Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The Monkey Puzzle: A Systematic Review of Studies of Stress, Social Hierarchies, and Heart Disease in Monkeys

  • Mark Petticrew ,

    mark.petticrew@lshtm.ac.uk

    Affiliation Department of Social and Environmental Health Research, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London, United Kingdom

  • George Davey Smith

    Affiliation Centre for Causal Analyses in Translational Epidemiology (CAiTE), School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom

Abstract

Background

It is often suggested that psychosocial factors, such as stress, or one's social position, may play an important role in producing social gradients in human disease. Evidence in favour of this model of health inequalities has relied, in part, on studies of the health effects of the natural social hierarchies found among non-human primates. This study aimed to assess the strength of this evidence.

Methodology/Principal Findings

A systematic review was carried out to identify all studies of psychosocial factors and coronary artery disease (CAD) in non-human primates. We searched databases (MEDLINE, PsycInfo, EMBASE, and Primatelit from inception to November 2010) to identify experimental and observational studies of the impact of social reorganisation, social instability, and disruption of dominance hierarchies on primate CAD outcomes. We also handsearched bibliographies and examined the citations to those studies in public health articles. Fourteen studies were found which presented evidence on CAD and social status and/or psychosocial stress. These suggested that the association between social status and disease may be sex-specific: in female monkeys dominant status may be protective, with subordinate females having a greater extent of atherosclerosis. In male monkeys the reverse may be the case.

Conclusions/Significance

Overall, non-human primate studies present only limited evidence for an association between social status and CAD, Despite this, there is selective citation of individual non-human primate studies in reviews and commentaries relating to human disease aetiology. Such generalisation of data from monkey studies to human societies does not appear warranted.

Introduction

Socioeconomic gradients in health have been observed in most countries and their existence has been widely accepted by most public health researchers, although the patterns differ for different diseases [1]. There is less agreement about the causes of these patterns, with ongoing debate between proponents of psychosocial and neo-materialist theories. The former group of explanations has been particularly prominent. These suggest that socio-economic position (at an individual level) and income inequality (at a community or country level) influence health primarily through psychosocial mechanisms [2]. A variety of such mechanisms have been discussed – for example, social stress, hostility, lack of control over work and social isolation – with one influential hypothesis being that one's perceived position in the social hierarchy is a particularly important underlying determinant of health and health inequalities [3], [4].

This hypothesis has been supported by international comparisons which suggest that psychosocial factors play an important role in producing social gradients in disease [5]. Conflicting evidence has, however, also been presented, with some evidence that that it may be more important to consider the complex interactions between historical, cultural, economic and other influences on inequalities [6]. The investigation of whether the primary causes of inequalities are psychosocial or materialist has broadened in recent years beyond the study of human populations, to include studies of other primates, such as the old world monkeys, particularly macaques. In such studies researchers based in the US have investigated the impact of social reorganisation, social instability, and the disruption of dominance hierarchies on outcomes such as CAD, and CAD risk factors. For instance, the eminent primatologist Robert Sapolsky's studies of free-living baboon troops in East Africa have found a receptive audience among public health researchers seeking to understand the direct health effects of social status in the absence of material differences in human societies [3], [7], [8], [9], [10], [11], [12], [13].

Generalisation from these non-human, but closely-related primate societies is often seen as appropriate because they are biologically similar to us, develop equivalent diseases, and, like most humans, live in complex, hierarchical societies. Their social organisations are also more amenable to experimental manipulation than human societies; in particular, the effects of deliberate experimental manipulation of social status on coronary arteries can be measured directly in primates in a way that is impossible in humans.

However before one can extrapolate from primate to human societies it is a reasonable to examine the strength of the evidence presented by these studies. There are many possible studies, of different designs, examining differing hypotheses, and not all researchers agree that findings from these studies should be applied to humans. While systematic reviews of animal studies are uncommon, they are important in this as in other fields as a means of assembling and exploring the evidence, exploring inconsistencies and more generally assessing the strength of evidence in support of or against a particular hypothesis. In this case, the review aimed to assess the strength of the evidence describing the association between social hierarchies, social stress and coronary artery disease (CAD) in primates, in the same way that one would review the strength of the epidemiological evidence for an association in studies of humans. We focus on these psychosocial characteristics because these are frequently cited as important mechanisms. A second, related aim of the review was to explore how this evidence has been used in the debate about human inequalities in health.

