Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Genetic Interactions with Sex Make a Relatively Small Contribution to the Heritability of Complex Traits in Mice

  • Jon Krohn,

    Affiliation Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics, Oxford, United Kingdom

  • Doug Speed,

    Affiliation UCL Genetics Institute, University College London, London, United Kingdom

  • Rupert Palme,

    Affiliation Department of Biomedical Sciences/Medical Biochemistry, University of Veterinary Medicine, Vienna, Austria

  • Chadi Touma,

    Affiliation Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry, Munich, Germany

  • Richard Mott,

    Affiliation Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics, Oxford, United Kingdom

  • Jonathan Flint

    jf@well.ox.ac.uk

    Affiliation Wellcome Trust Centre for Human Genetics, Oxford, United Kingdom

Abstract

The extent to which sex-specific genetic effects contribute to phenotypic variation is largely unknown. We applied a novel Bayesian method, sparse partitioning, to detect gene by sex (GxS) and gene by gene (GxG) quantitative loci (QTLs) in 1,900 outbred heterogeneous stock mice. In an analysis of 55 phenotypes, we detected 16 GxS and 6 GxG QTLs. The increase in the amount of phenotypic variance explained by models including GxS was small, ranging from 0.14% to 4.30%. We conclude that GxS rarely make a large overall contribution to the heritability of phenotypes, however there are cases where these will be individually important.

Introduction

Genome-wide association studies (GWAS) typically seek only main effects of genetic variation on phenotypes. While this methodology has succeeded in identifying quantitative trait loci (QTL), there are two reasons for being interested in interaction of genetics with other factors, such as sex and aspects of the organisms' environment. First, some loci might not be detected without taking interactions into account. Lander and colleagues have recently argued that a significant portion of ‘missing heritability’ in human GWAS is not due to the failure to detect sequence variants that contribute to phenotypic variation, but is hidden within unacknowledged interactions [1]. Second, identifying QTL involved in interactions might be important for understanding specific mechanisms, such as the biology of sex differences. It is possible that sex effects are manifest in a subset of the main effect QTL, but it is also possible that they represent a completely different set of loci whose biological function is restricted to the sex specific features of the phenotype.

Gene-by-sex (GxS) interaction QTL are genetic contributions to phenotypic variation that manifest themselves differently depending on the organism's sex. In contrast to sex as a main effect, which may induce sex-based dimorphism via broadly acting mechanisms like sex hormones, GxS interactions are associated with a specific locus of the genome and can account for phenotypic variance left unaccounted for by main effects alone. Observations from several species suggest that sex-specific genetic architecture plays a key role in the sex-based dimorphism of many traits, in Drosophila [2], mice [3], and rats [4], and quantitative traits associated with heart disease, hypertension, diabetes, asthma and autoimmune disease in humans [5].

Using crosses between inbred mouse lines, GxS interaction QTL have previously been identified for a number of phenotypes in mice, including body weight [6], [7], fat deposition [8], autoimmunity [9], and susceptibility to cancer [10], [11]. However, the poor mapping resolution inherent in designs that use inbred lines, and the relatively small number of phenotypes examined, leaves open the question of the extent to which GxS QTL contribute to phenotypic variation. Specifically, it is not clear to what extent GxS and main effect loci coincide, nor whether the contribution of GxS varies among phenotypes.

We set out to answer these questions using 55 phenotypes mapped at high resolution in heterogeneous stock (HS) mice. The HS mice are descended from eight inbred progenitors (A/J, AKR/J, BALB/cJ, C3H/HeJ, C57BL/6J, CBA/J, DBA/2J and LP/J [12]), each HS animal consisting of a fine-grained mosaic of the founder chromosomes, hence providing mapping of quantitative traits to an average resolution of about 3 Mb [13].

Mapping GxS loci in the HS has to deal with two problems. First, the degree of relatedness varies between individual HS mice so that mapping is more complicated than in classical inbred strain crosses; mapping in an HS has to take into account this population structure. Second, power to detect interactions is limited by the need to search through many possible combinations of predictors.

Both problems involve finding appropriate models, which is difficult to do with frequentist methods because of the large number of parameters that need to be fitted. Bayesian methods can be designed to deal with this situation by starting with more parameters than can be included in a frequentist approach. In this paper we used a Bayesian analytical tool called Sparse Partitioning [14] to map genetic loci and their interactions. Sparse Partitioning allows for models in which multiple predictors and their interactions influence outcome. This enables us to consider the contributions of GxS and also gene-by-gene interactions (GxG), or epistasis, on the phenotypes in the HS.

