook).

Statistical analyses.

Nonparametric Wilcoxon tests compared behavioural data of females from respectively groups A, B and C in relation to pitch of stimulus (Low vs High). Mann-Whitney tests compared total numbers of head and body orientations (regardless of side), as well as total durations in both corridors were between females oestrus and anoestrus states. We estimated the strength of preference for type of voice by calculating the following ratio: number of head orientations towards the Low-pitched voice / number of head orientations towards the high-pitched voice. We compared the ratio of mares in oestrus vs anoestrus states (Experiment 1) and tall vs short mares (Experiment 2) with Mann-Whitney tests. Statistics were run on Statistica 8.0 (Statsoft) with a significance threshold of 0.05.

Results

Experiment 1.

Data for both groups (A and B) of mares showed that they spent significantly more time in the corridor where the low-pitched voice was played back than in the corridor with the high-pitched voice (Table 6). Females displayed significantly more first head orientations, total head orientations and total body orientations towards the loudspeaker with the low-pitched voice. All the above-mentioned variables, except total body orientations, remained significant for group B mares. However, only the number of first head orientations remained significant for group A mares. See S1 and S2 Videos for examples of immediate responses.

Whether mares were or not in estrus did not influence the strength of the preference for one type of voice (ratio of head orientations towards Low- / High-pitched voices, Mann-Whitney test, N1 = 11, N2 = 6, U = 32, P = 0.919). It also did not influence the number of head (U = 16, P = 0.088) and body (U = 23.5, P = 0.338) orientations. However, females in anoestrus state spent longer (1227 +/- 47 s) in the corridors broadcasting both low- and high-pitched voices than did estrus females (981 +/- 90 s) (U = 11, P = 0.027).

Experiment 2.

Group C mares displayed significantly more head and body orientations towards, and spent significantly longer in the corridor with low-pitched voice than the other corridor (Table 7). However, numbers of first head orientations did not differ between the two types of stimuli.

This group included seven ‘tall’ (“horse” size) and four ‘small’ (“pony” size) mares (see methods for definition). In line with the assortative matching prediction, and knowing that taller stallions possess lower pitched voices [33], the preference for low-pitched voices was significantly higher in tall than in small females (Fig. 6).

thumbnail
Fig 6. Group C mares’ preference for low-pitched voices in relation with their size: tall (N = 7), short (N = 4).

*Result of Mann-Whitney test: U = 3, P = 0.038.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118468.g006

Discussion

Our analyses of stallions’ voice acoustic features revealed that some parameters, essentially frequency-related measures, were correlated to fertility (survival success of embryos) as well as heart rate; the lower-pitched the stallion’s voice (fundamental and dominant frequency), the slower its heart beat and the higher its reproductive success (Study 1). No correlation was found between hormonal profile or sperm quality and any other physiological and acoustic parameters. Playback experiments revealed that females from our three studied groups are able to discriminate low- and high-pitched voices, and are more attracted by males displaying low-pitched voices (assessed by the time spent near the corresponding loudspeaker) (Study 2). Whether mares were or not in oestrus state did not influence the strength of their preference for one type of voice, suggesting that the above preference is not systematically related to mating or fertility (Experiment 1), as can be expected in social groups when males and females establish long-term stable bonds. Indeed, directing its attention and interest towards a particular male is the first indispensable step for an active choice of partner but not a direct proof of sexual motivation. Supporting the assortative mating prediction [37], the strength of the preference for low-pitched voices of tall females was higher than that of small females (Experiment 2).

Females’ preference for low pitched voices, as found here in mares, is a widespread characteristic. Attractiveness of human voices varies and listeners largely agree on which sounds are attractive (reviewed by McDermott [36]). Occidental woman, more so during the fertile phase [41], judge low-pitched male voices more attractive [42, 43, 44, 45]. Females of various mammal species are also more attracted by voices with lower pitched frequencies (red deer [3], koalas [4], giant pandas [5]). The attractiveness of males with low-pitched voices is also confirmed by studies showing related higher mating success (humans [46], bison [47]).

According to some authors, the main reason why low-pitched voices are found attractive is that frequencies are often negatively correlated with male size and/or weight and thus fighting/protecting abilities (red deer [22], koalas [4], giant pandas [48], otters [49], killer whales [50], bison [47], humans [10]). In line with this, our previous study showed that fundamental or dominant frequencies and stallion size were negatively correlated [33]. Women also relate low-pitched voices to masculinity, hard working ability and leadership [51, 52]. However, the relation between absolute frequency measure (e.g. fundamental or dominant frequency) and body size is not universal as sometimes spacing between formants is a better predictor of the emitter size in mammals (macaques [53]).