From monkey hierarchies to human health

As noted above, the view that one can map such studies in monkeys onto the debate about human inequalities is not without its critics. There is a growing recognition among primatologists that social rank in primates has less to do with physiology than has previously been thought [14]. If this is the case, then theories about human health based on monkey studies may not be well-supported, and it may therefore be timely for research into socioeconomic determinants of health to examine the evidence that psychosocial factors affect health in monkeys, and to consider how this evidence should influence theories about the development of health inequalities in human societies.

We know that in the health literature more generally, “positive” findings are cited much more often than “negative” findings, and that, in general, selective citation and mis-citation can have a distorting effect, overstating the strength of associations between variables and downplaying the importance of studies which do not “fit the argument” [15], [16]. Knowledge of this bias has fostered the use of systematic literature reviews, which attempt to locate, appraise and synthesise all the relevant research evidence on a particular topic, rather than a selected sample of such evidence. However there is no systematic review of the results of the monkey studies to guide public health researchers, although there have been calls for a more systematic approach to the synthesis of animal studies [17].

What do the primate studies show?

A systematic review of the non-human primate literature provides a basis for assessing the strength of the evidence that status in monkeys predicts ill health, as well as providing an indirect test of the psychosocial hypothesis in humans. An appropriate starting point for such a review is to examine the results of the primate studies of the effect of social stress on actual CAD outcomes. The specific null hypotheses tested here are (i) that there is no association between social hierarchy and CAD outcomes, and (ii) that increased stress in these animals is not associated with increased risk of CAD. These specific hypotheses were chosen because the psychosocial model has drawn upon studies of these mechanisms and outcomes in monkeys to propose possible explanations for health inequalities in humans; although other indirect outcomes are of interest (e.g., health behaviours) it is the direct, negative health effects of hierarchies on disease outcomes that have been the subject of most debate, rather than the indirect effects (e.g., effects on health behaviours).

Methods

Search strategy

We undertook an extensive electronic search and hand search of bibliographies in order to identify studies of the impact of social status or social stress on CAD outcomes in monkeys. We searched MEDLINE (1950–Nov 2010), PsycInfo, EMBASE, and Primatelit from inception (i.e., earliest possible start date) up to the end of November 2010, using free text search terms primate* or monkey*, or macaque*, plus heart, athero*, disease, artery, coronary, CHD, or CAD, plus social stress, stress*, or status. While we accepted that this simple search strategy would produce a large number of irrelevant hits we felt that this would be manageable, and that a thorough search of bibliographies would identify any additional studies missed by the electronic searches. We therefore conducted an extensive search of the bibliographies of all primary studies, previous review papers, and book chapters.

Study inclusion criteria

We included studies of social reorganisation, social instability, and disruption of dominance hierarchies. Only studies reporting CAD outcomes (such as extent of arteriosclerosis, size of intimal area, and plaque or lesion size) were included. Studies which reported only risk factors without disease outcomes were excluded. Studies reported in any language were included. Studies of social status/rank, social reorganisation, social instability or social stress with CAD outcomes, as reported by study authors (e.g., extent of atherosclerosis; plaque size; size of intimal area; or other measure of presence or extent of disease) in monkeys were included. Studies with no CAD outcomes (e.g., studies which included only changes in stress hormone or glucocorticoid levels, heart rate reactivity, blood pressure, or lipid levels as outcomes) were excluded. These inclusion criteria were applied by two reviewers working independently.

The intervention being investigated in this review is therefore social reorganisation, social instability, or disruption of dominance hierarchies. The comparison group is either animals whose position in the social hierarchy was not disrupted, or who were at a different position in the hierarchy (e.g. dominant vs subordinate animals are compared). Both experimental and observational study designs were eligible for inclusion.

Study selection

One reviewer screened the titles and abstracts for possible inclusion to produce a list of 148 possibly relevant studies. The final decision on inclusion was made by two reviewers.

Quality assessment

Methodological information relating to study quality was tabulated and checked by two reviewers. We used the widely-used Quality Assessment Tool for quantitative studies developed by the Effective Public Health Practice Project at McMaster University (see: http://www.ephpp.ca/Tools.html).