Materials and Methods

Ethics statement

Animal work was conducted according to the provisions of the UK Home Office. The protocol was approved by the Local Ethical Review Committee, Oxford University, and by a UK Home Office Project license. The protocol conforms to the principles of refinement, reduction and replacement and all tests were designed to minimize suffering.

Animals and phenotypes

For this experiment we selected 55 phenotypes from fourteen different tests used to assess 1,900 HS mice. Data for this experiment are available from http://mus.well.ox.ac.uk/mouse/HS/and http://mus.well.ox.ac.uk/gscandb/. We selected these phenotypes from a larger set of 100, excluding those highly correlated within a test (for example, in the elevated plus maze, the time mice spent in the open arms was highly correlated with the number of times they entered the open arms), those with asymmetric highly skewed long-tailed distributions, and categorical and latency phenotypes. For an individual phenotype, n ranged from 712 to 1873, with a mean of 51.9% male (range of 50.6% to 56.0%; Table S2). Phenotypes that were highly non-Gaussian were normalised prior to analysis following the Box-Cox power transformation technique (Table S2). Phenotypes were adjusted for relatedness following the method described in [15].

Fitting phenotype predictors

Our predictor set consisted of 12545 SNPs from across the whole genome, plus sex (any other known covariates, such as weight, if significant, were regressed out of phenotypes prior to analysis). To increase computational efficiency, wherever there were SNPs on the same chromosome with 99% concordance, only one of these SNPs was used for analysis (5332 SNPs remained). We applied a Bayesian method (Sparse Partitioning, or SP) that is designed to detect both main effects and interactions simultaneously [14]. SP defines models according to which predictors are associated with the phenotype and which of these predictors interact. SP was configured to settle upon models containing up to ten of the predictors defined in the above paragraph, with at most 2 three-way interactions between these predictors. SP models were iteratively fit over 2000 iterations of Markov Chain Monte Carlo. The first 500 iterations were discarded and the final 1500 used to calculate posterior probabilities. Thus, the posterior probability of association for each predictor is a fraction of 1500 representing the proportion of models in which that predictor was included. The posterior probability of two predictors interacting equals the proportion of models in which those two predictors are included and interact. For our primary SP analysis, any two predictors could interact (allowing both GxS and GxG interactions); to specifically assess the contribution of GxS interactions, we performed a secondary analysis in which only SNPs were allowed to interact (GxG). To obtain a false discovery rate at a given posterior probability threshold, we re-ran SP once for each phenotype using permuted response values. As shown in Figure S1, three interaction effects surpass the 0.2 posterior probability threshold we selected, suggesting we should expect a total of three false positives across all 55 phenotypes.

Comparative fitting with resample model averaging

To confirm SP is appropriate for use with structured populations like the HS mice, we employed a resample model averaging method, Bagphenotype, to map main effect QTL for the same 55 phenotypes. Bagphenotype is an established tool for carrying out GWAS in HS animals and we ran it following a methodology described previously [13]. While SP is a Bayesian method that iteratively removes predictor variables from the models it fits, Bagphenotype is a frequentist method that iteratively adds predictors to the models it fits. With Bagphenotype, we ran one hundred bootstrapped multiple QTL regression models, resulting in a statistic called a resample model inclusion probability (RMIP) out of one hundred representing the strength of each predictive peak across the genome. A peak's RMIP represents the proportion of the multiple QTL models in which the peak's addition to the model both (1) improves the model fit, and (2) does not increase the model's adjusted p-value above a .05 significance threshold. Thus, a particular QTL that is included 70 times out of 100 based on these criteria, that QTL would be assigned an RMIP of .70. The genotypic data fed into Bagphenotype were founder haplotype probabilities, as estimated by HAPPY [16], for each interval between the 12545 genome-wide mouse SNPs. For a comparison with the GxS QTL identified by SP, Bagphenotype was also run using data from each sex alone.

Heritability estimation

We estimated heritability, h2, by constructing from the SNP data a kinship matrix based on alleleic correlations [17], [18], then performing mixed model analysis supposing that the variation for each phenotype can be divided into an additive genetic component (with correlation structure specified by the kinship matrix) and an environmental component (corresponding to an identity matrix). We estimated the genetic and environmental components using REML [19], then our h2 estimate for each phenotype was then the proportion of phenotypic variance estimated to be genetic.