Besides protection abilities, female preference for low-pitched voices can be related to other male characteristics such as reproduction qualities. Our study showed that frequency-related voice features are reliable predictors of male reproduction success. Most reports show that fundamental and dominant frequencies are key acoustic features carrying sexual information. For example, the dominant frequency (e.g. Barbary macaques [15]) and the harshness (i.e. atonality) of frequency distribution (e.g. humans [17]) vary throughout the oestrous cycle in females. Female human voices during menstruation appear less attractive to men [17]. Several studies confirm the link between frequencies and fertility. Female high-pitched voices [20] and male low-pitched voices [21] predict higher infant survival rates respectively for indigenous Namibians and Tanzanian hunter-gatherers. The fundamental frequency of male red deer voices was positively correlated with embryo survival rates [22].

However, our study also questions the relevance of different physiological parameters for assessing male reproduction qualities. Here, male reproduction success (measured by artificial inseminations) was not related to sperm quality or hormonal profile. On one hand, this contradicts previous reports showing a correlation between reproduction success of male wild mammals and sperm quality (red deer [54], Mexican gray wolves [55]). On the other hand, the correlation, intensively studied, between semen quality, notably mobility and morphological abnormalities, and fertility of domestic mammals is highly controversial and subject to great inter-study variations [56, 57], notably for horses (reviewed by Graham [35]). Testosterone concentrations and fertility of red deer were not correlated [54]. Humans’ probability of conception increases with increased sperm concentration, but only up to a certain threshold. However semen quantity and quality were not good predictors of pregnancy for Danish couples [58]. As in our study, other authors found no correlations or only limited correlations between voice pitch and sperm quality or quantity (e.g. humans [44]) or testosterone concentrations [36]. Nevertheless, we must acknowledge that semen and acoustic collections were not temporally synchronized in our study which may have played a role in the absence of correlation.

Interestingly, our data revealed a correlation between reproduction success and male calmness. Based on this study (reproduction success, heart beat) and our previous report (size: [33]), we can say that mares prefer the voices of taller stallions with lower heart beat rates and higher fertility. Lower heart beat rates are found in calmer situations and in calmer individuals. Previous studies confirmed that stressful events trigger increase in heart rates (dog [59], horses [60, 61]). The relative influence of these different male characteristics (fertility, size, arousal) on mares’ preferences remains open to debate. However, experiment 2 data indicate that at least stallion size matters, as the preference for low-pitched voices was stronger in taller than in smaller mares. This is an intriguing but interesting finding that deserves further investigation with a larger number of males with different sizes. One possible interpretation suggests that females make choices following an assortative mating / pairing pattern, as do humpback whales [62] and humans [42] for size and fallow deer for age [63]). Another possibility is that previous experience with same breed stallions may have created a memory in these mares, although at least some of them were never mated. Unfortunately, the life history was not known for all these adult females. Although we tested four males and four calls per male, we cannot totally exclude any influence of pseudoreplication. Replicating this playback experiment with larger numbers of males and calls per male would be necessary to control it.

Conclusions

Our study highlights the importance of vocal communication in a species better known as a visual communicant [25]. Stallion acoustic signals may play a role in female attraction at the time of group formation, as well as a role in sexual stimulation or libido maintenance when a group is formed. This supports preliminary findings showing that (1) females base their approach preference towards certain unfamiliar males on the amount of vocal signals uttered during their first encounter [31]; and (2) some male vocal signals trigger mare behavioural preparation for mating (e.g. tail lifting, leg parting) [64, 65]. Our data also confirm that not all male voices are equally attractive [66], opening new lines of bioacoustic research on mammal reproduction.

Supporting Information

S1 Sound. Example of high-pitched acoustic stimulus.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118468.s001

(WAV)

S2 Sound. Example of high-pitched acoustic stimulus.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118468.s002

(WAV)

S3 Sound. Example of low-pitched acoustic stimulus.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118468.s003

(WAV)

S4 Sound. Example of low-pitched acoustic stimulus.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118468.s004

(WAV)

S1 Video. Immediate response in trials with a low-pitched sound on the right.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118468.s005

(WMV)

S2 Video. Immediate response in trials with a low-pitched sound on the left.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118468.s006

(WMV)

Acknowledgments

We thank the direction and the staff of the different stud farms (Le Pin, Lamballe, Saint-Lô) and riding school (Ploermel) for their logistical support, notably Sophie Danvy and Geneviève D’Ardaens, as well as Séverine Henry. We are grateful to Ann Cloarec for correcting the English.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: AL KR FC MT MH. Performed the experiments: KR FC. Analyzed the data: AL KR MT MH. Wrote the paper: AL KR FC MT MH.