Data collection process

Data were extracted by one reviewer and checked by a second. These were summarised in the overall summary table (Table 1), with the data on the methodological assessment in Table 2; the full data appear in Table S1. We included numbers and sex of animals in the study, study design, CAD outcome data where reported, and the statistical significance of any test where this was reported. Effect size data were extracted directly from each paper, or were recalculated where possible.

thumbnail
Table 1. Studies of the effects of social stress and/or social status on the development of CAD in non-human primates (full results table available: Table T1).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027939.t001

thumbnail
Table 2. Methodological assessment of the included studies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027939.t002

Summary measures, and synthesis of results

The main summary measures used in the studies were extent of coronary artery atherosclerosis in mm2; % stenosis (for example. “extent of atherosclerosis in arterial section, measured as the area in mm2 between the internal; elastic lamina and the lumen” [18]); and % of animals with serious CAD The findings were summarised narratively, given the heterogeneity in study designs and the observational nature of the data.

Citation search

We hand-searched the full-text and bibliographies of articles and book chapters on health inequalities, and carried out citation searches in Web of Science to identify papers which had cited any of the studies we identified (Table 3). We also checked the references of a 2009 review which summarised the findings of studies conducted by the main research laboratory working in this field [19]. Finally, to inform the discussion section of this paper, a citation search was run on each included study and the titles and abstracts of the citing papers were reviewed.

thumbnail
Table 3. Citations of primate studies in public health articles up to 2009.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027939.t003

Results

Studies of status, social stress and CAD

Our searches produced 3020 hits (See PRISMA flowchart; Figure S1], from which we identified 14 studies (Table 1; methodological assessment appears in Table 2) which met the inclusion criteria. Social status generally emerges as a result of encounters among individual animals, in some cases following a period of deliberate disruption of the social groups by experimenters. The findings are summarised in these two broad categories below. Ten studies presented information on the relationship between social status (dominance, subordinance) and CAD outcomes. Four studies reported on the effects of social disruption or other social stress. Two studies reported data on both social status, and stress.

Studies of social status (Table 1)

The ten studies of social status and atherosclerosis reported variously that dominant status is protective of CAD (Study Nos. 3, 6, 9,11) [20], [21], [22], [23], that it is a risk factor for CAD (Study No. 7) [18], and that there is no association (Study No. 13) [24]. One other study of female monkeys presented too few data to assess the relationship (Study No.2) [25]. There may be an interaction with sex, such that subordinate females have a greater extent of atherosclerosis than dominant females (Study Nos. 1, 3, 6, 9,11) [20], [21], [22], [23], [26], while the reverse association has been reported for males, (Study Nos. 4,7) [18], [27] although not in all studies (Study No. 3) [20]. One study (No. 13) [24] of female monkeys reported no difference in the extent of atherosclerosis between subordinate and dominant animals, and another study found that change in social status, in both dominant and subordinate female animals, predicted the extent of atherosclerosis (Study No. 12) [28]. One study reported that there was no main effect for dominance in male monkeys, though dominant monkeys in unstable social conditions developed more severe atherosclerosis (Study No. 4) [27].

Studies of psychosocial stress

Six studies (Studies numbered 4–6, 8, 12, and 15 in Table 1) examined the effects on CAD of psychosocial stress (generally in the form of experimental disruption to social groups). (Two studies reported data on both social status, and social stressors: Study Nos. 4 [27] and 6 [21]) In one study, male monkeys were separated into unstable (periodically disrupted) and stable groups, and the extent of atherosclerosis was measured at follow-up (Study No. 4) [27] No main effect was found for social stress, although an interaction with status was observed, such that dominant animals had more severe atherosclerosis in conditions of social disruption (this study was also described in the previous section). In a further study in male monkeys (Study No. 5) [29], [30] the same researchers periodically reorganised the animals' social groups and compared these animals to those in a control group; the extent of CAD was found to be greater in the reorganised (stressed) group. This finding was not confirmed in a subsequent study in male and female monkeys (Study No. 6), which found no difference between stable and unstable groups [21]. One study (Study No. 8) [31] found that stressed male monkeys on a high fat diet had more atherosclerosis than stressed monkeys consuming a low fat diet, or unstressed monkeys. Two other studies (Studies 12 [32] and 14 [33]) in adult male cynomolgus monkeys found no associated between stress due to social reorganisation and CAD. One further small study (not tabulated) has reported that social stress caused CAD, but few data were presented [34].