Results

We employed SP to analyse 55 phenotypes in the HS and identified 47 that had a significant main effect of sex on the phenotype (at a 5% FDR). The distribution of effect sizes is highly skewed, ranging from 58% (body weight) to 0.2% (home cage activity), with a median of 2.4%. Figure 1 shows the distribution of effect sizes. Using SP, we identified 60 main effect loci from across the mouse genome (Table S1) above a posterior probability threshold of 0.2. As illustrated in Figure 2, the frequency of QTL below this threshold increases rapidly. QTL with posterior probabilities near zero are likely noise so these were discarded.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Main effects of sex on 55 heterogeneous stock mouse phenotypes.

The vertical axis is the percent of variation explained. The ten largest effects are labeled.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096450.g001

thumbnail
Figure 2. Histogram of the posterior probabilities of the main effect QTLs found by Sparse Partitioning.

This histogram includes all main effect QTLs identified by Sparse Partitioning with a posterior probability greater than .05. The horizontal red line at .20 represents the threshold we selected: QTL above it were retained.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096450.g002

We compared SP main effect results with those from a resample model averaging approach (Bagphenotype) designed for use with structured populations like the HS mice [13]. Conservatively, we included only strictly overlapping QTL in our comparison of the two methods. The summary statistic for Bagphenotype is called a resample model inclusion probability (RMIP). For the 55 phenotypes, there are 294 QTL that exceed an RMIP >.25 threshold (an RMIP threshold of 0.25 was found by simulation to be equivalent to one false positive per genome wide scan [13]). Twenty-six (26/55 = 47.3%) of our SP-identified main effect QTL fell within 2 Mb of the 95% confidence interval (CI) of the Bagphenotype-identified main effect QTL.

We investigated whether there was greater consistency for QTL detected with higher certainty by each of the two methods. We found, as expected, that the posterior probabilities of the matched QTL (mean  = 0.64) were significantly higher than unmatched (mean  = 0.40), Wilcoxon rank sum test W = 581, p<.005. Similarly, the RMIPs of Bagphenotype QTL that matched (n = 26, mean  = 0.84) to SP QTL were significantly higher than those that did not (n = 268, mean  = .51; W = 6236, p<.0001).

Setting the same posterior probability threshold for detecting interactions as main effects (0.20), SP detected 16 GxS interaction QTL (Table 1), summarized by a histogram of posterior probabilities in Figure 3, and well above the three false positives we expected at this threshold based on permutation analysis (Figure S1). These effects were associated with 15 of the 55 phenotypes investigated, with two GxS QTL for high-density lipoproteins (HDL) and one for each of the other 14 phenotypes. The highest GxS interaction posterior probability (.53) involved the time spent freezing in fearful context, and is located at 27.7 megabases (Mb) on chromosome 13.

thumbnail
Figure 3. Histogram of the posterior probabilities of the GxS QTLs found by Sparse Partitioning.

This histogram illustrates the frequency all GxS interaction QTL identified by SP with a posterior probability above the .20 threshold.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096450.g003

The GxS QTL identified by SP in many cases corresponded with main effect QTL we observed when using Bagphenotype on data from only one sex at a time. Of the 16 GxS QTL, six had a single-sex main effect QTL within 5 Mbp of the GxS QTL (female-only for adrenal gland weight and HDL cholesterol; male-only for alanine transaminase, triglycerides, startle response, and B220+ cell percentage) with no main effect QTL present nearby for the other sex. In only one instance, the alkaline phosphatase GxS QTL, was there both male- and female-only main effect QTL within 5 Mbp. Single-sex QTL scan results are provided in Table S3. This adds to the above finding of the considerable overlap (47.3%) of both-sexes-together main effects identified by SP and Bagphenotype. Although highly different mapping methods (SP's Bayesian, Bagphenotype's frequentist; SP removes model parameters, Bagphenotype adds them; SP uses three-level SNP allele data, Bagphenotype uses eight-level founder probabilities), we nevertheless observe consistency in both genetic main effect and GxS interaction QTL.

In addition to fitting GxS interactions, SP was configured to allow GxG interactions between QTL to explain variance in a phenotype. Seven such GxG interactions were identified above our 0.20 posterior probability threshold. Table 2 shows that each of the seven interactions is associated with a different phenotype; two also had GxS interactions (triglycerides and area of the glucose response curve) and four that did not (CD8+ T-cell count; blood glucose level; mean corpuscular hemoglobin, MCH; and mean corpuscular volume, MCV). The GxG interactions with the highest posterior probabilities (.45) were associated with MCH.