References

  1. 1. Lemasson A, Zuberbühler K, Hausberger M. Socially meaningful vocal plasticity in Campbell’s monkeys. J Comp Psychol. 2005; 119: 220–229. pmid:15982165
  2. 2. Lemasson A, Mikus M- A, Blois-Heulin C, Lodé T. Social partner discrimination based on sounds and scents in Asian small-clawed otters (Aonyx cinereus). Naturwissenschaften. 2013;100: 275–279. pmid:23397263
  3. 3. Charlton BD, Reby D, McComb K. Female perception of size-related formant shifts in red deer, Cervus elaphus. Anim Behav. 2007; 74: 707–714.
  4. 4. Charlton BD, Ellis WAH, Brumm J, Nilsson K, Fitch WT. Female koalas prefer bellows in which lower formants indicate larger males. Anim Behav. 2012; 84: 1565–1571.
  5. 5. Charlton BD, Zhihe Z, Snyder RJ. Giant pandas perceive and attend to formant frequency variation in male bleats. Anim Behav. 2010; 79: 1221–1227.
  6. 6. Lemasson A, Palombit RA, Jubin R. Friendships between males and lactating females in a free-ranging group of olive baboons (Papio hamadryas Anubis): evidence from playback experiments. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2008; 62: 1027–1035.
  7. 7. Charrier I, Aubin T, Mathevon N. Calf’s vocal recognition by Atlantic walrus mothers: ecological constraints and adaptations. Anim Cogn. 2010; 13: 471–482. pmid:19960216
  8. 8. Fischer J, Kitchen DM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. Baboon loud calls advertise male quality: acoustic features and their relation to rank, age, and exhaustion. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2004; 56: 140–148.
  9. 9. Mathevon N, Koralek A, Weldele M, Glickman SE, Theunissen FE. What the hyena’s laugh tells: Sex, age, dominance and individual signature in the giggling call of Crocuta crocuta. BMC Ecol. 2010; 10: 9. pmid:20353550
  10. 10. Bruckert L, Liénard JS, Lacrois A, Kreutzer M, Leboucher G. Women use voice parameters to assess men’s characteristics. Proc R Soc B. 2006; 273: 83–89. pmid:16519239
  11. 11. Craul M, Zimmermann E, Radespiel U. First experimental evidence for female mate choice in a nocturnal primate. Primates. 2004; 45: 271–274. pmid:15241637
  12. 12. Geissmann T, Orgeldinger M. The relationship between duet songs and pair bonds in siamangs, Hylobates syndactylus. Anim Behav. 2000; 60: 805–809. pmid:11124879
  13. 13. Pfefferle D, Heistermann M, Hodges JK, Fischer J. Male Barbary macaques eavesdrop on mating outcome: a playback study. Anim Behav. 2008; 75:1885–1891
  14. 14. Taylor AM, Reby D. The contribution of source-filter theory to the study of mammal vocal communication. J Zool. 2010; 280: 221–236.
  15. 15. Semple S, McComb K. Perception of female reproductive state from vocal cues in a mammal species. Proc Biol Sci. 2000; 267: 707–712. pmid:10821617
  16. 16. Charlton BD, Keating JL, Huang Y, Rengui L, Swaisgood RR. Female giant panda (Ailuropoda melanoleuca) chirps advertise the caller’s fertile phase. Proc R Soc B. 2010; 277: 1101–1106. pmid:19955154
  17. 17. Pipitone RN, Gallup GG. The unique impact of menstruation on the female voice: implications for the evolution of menstrual cycle cues. Ethol. 2012; 118:281–291
  18. 18. Evans S, Neave N, Wakelin D, Hamilton C. The relationship between testosterone and vocal frequencies in human males. Physiol Behav. 2008; 93: 783–788. pmid:18155094
  19. 19. Charlton BD, Swaisgood RR, Zhihe Z, Snyder RJ. Giant pandas attend to androgen-related variation in male bleats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2012; 66: 969–974.
  20. 20. Atkinson J, Pipitone RN, Sorokowska A, Sorokowski P, Mberira M, Bartels A, et al. Voice and handgrip strength predict reproductive success in a group of indigenous African females. PLOS ONE 2012; 7: e41811. pmid:22870251
  21. 21. Apicella CL, Feinberg DR, Marlowe FW. Voice pitch predicts reproductive success in male hunter-gatherers. Biol Lett. 2007; 3: 682–684. pmid:17895219
  22. 22. Reby D, McComb K. Anatomical constraints generate honesty: acoustic cues to age and weight in the roars of red deer stags. Anim Behav. 2033; 65: 519–530.