Discussion

In summary, the studies of social status in primates do not appear to provide strong evidence that social status is an important predictor of CAD in monkeys. Rather, they seem to suggest that the relationships between social status and CAD may be sex-specific, and in particular that subordinate status is a more fallible predictor of atherosclerosis than is sometimes assumed. Indeed, in male monkeys it appears that dominant, not subordinate, status may sometimes be pathological. With respect to the studies which have examined the effects of social stress on CAD outcomes, the studies are suggestive, but there are probably too few data at present to come to any firm conclusions.

One study here is of particular interest, as it is widely cited as providing supporting evidence for a pathological relationship between low social rank and poor health [28]. In this study monkeys were randomly allocated to 4-member groups for 8 weeks until the social rankings stabilised. Dominant monkeys were then housed together, and the subordinates housed together, forming 4 groups in total: females that were initially dominant, and remained dominant after regrouping (n = 11), monkeys that were initially dominant, but became subordinate after regrouping (n = 11); those that were initially subordinate, but became dominant (n = 8) and those that were initially subordinate, and remained so (n = 12). One finding from this study is often cited in support of the psychosocial interpretation of health inequalities, to the effect that among dominants that became subordinate the extent of coronary artery atherosclerosis increased by 500% - which is used as evidence of the harmful effects of low social status (See Table S2). This figure is however based on a comparison between the extent of atherosclerosis among monkeys who stayed at the same rank (either dominant or subordinate) (mean extent of CAA approximately  = 0.035 mm2), and those who became subordinate (mean =  0.19 mm2) and these data from only 42 monkeys are adjusted for skewness, heterogeneity of variance, and two potential CAD risk factors (thigh circumference and ratio of TPC-HDL). The unadjusted data (as estimated from the original scatterplot shown in Figure S2) seem to show no meaningful association between plaque size, and rank. Moreover, monkeys who were previously subordinate, but became dominant, also experienced an increase in CAA, by 44% greater than the subordinates that stayed subordinate. If any message is to be drawn from this small study, then the message is that change in status in either direction, and not status itself, is pathogenic. The researchers who conducted the study themselves point this out: “All animals with altered social positions (dominants that became subordinate, and subordinates that became dominant) had worsened coronary artery atherosclerosis” (page 725) The straightforward “500% increase” claim on its own is therefore misleading; if anything, subordinate monkeys are better off staying subordinate.

Finally, though we did not formally assess publication bias, the risk of this and other biases in this small sample of small studies should also be borne in mind, including outcome reporting bias.

Use of the primate evidence in public health

Although as we have seen the evidence appears equivocal, these studies are frequently cited in epidemiological and public health literature in support of the “psychosocial hypothesis”, a conceptual model describing the relationships between social stress, human hierarchies, and human health outcomes [35]. Robert Sapolsky's baboons (not included in this review, as objective CAD measures have not been collected on this group) are particularly prominent in these discussions:

“The Whitehall and Serengeti studies are in a sense starting from opposite ends of a possible bridge. While the baboons show hierarchically associated variations in physiological responses to stress that are consistent with health effects, the civil servants show hierarchical variations in health outcomes that must emerge from some physiological pathway.” [36]

The experimental studies in macaques are also used to suggest that the low status is harmful in monkeys and by extension, in humans. As illustration, Table 3 shows the frequency of citation of most of the primate studies whose results are described above. A sizeable percentage of citations derive from articles in epidemiology and public health journals, but more interesting is the popularity of the Shively and Clarkson (1994) study described above – most of its citations are by researchers outside of the field of primatology, and in these papers it is often cited in the context of discussion of the direct health effects of human social status [37], [38]. Despite its equivocal findings, this study, more than any other primate study, is used to support the view that low rank in human societies is directly harmful to health. The disconnect between the reality of available non-human primate data and the interpretation given to it by commentators from various disciplines has been commented on in a different context [39].