To estimate how much phenotypic variance was attributable to GxS interactions, cross-validation was employed. Cross-validation randomly selects a proportion of the data with which the model is trained, and subsequently tests how well this model fits the unselected data. Here, we divided the data into ten evenly-sized tranches. Nine tranches were used to fit predictors to the data and then the quality of this fit was tested upon the tenth tranche. This was repeated ten times, allowing us to rotate through the ten tranches as the test tranche. We performed cross-validation twice with each of the 15 phenotypes that included a GxS interaction: first while allowing sex to act as both a marginal effect and as an interacting term with SNPs, then again while allowing sex to act only as a marginal effect. In both models, SNPs were able to interact (i.e., epistasis). The proportion of variance explained under these two scenarios is summarised in Table 3. The specific contribution of GxS interactions was assessed by considering how much more variance was explained under the scenario allowing GxS interactions.

thumbnail
Table 3. The percentage of phenotypic variance explained by two models: one permitting main and GxS effects, and one permitting only main effects.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096450.t003

The best fitting models are for adrenal weight, where the GxS interaction-prohibited and -permitted models explain 12.3% and 13.9% of the phenotypic variance, respectively. The proportion of variance explained by the GxS interaction-permitted models was higher than in their GxS interaction-prohibited counterparts, ranging from 0.14% (area of the glucose response curve) to 4.30% (body weight) higher. Other than adrenal weight (1.57%), allowing GxS interactions did not account for more than about half a percent of the phenotypic variance.

Across the 15 phenotypes with a GxS QTL identified by SP, the proportion of phenotypic variance explained by sex as a main effect (determined by a simple linear model) correlates with the proportion of variance explained by GxS interactions, r = .57, t(14)  = 2.6, p<.05 (column four, “difference”, in Table 3). The correlation is shown in Figure 4.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Correlation between GxS and main effect of sex.

The proportion of phenotypic variance explained by sex as a main effect is plotted on the horizontal axis and the proportion of variance explained by the interaction effect is shown on the vertical axis. The two correlate with a P value <0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096450.g004

Heritability estimates, h2, for all phenotypes are provided in Table S4. h2 did not correlate significantly with the proportion of phenotypic variance explained by main effects alone, nor by the variance explained by main and interaction effects together, nor with the proportion of variance explained by GxS interactions (columns two through four in Table 3; p>.1 in all three cases).

Discussion

Our principle finding is that GxS interactions contribute little to phenotypic variation in addition to that attributable to main effect QTL. While we found GxS QTL at just over a quarter of phenotypes, the median percentage of variation accounted for by a GxS QTL was 0.23% (Table 3). By contrast, the main effect QTL had a median contribution of 6.2% (maximum  = 24.7%). Furthermore, since every GxS QTL we identified coincides with one of the 60 main effect QTL (Table S1), our data indicate that the GxS effect does not arise from biological pathways independent of those of the main effect QTL.

The search for GxG interactions yielded an even smaller list of loci. Seven epistatic interactions were found, each for a different phenotype (Table 2). Three of the GxG interactions coincided precisely with main effect QTL (loci interacting to predict triglyceride levels, loci for area under the curve of glucose levels, loci for CD4+ cell count). Two others (for mean corpuscular haemoglobin (MCH) and mean cellular volume) lie within 2 Mb of main effect QTL.

Attempts to find sex specific effects have met with varying success, partly because of methodological limitations. A review of the literature on human genetic association studies, despite identifying 432 claims for sex-specificity, concluded that the majority of claims were spurious [20]. Yet there is evidence from twin and genetic linkage studies that, for some phenotypes, a considerable proportion of the genetic variance is sex specific. Thus in two independent twin studies of the heritability of depression, Kendler and colleagues [21], [22] estimate that genetic correlation in risk factors for major depression in men and women to be approximately 0.6. In an analysis of 17 quantitative phenotypes, subject to genetic linkage analysis, Ober and colleagues reported that eleven were sexually dimorphic, twelve showed evidence of differences between the sexes in heritability or linkage, and all three genome-wide significant linkage peaks were significant when tested for an interaction between sex and genotype [5].