  23. 23. Vannoni E, McElligott AG. Low frequency groans indicate larger and more dominant fallow deer (Dama dama) males. PLOS ONE 2008; 3: e3113. pmid:18769619
  24. 24. Linklater WL. Adaptive explanation in socio-ecology: lessons from the Equidae. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 2000; 75: 1–20. pmid:10740891
  25. 25. Waring GH. Horse behavior. Park Ridge: William Andrew Publishers; 2003.
  26. 26. Feh C. Relationships and communication in socially natural horse herds. In: Miller D, McDonnell S, editors. The domestic horse. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ Press; 2005. pp. 83–93.
  27. 27. Linklater WL, Cameron EZ. Social dispersal but with philopatry reveals incest avoidance in a polygynous ungulate. Anim Behav. 2009; 77: 1085–1093.
  28. 28. Fraser FF. The Behavior of the Horse. London: CAB International; 1992.
  29. 29. Burger D, Wedekind C, Wespi B, Imboden I, Meinecke-Tillmann S, Sieme H. The potential effects of social interactions on reproductive efficiency of stallions. J Equin Vet Sci. 2012; 32: 455–457.
  30. 30. Boyd L. The behaviour of Przewalski’s horses and its importance to their management. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1991; 29: 301–318.
  31. 31. Pickerel TM, Crowell-Davies SL, Caudle AB, Estep DQ. Sexual preference of mares (Equus caballus) for individual stallions. App Anim Behav Sci. 1993; 38: 1–13.
  32. 32. Burger D, Meuwly C, Marti E, Oberthür M, Sieme H, Lazary S, et al. Investigation on female mate choice in horses and possible association with the MHC. Anim Reprod Sci. 2010; 121: 63–64. pmid:20483549
  33. 33. Lemasson A, Boutin A, Boivin S, Blois-Heulin C, Hausberger M. Horse (Equus caballus) whinnies, a source of social information. Anim Cogn. 2009; 12: 693–704. pmid:19449192
  34. 34. Proops L, McComb K, Reby D. Cross-modal individual recognition in domestic horses (Equus caballus). Proc Nat Ac Sci. 2009; 106: 947–951. pmid:19075246
  35. 35. Graham JK. Analysis of stallion semen and its relation to fertility. Vet Clin North Am Equine Pract. 1996; 12: 119–130. pmid:8726454
  36. 36. McDermott JH. Auditory preferences and aesthetics: music, voices, and everyday sounds. In: Sharot T, Dolan RJ, editors. The neuroscience of preference and choice. London: Elsevier; 2011. pp. 227–256.
  37. 37. Burley N. The meaning of assortative mating. Ethol Sociobiol. 1983; 4: 191–293.
  38. 38. Richard J-P. Sound analysis and synthesis using an Amiga micro-computer. Bioacoust. 1991; 3: 45–60.
  39. 39. Palmer E, Fauquenot A. Mesure et prédiction de la fertilité des étalons. Etude méthodologique. In: Jarnige R, Martin-Rosset W, editors. Le Cheval, Reproduction, Sélection, Alimentation, Exploitation. Theix: INRA; 1984. pp. 113–127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2007.12.009 pmid:18328636
  40. 40. Asa CS. Sexual behaviour of mares. Vet Clin No Amer: Equine Pract. 1986; 2: 519–534. pmid:3492243
  41. 41. Feinberg DR, Jones BC, Law Smith MJ, Moore FR, DeBruine LM, Cornwell RE, et al. Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice. Horm Behav. 2006; 49: 215–222. pmid:16055126
  42. 42. Feinberg DR, Jones BC, Little AC, Burt DM, Perrett DI. Manipulations of fundamental and formant frequencies affect the attractiveness of human male voices. Anim Behav. 2005; 69: 561–568.
  43. 43. Puts DA. Mating context and menstrual phase affect women’s preferences for male voice pitch. Evol Hum Behav. 2005; 26: 388–397.
  44. 44. Simmons LW, Peters M, Rhodes G. Low pitched voices are perceived as masculine and attractive but do they predict semen quality in men? PLOS ONE 2011; 6: e29271. pmid:22216228
  45. 45. Re DE, O’Connor JJM, Bennett PJ, Feinberg DR. Preferences for very low and very high voice pitch in humans. PLOS ONE 2012; 7: e32719. pmid:22403701