A common form of citation is to refer to both the Shively and Clarkson study of female monkeys (which studied coronary atheroma), and Sapolsky's study of male monkeys (which examined blood lipid levels) (See Table S2 for examples). This suggests that the finding relating to CAD is robust and also relates to males, but when studies of CAD in male monkeys are examined the results are, if anything, in an opposite direction to those (possibly) found by Shively for female monkeys. A further popular claim is that a dramatic five-fold increase in atherosclerosis was generated by downward mobility in the social hierarchy, again referring to the Shively and Clarkson study, though the unadjusted data show little effect.

The limits to generalisation

The evidence from these studies does not provide strong support for a psychosocial explanation of health inequalities. The data derive from studies, which are, almost by necessity, small, ranging from 23 to 193 animals. By contrast, the number of statistical tests carried out in these studies is often large, and power calculations to justify the sample sizes are absent. The need for small sample sizes is understandable in primate research, as is the need to include animal participants in more than one study, but similar biases apply to animal studies as apply to other epidemiological studies, including lack of intention to treat analyses, and lack of blinding of outcome assessment in most studies. Drop-outs (e.g. in this case due to animals dying) are however generally low, at least compared to community-based studies in humans. Other unknown observer biases may also be operating; for example, one meta-analysis of field studies of baboon behaviour has reported that observers recording behaviour of troops of baboons had a tendency to favour watching larger groups, which tend to travel less far, and also differ in other behaviours [40].

Primatologists themselves have warned repeatedly about over-generalising from primate data to human societies [12], [41], [42]. Indeed the data may not even be generalisable between similar species of monkey, as comparative research and field studies suggest that there are striking differences in group composition, social spacing, dominance and aggression between species [41]. The social and hierarchical behaviour of Macaca fascicularis, the species used in many of these studies, may not therefore even be representative of all of its own genus, which raises doubt about extrapolation to higher primates.

Robert Sapolsky has raised another problem for researchers seeking to generalise to humans. He describes “the circuitous and often tragic routes by which primates come to find themselves in laboratories…It is not generally known that there is an extremely high mortality rate among primates during transit…survivors may well be those which have passed successfully through what evolutionists refer to a “selective bottleneck” – and either may be physically and psychiatrically robust, or permanently weakened by their vicissitudes…they could well be “supermonkeys” [43]. Even if this is not the case, he concludes that the primates available for study represent a far-from-random sample.

This in itself represents a limitation of our review, in that it examines evidence from a selected group of animals. Perhaps more importantly in some cases the investigation of social status is not a primary focus of the study (see Table 1, column 1), and in all cases the numbers are small and so they may have had limited power to detect real differences in outcomes between subordinate and dominant (or stressed and unstressed) animals. This heterogeneity in study design and study purpose makes drawing firm conclusions about CAD and social position in primates difficult.

Simplistic interpretations of human hierarchies have also been criticised by Rohde (2001), who agrees that human hierarchies are common, with governments, religions, workplaces, and schools often arranged hierarchically [44]. The term “hierarchy” is of Greek origin, originally referred to ranks of ecclesiastical rulers, and was later used to describe the pecking order of angels. Hierarchies often appear rapidly and spontaneously, and phrases like “pecking order” itself are well–recognised. However interpretation of human hierarchies is often difficult, and Rohde suggests four reasons for this. Firstly, higher-ranking people often make paradoxically submissive gestures, (such as allowing others to pass through a doorway first, and there is the obligation of the strong to care for the weak in some religions and in other forms of social obligation, and for the well to care for the sick). Secondly, human hierarchical aspirations can be expressed purely cognitively, with no obligation to action, because humans, with symbolic thought, can model likely outcomes of confrontations. Thirdly, there is a thick cultural veneer (including manners) which overlays hierarchical relations in humans. Finally, humans may have many alternative hierarchies, with perfectly satisfactory alternative hierarchical positions [43].

Primate social rank and the stress response

This overview has concentrated on CAD outcomes, but studies have also examined associations between dominance hierarchies and hormonal responses. These markers of stress response also appear to suggest psychoneuroendocrine pathways underlying the development of human inequalities in cardiovascular disease. For example it has been suggested that there is an established relationship between high basal cortisol levels and subordinate status, and that this is consistent across primate species [45]. However a comparative analysis of this issue reached the opposite conclusion. This study involved a systematic review of data on subordinate/dominant cortisol ratios across ten different primate species, and found cortisol ratios associated with dominant/subordinate status to be highly heterogeneous. The authors concluded that there is no consistent relationship between social rank and stress response in primates and argue that there is no generalisable relationship between social status and any aspect of stress physiology across monkey species [46] (Table 4). A recent large study has also found that the highest rank wild male baboons had an unfavourable profile of stress-related hormonal measure, in contradiction to the anticipated favourable effects of hierarchy on such indicators [47].

thumbnail
Table 4. Variation in subordinate/dominant cortisol ratios, by primate species [45].