Our results add to this debate by finding evidence for GxS at just over a quarter of phenotypes, suggesting that GxS QTL are relatively common, in agreement with the genetic linkage analyses of human phenotypes [5]. Furthermore, we observed a linear relationship between sex effects and GxS: the larger the main effect of sex, the larger the effect of the interaction loci (Figure 4). This justifies the reasonable assumption that it will be worth examining highly sexually dimorphic phenotypes for GxS effects. This linear relationship between sex effects and GxS might be interpreted to mean that the genetic basis of sex differences arise from the conjoint effect of many loci, rather than being due to a specific and relatively constrained biological pathway. However it is important to realize that the distribution of effect sizes we observed is skewed. This may indicate that in some phenotypes the sex effect arises from a few key loci. One example might be the GxS interaction based at 21 Mbp on chromosome seven that explains 1.5% of adrenal gland weight.

One important caveat to our approach is that we found relatively few main effect loci. Compared to the 294 main effect QTL found with a model averaging mapping method, SP found just 60 loci, of which 43% were common to the two methods. This raises the question as to whether reliance on SP for QTL mapping might be biasing our results. It is surprising, for example, that we found no GxS for weight, even though this phenotype has by far the largest sex effect of any phenotype we measured (58%, Figure 1). This is most likely due to our relatively low power, given the expected small effects of each GxS QTL.

While the smaller numbers of main effects found with SP suggests that more interacting loci might exist, it does not invalidate our main finding of the paucity of interacting loci and their small effect size, relative to main effect loci. For example reducing the posterior probability threshold for SP results would not identify sufficient additional GxS effects to alter our conclusion that this set of loci makes only a small contribution to the total genetic variance (Figure 5).

thumbnail
Figure 5. Proportion of phenotypic variance explained by GxS versus by main effects.

For the 15 phenotypes that had a GxS QTL, this plot depicts the percentage of phenotypic variance explained by models where sex was permitted to act as a main effect only relative to when it could act as a both main effect and interaction term. As detailed in Table 3, allowing GxS interaction effects in the model at least marginally improved the amount of phenotypic variance explained by predictors. Thus, all the points fall above the grey line, x = y. It is clear from the figure that adrenal gland weight (“aw”) had the greatest improvement in its variance explained by allowing interacting predictors (difference of 1.6%). Additional non-trivial abbreviations are as follows: chloride (“Cl”), triglycerides (“tg”), time spent frozen in fearful context (“c”), time spent frozen after fearful cue (“CuF”), startle response (“FPS”), area of glucose response curve (“ga”), B220+ cell percentage (“B”), and boli produced in the open field test (“b”). ALT occupied nearly the same position as chloride so is represented by the same symbol (“Cl”).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096450.g005

The advantage of using SP is that it is less subject to the problem of multiple testing. Like other sparse Bayesian methods, SP allows for very complicated models defined by a very large number of parameters. It does this by assuming most parameters are zero (i.e., that most predictors do not influence the outcome either marginally or through interactions). This type of approach is particularly effective in situations where the number of predictors in a model greatly exceeds the sample size (the large p, small n problem). For GxG interactions, where a genome scan would involve testing pairwise interactions between thousand of loci, identifying significant effects would otherwise require extremely large sample sizes, too large to be feasible.

Finally, although we found that first-order interactions contribute little to phenotypic variation beyond main effect QTL and other covariates, we emphasize that this does not imply that they can be ignored. One example is the GxS interaction based at 21 Mbp on chromosome seven influencing adrenal gland weight. While GxS rarely make a large overall contribution to the missing heritability of phenotypes, there are cases where there will be individually important.

Supporting Information

Figure S1.

Posterior probabilities of interaction effects obtained using permuted values for each of the phenotypes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096450.s001

(DOCX)

Table S1.

Main effect QTL identified by sparse partitioning.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096450.s002

(DOCX)

Table S2.

By phenotype, the Box-Cox power technique-identified transform to Gaussian distribution, the number of HS mice, and number of mice that were male.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096450.s003

(DOCX)

Table S3.

Main effect QTL identified using resample model averaging and data from only one sex. Those within 5 Mbp of GxS QTL identified by sparse partitioning (Table 1) are shown in bold.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096450.s004

(DOCX)

Table S4.

Heritability estimates across phenotypes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096450.s005

(DOCX)

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: JF JK. Performed the experiments: CT RP. Analyzed the data: RM JK DS. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: RP CT. Wrote the paper: JK JF.