  46. 46. Hodges-Simeon CR, Gaulin SJC, Puts DA. Voice correlates of mating success in men: examining ‘‘contests” versus ‘‘mate choice” modes of Sexual Selection. Arch Sex Behav. 2011; 40: 551–557. pmid:20369377
  47. 47. Wyman MT, Mooring MS, McCowanc B, Penedod MCT, Reby D, et al. Acoustic cues to size and quality in the vocalizations of male North American bison, Bison bison. Anim Behav. 2012; 84: 1381–1391.
  48. 48. Charlton BD, Zhihe Z, Snyder RJ. The information content of giant panda, Ailuropoda melanoleuca, bleats: acoustic cues to sex, age and size. Anim Behav. 2009; 78: 893–898.
  49. 49. Lemasson A, Mikus M-A, Blois-Heulin C, Lodé T. Vocal repertoire, individual acoustic distinctiveness, and social networks in a group of captive Asian small-clawed otters (Aonyx cinerea). J Mammal. 2014; 95: 128–136.
  50. 50. Kremers D, Lemasson A, Almunia J, Wanker R. Vocal sharing and individual acoustic distinctiveness within a group of captive orcas. J Comp Psychol. 2012; 126: 433–445. pmid:22866769
  51. 51. Apicella CL, Feinberg DR. Voice pitch alters mate-choice-relevant perception in hunter-gatherers. Proc R Soc B. 2009; 276: 1077–1082. pmid:19129125
  52. 52. Klofstad CA, Anderson RC, Peters S. Sounds like a winner: voice pitch influences perception of leadership capacity in both men and women. Proc R Soc B. 2012; 297: 2698–2704.
  53. 53. Fitch WT. Vocal tract length and formant frequency dispersion correlate with body size in rhesus macaques, J Acoust Soc Am. 1997; 102: 1213–1222. pmid:9265764
  54. 54. Gomendio M, Malo AF, Garde JJ, Roldan ERS. Sperm traits and male fertility in natural populations. Reprod. 2007; 134: 1–12.
  55. 55. Asa CS, Miller P, Agnew M, Rebelledo JAR, Lindsey SL, Callahan M, et al. Relationship of inbreeding with sperm quality and reproductive success in Mexican gray wolves. Anim Conserv. 2007; 10: 326–331.
  56. 56. Petrunkina AM, Waberski D, Günzel-Apel AR, Töpfer-Petersen E. Determinants of sperm quality and fertility in domestic species. Reprod. 2007; 134: 3.
  57. 57. Tsakmakidis IA, Lymberopoulos AG, Khalifa TAA. Relationship between sperm quality traits and field-fertility of porcine semen. J Vet Sci. 2010; 11: 151–154. pmid:20458156
  58. 58. Bonde JP, Ernst E, Jensen TK, Hjollund NH, Kolstad H, Henriksen TB, et al. Relation between semen quality and fertility: a population-based study of 430 first-pregnancy planners. Lancet 1998; 352: 1172–1177. pmid:9777833
  59. 59. Beerda B, Schilder MBH, Van Hooff JARAM, De Vries W, Mol JA. Behavioural, saliva cortisol and heart rate responses to different types of stimuli in dogs, Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1998; 58: 365–381.
  60. 60. Visser EK, van Reenen CG, van der Werf JT, Schilder MB, Knaap JH, Barneveld A, et al. Heart rate and heart rate variability during a novel object test and a handling test in young horses. Physiol Behav. 2002; 76: 289–296. pmid:12044602
  61. 61. Sankey C, Henry S, Gorecka-Bruzda A, Richard-Yris MA, Hausberger M. The way to a man’s heart is through his stomach: what about horses? PLOS ONE 2010; 5: e15446. pmid:21085576
  62. 62. Pack AA, Herman LM, Spitz SS, Craig AS, Hakala S, Deakos M, et al. Size-assortative pairing and discrimination of potential mates by humpback whales in the Hawaiian breeding grounds. Anim Behav. 2012; 84: 983–993.
  63. 63. Farrell ME, Briefer E, McElligott AG. Assortative mating in fallow deer reduces the strength of sexual selection. PLOS ONE 2011; 6: e18533. pmid:21494678
  64. 64. Veeckman J, Odberg FO. Preliminary studies on the behavioural detection of oestrus in Belgian “warmblood” mares with acoustic and tactile stimuli. Appl Anim Ethol. 1978; 4: 109–118.
  65. 65. Janson K, McDonnell SM. Estrus detection in mares using contextually congruent stallion vocalization playback with and without stallion scent. Anim Repro Sci. 2010; 121: S123–S125.
  66. 66. McCall CA. Utilizing taped stallion vocalizations as a practical aid in estrus detection in mares. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1991; 28: 305–310.