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027939.t004

Finally, this review provides an example of the difficulty in extrapolating from very few or single studies, particularly when those few studies provide conflicting evidence. There are many examples in the literature of where single studies taken out of context may be misleading, which has fostered an awareness of the risks of relying on single studies, and of the need for comprehensive systematic reviews of the evidence. However such reviews are still relatively uncommon outside of the health and social sciences, though the case has been made previously for the need for more systematic reviews of animal research in particular, both to summarise existing literature and to help direct future research [48], [49].

Conclusions

Two conclusions can be drawn from this review of the monkey evidence on social rank and CAD. The first is that non-human primate studies present limited evidence for an association between rank and CAD in monkeys; the effects of stress, and social status appear to be more inconsistent than is often assumed, and the relationships may be sex-specific. The data presented to support these associations themselves are also limited, deriving from small studies in highly–selected populations. We took the view that the strongest evidence is likely to come from studies which analyse the impact of these psychosocial factors on CAD outcomes, rather than intermediate outcomes. However it may also be informative in future to conduct a systematic review of studies which address factors related to CAD even if they do not directly measure it.

Secondly, generalisation of these data to human societies may not be warranted, and is against the advice of the primatologists conducting such studies. Given the pre-eminence of Robert Sapolsky's Serengeti baboons in the public health literature, it is probably appropriate to conclude with Sapolsky's own views on rank in monkeys, and its application to humans:

“It seems virtually meaningless to think about the physiological correlates of rank outside the context of a number of other modifiers… This dovetails nicely with the de-emphasis of rank in other niches of primatology …It leads to a final, somewhat obvious point – if we are endlessly struck with the complexity of these issues as they apply to non-human primates, the complexity expands exponentially when considering humans” [12].

Supporting Information

Figure S2.

Atherosclerosis in female monkeys after change in social status. Key: A: Dominant animals who stayed dominant ; B: dominants who became subordinate ; C: Subordinates who became dominant; D: Subordinates who stayed subordinate.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027939.s002

(TIF)

Table S1.

Studies of the effects of social stressors and/or social status on the development of CAD in non-human primates.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027939.s003

(DOC)

Table S2.

Examples of quotations from papers citing primate studies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027939.s004

(DOC)

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge the help of Sian Thomas, Linda McIntyre and Val Hamilton who helped in literature searching and sifting, data extraction to tables, and checking of study data.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MP GDS. Performed the experiments: MP GDS. Analyzed the data: MP GDS. Wrote the paper: MP GDS.