References

  1. 1. Zuk O, Hechter S, Sunyaev SR, Lander ES (2012) Genetic interactions create phantom heritability. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109: 1193–1198.
  2. 2. Mackay T (2004) The genetic architecture of quantitative traits: lessons from Drosophila. Curr Opin Genet Dev 14: 253–257.
  3. 3. Peirce J, Derr R, Shendure J, Kolata T, LM S (1998) A major influence of sex-specific loci on alcohol preference in C57Bl/6 and DBA/2 inbred mice. Mamm Genome 9: 942–948.
  4. 4. Mattson D, Dwinell M, Greene A, Kwitek A, Roman R, et al. (2007) Chromosomal mapping of the genetic basis of hypertension and renal disease in FHH rats. Am J Physiol Renal Physiol 293: F1905–1914.
  5. 5. Weiss L, Pan L, Abney M, Ober C (2006) The sex-specific genetic architecture of quantitative traits in humans. Nat Genet 38: 218–222.
  6. 6. Brockmann G, Haley C, Renne U, Knott S, Schwerin M (1998) Quantitative trait loci affecting body weight and fatness from a mouse line selected for extreme high growth. Genetics 150: 369–381.
  7. 7. Brockmann G, Karatayli E, Haley C, Renne U, Rottmann O, et al. (2004) QTLs for pre- and postweaning body weight and body composition in selected mice. Mamm Genome 15: 593–609.
  8. 8. Taylor B, Tarantino L, Phillips S (1999) Gender-influenced obesity QTLs identified in a cross involving the KK type II diabetes-prone mouse strain. Mamm Genome 10: 963–968.
  9. 9. Wanstrat A, Wakeland E (2001) The genetics of complex autoimmune diseases: non-MHC susceptibility genes. Nat Immunol 2: 802–809.
  10. 10. Fijneman R, de Vries S, Jansen R, Demant P (1996) Complex interactions of new quantitative trait loci, Sluc1, Sluc2, Sluc3, and Sluc4, that influence the susceptibility to lung cancer in the mouse. Nat Genet 14: 465–467.
  11. 11. van Wezel T, Ruivenkamp C, Stassen A, Moen C, Demant P (1999) Four new colon cancer susceptibility loci, Scc6 to Scc9 in the mouse. Cancer Res 59: 4216–4218.
  12. 12. Demarest K, McCaughran JJ, Mahjubi E, Cipp L, Hitzemann R (1999) Identification of an acute ethanol response quantitative trait locus on mouse chromosome 2. J Neurosci 19: 549–561.
  13. 13. Valdar W, Solberg L, Gauguier D, Burnett S, Klenerman P, et al. (2006) Genome-wide genetic association of complex traits in heterogeneous stock mice. Nat Genet 38: 879–887.
  14. 14. Speed D, Tavare S (2011) Sparse partitioning: nonlinear regression with binary or tertiary predictors, with application to association studies. The Annals of Applied Statistics 5: 873–893.
  15. 15. Atwell S, Huang Y, Vilhjalmsson B, Willems G, Horton M, et al. (2010) Genome-wide association study of 107 phenotypes in Arabidopsis thaliana inbred lines. Nature 465: 627–631.
  16. 16. Mott R, Talbot C, Turri M, Collins A, Flint J (2000) A method for fine mapping quantitative trait loci in outbred animal stocks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97: 12649–12654.
  17. 17. Astle W, Balding D (2009) Population structure and cryptic relatedness in genetic association studies. Statist Sci 24: 387–576.
  18. 18. Speed D, Hemani G, Johnson M, Balding D (2012) Improved heritability estimation from genome-wide SNPs. Am J Hum Genet 91: 1011–1021.
  19. 19. Corbeil R, Searle S (1976) Restricted maximum likelihood (REML) estimation of variance components in the mixed model. Technometrics 18: 31–38.
  20. 20. Patsopoulos N, Tatsioni A, Ioannidis J (2007) Claims of sex differences: an empirical assessment in genetic associations. J Am Med Assoc 298: 880–893.
  21. 21. Kendler K, Gardner C, Neale M, Prescott C (2001) Genetic risk factors for major depression in men and women: similar or different heritabilities and same or partly distinct genes? Psychol Med 31: 605–616.
  22. 22. Kendler K, Gatz M, Gardner C, Pedersen N (2006) A Swedish national twin study of lifetime major depression. Am J Psychiatry 163: 109–114.