References

  1. 1. Davey Smith G, editor. (2003) Bristol: The Policy Press.
  2. 2. Pearce N, Davey Smith G (2003) Is social capital the key to inequalities in health? Am J Pub Health 93: 122–129.
  3. 3. Marmot M (2004) Status syndrome. London: Bloomsbury.
  4. 4. Wilkinson R, Pickett K (2009) The spirit level: why more equal societies almost always do better: Allen Lane.
  5. 5. Bobak M, Marmot M (1996) East-West mortality divide and its potential; explanations: proposed research agenda. Br Med J 312: 421–425.
  6. 6. Lynch J, Smith G, Hillemeier M, Shaw M, Raghunathan T, et al. (2001) Income inequality, the psychosocial environment, and health: comparisons of wealthy nations. Lancet 358: 194–200.
  7. 7. Wilkinson R (1999) Income inequality, social cohesion and health: clarifying the theory - a reply to Muntaner and Lynch. Int J Health Serv 29: 525–543.
  8. 8. Marmot M, Wilkinson R (2001) Psychosocial and material pathways in the relation between income and health: a response to Lynch, et al. Br Med J 322: 1233–1236.
  9. 9. Adler N, Ostrove J (1999) Socioeconomic status and health: what we know and what we don't. Ann NY Acad Sci 896: 3–15.
  10. 10. Wilkinson R (1997) Socioeconomic determinants of health: health inequalities: relative or absolute material standards? Br Med J 314: 591-.
  11. 11. Wilkinson R (1999) Putting the picture together: prosperity, redistribution, health and welfare. In: Marmot M, Wilkinson R, editors. Social determinants of health. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. p256–274.
  12. 12. Sapolsky R (1999) Hormonal correlates of personality and social contexts: from non-human to human primates. In: Panter-Brick C, Worthman C, editors. Hormones, health and behaviour. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  13. 13. Clarkson TB, Adams MR, Kaplan JR, Shively CA (1989) Pathophysiology of coronary artery atherosclerosis: Animal studies of gender differences. Cardiovascular Clinics 19: 147–158.
  14. 14. Kaplan J, Manuck S (1999) Status, stress, and atherosclerosis: The role of environment and individual behaviour. Ann NY Acad Sci 896: 145–161.
  15. 15. Sterne J, Egger M, Davey Smith G (2001) Investigating and dealing with publication and other biases. In: Egger M, Davey Smith G, Altman D, editors. Systematic reviews in health care. London: BMJ Publishing Group.
  16. 16. Cummins S, Macintyre S (2002) “Food deserts”: evidence and assumption in health policy making. Br Med J 325: 436–438.
  17. 17. Pound P, Ebrahim S, Sandercock P, Bracken M, Roberts I (2004) Where is the evidence that animal research benefits humans? Br Med J 328: 514–517.
  18. 18. Kaplan J, Manuck S, Adams M, Weingand K, Clarkson T (1987) Inhibition of coronary atherosclerosis by propranolol in behaviorally predisposed monkeys fed an atherogenic diet. Circulation 76: 1364–1372.
  19. 19. Kaplan J, Haiying C, Manuck S (2009) The relationship between social status and atherosclerosis in male and female monkeys as revealed by meta-analysis. Am J Primatol 71: 732–741.
  20. 20. Hamm T, Kaplan J, Clarkson T, Bullock B (1983) Effects of gender and social behavior on the development of coronary artery atherosclerosis in cynomolgus macaques. Atherosclerosis 48: 221–223.
  21. 21. Kaplan J, Adams M, Clarkson T, Koritnik D (1984) Psychosocial influences on female “protection” among cynomolgus macaques. Atherosclerosis 53: 283–295.
  22. 22. Kaplan J, Manuck M (2001) CHD: A hypothesis derived from observations on monkeys. Psychosom Med 63: 91–190.
  23. 23. Shively CA, Clarkson TB, Kaplan JR (1989) Social deprivation and coronary artery atherosclerosis in female cynomolgus monkeys. Atherosclerosis 77: 69–76.
  24. 24. Williams J, Shively C, Clarkson T (1994) Ischemia/Vasoconstriction/Infarction: determinants of coronary artery reactivity in premenopausal female cynomolgus monkeys with diet-induced atherosclerosis. Circulation 90(2): 983–987.
  25. 25. Clarkson TB, Shively CA, Morgan TM, Koritnik DR, Adams MR, et al. (1990) Oral contraceptives and coronary artery atherosclerosis of cynomolgus monkeys. Obstet Gynaecol 72: 217–222.
  26. 26. Adams MR, Kaplan JR, Clarkson TB, Koritnik DR (1985) Ovariectomy, social status, and atherosclerosis in cynomolgus monkeys. Arteriosclerosis 5: 192–200.
  27. 27. Kaplan J, Manuck S, Clarkson T, Lusso F, Taub D (1982) Social status, environment, and atherosclerosis in cynomolgus monkeys. Arteriosclerosis 2: 359–368.
  28. 28. Shively C, Clarkson T (1994) Social status and coronary artery atherosclerosis in female monkeys. Arterioscler Thromb 14: 721–726.
  29. 29. Kaplan JR, Manuck SB, Clarkson TB, Lusso FM, Taub DM, et al. (1983) Social stress and atherosclerosis in normocholesterolemic monkeys. Science 220: 733–735.
  30. 30. Manuck S, Kaplan J, Clarkson T (1983) Social instability and coronary artery atherosclerosis in cynomolgus monkeys. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 7: 485–491.
  31. 31. Kaplan J, Manuck S, Adams M, Williams J, Register T, et al. (1993) Plaque changes and arterial enlargement in atherosclerotic monkeys after manipulation of diet and social environment. Arterioscler Thromb 13: 254–263.
  32. 32. Williams JK, Vita JA, Manuck SB, Selwyn AP, Kaplan JR (1991) Psychosocial Factors Impair Vascular Responses of Coronary Arteries. Circulation 2146–2153.
  33. 33. Williams J, Kaplan J, Suparto I, Fox J, Manuck S (2003) Effects of exercise on cardiovascular outcomes in monkeys with risk factors for coronary heart disease. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol 23: 864–871.
  34. 34. Lapin B, Cherkovich G (1971) Environmental changes causing the development of neuroses and corticovisceral pathology in monkeys. In: Levi L, editor. Society, stress and disease. London: OUP.
  35. 35. Macleod J, Davey Smith G (2003) Psychosocial factors and public health: a suitable case for treatment? J Epidemiol Community Health 57: 565–570.
  36. 36. Evans R (2002) Interpreting and addressing inequalities in Health: from Black to Acheson to Blair to…? Office of Health Economics, London.
  37. 37. Nordstrom C, Dwyer K, Bairey Merz C, Shircore A, Dwyer J (2001) Work-related stress and early atherosclerosis. Epidemiol 12: 180–185.
  38. 38. Marmot M, Bosma H, Hemingway H, Brunner E, Stansfeld S (1997) Contribution of job control and other risk factors to social variations in coronary heart disease incidence. Lancet 350: 235–239.
  39. 39. Harris J (2006) Monkey business. In: Harris J, editor. No two alike: human nature and human individuality. London: W.W.Norton and Company.
  40. 40. Sharman M, Dunbar R (1982) Observer bias in selection of study group in baboon field studies. Primates 23: 567–573.
  41. 41. Mason W (1990) Premises, promises, and problems of primatology. Am J Primatol 22: 123–138.
  42. 42. Holst V (1998) The concept of stress and its relevance for animal behavior. Adv Stud Behav 27: 1–131.
  43. 43. Sapolsky RM (1993) Endocrinology Alfresco: Psychoendocrine Studies of Wild Baboons. Recent Prog Horm Res 48: 437–468.
  44. 44. Rohde P (2001) The relevance of hierarchies, territories, defeat for depression in humans: Hypotheses and clinical predictions. Affect Disord 65: 221–230.
  45. 45. Steptoe A, Marmot M (2002) The role of psychobiological pathways in socio-economic inequalities in cardiovascular disease risk. Eur Heart J 23: 13–25.
  46. 46. Abbott D, Keverne E, Bercovitch F, Shively C, Mendoza S, et al. (2003) Are subordinates always stressed? A comparative analysis of rank differences in cortisol levels among primates. Horm Behav 43: 67–82.
  47. 47. Gesquiere L, Learn NH, Simao CM, Onyango PO, Alberts SC, et al. (2011) Life at the top: Rank and stress in wild male baboons. Science 333: 357–360.
  48. 48. Sandercock P, Roberts I (2002) Systematic reviews of animal experiments. Lancet 360: 586.
  49. 49. Perel P, Roberts I, Sena E, Wheble P, Briscoe C, et al. (2006) Comparison of treatment effects between animal experiments and clinical trials: systematic review. Br Med J 334: 197.
  50. 50. Hamm T (1980) A nonhuman primate model of the effect of sex and social interaction on coronary artery atherosclerosis. Diss Abstr Int B41: 1319.
  51. 51. Kaplan J, Manuck S, Anthony M, Clarkson T (2002) Premenopausal social status and hormone exposure predict postmenopausal atherosclerosis in female monkeys. Obstet Gynaecol 99: 381–388.
  52. 52. Kaplan J, Adams M, Anthony M, Morgan T, Manuck S, et al. (1995) Dominant social status and contraceptive hormone treatment inhibit atherogenesis in premenopausal monkeys. Artherioscler Thromb Vasc Biol 15: 2094–2100.
  53. 53. Shively C, Kaplan J, Clarkson T (1990) Carotid artery atherosclerosis in cholesterol-fed female cynomolgus monkeys. Artheriosclerosis 10: 358–366.