Reader Comments

Post a new comment on this article

the aim of the study

Posted by MarkvP on 06 Jan 2007 at 23:26 GMT

Instead of moving towards an unbiased metric of selection for the most apt candidates for faculty positions and obtaining grants, the authors aim to define an index which is the ultimate bias. Research productivity, a criterion of selection suitable for the sciences, is being modified to include more females. Is there a need to adjust apparent appropriate indices reflecting scientific merit only to benefit an(y) underrepresented subpopulation?

I understand the need to exclude gender discrimination in the sciences, to allow everyone to pursue a scientific career as far as their individual capacities allow it. And I understand the perspective that when two candidates are equally suitable, the underrepresented (female) candidate may be preferred in order to compensate existing biases. This latter strategy obviously needs to include the notion that not all fields have equal ratios of male and female candidates (i.e., the student population). Therefore faculty staff positions need not always reflect 50/50 ratios, but instead the potential candidate gender ratios, which may differ from the ratios of the current student population for each respective discipline.

Nevertheless, adjusting selection criteria that reflect suitability in order to hire more women seems absurd to me. The authors end their discourse with “...until the career structure of science finds ways to assess females and males on a level playing field that takes into account the prevalent gender differences and imbalances ... we will ... fail to maximise our intellectual capital” (page 4). In this discussion I miss a sound objection against the criterion of research output as an appropriate factor for obtaining grants and jobs. If the sole objection against research output as a selection criterion is that females score lower, than an objective could be how we can stimulate this subpopulation to increase performance in such a way that will increase accessibility to grants and jobs!

The authors write on page 3 "Given that there are differences between males and females in the quantity, and potentially quality, of research output, how can we establish academic selection systems that do not discriminate on the grounds of gender?” This obstinate quest for an index that would favour an(y) underrepresented population, even if that means downplaying apparently appropriate selection criteria, seems counterproductive to me, indeed potentially reducing our intellectual capacity.

RE: the aim of the study

mresymonds replied to MarkvP on 28 Jan 2007 at 05:31 GMT

Thankyou for your comments.

Clearly the main issue for debate here is whether research productivity is a fair and impartial means of assessing research performance. If you assume it is, then you also implicitly assume that, on average, women show less aptitude and ability for science. This position is one with which we do not agree.

The first reason why we would not agree is due to the uncertainty about the causes of the difference in productivity (and why it occurs so early). There is certainly enough anecdotal evidence (see e.g. commentaries by Barres and Knapp cited in our article, and the references therein), to suggest that different demands are placed on men and women in science, which may result in decreased productivity in women. The 'sound objection' therefore is that the reduced research output by women may not be due to any failings on their part.

The second reason we disagree is that productivity is simply one aspect of research performance. To use an analogy, if a school were appointing a new sprint-running coach, would you select from the field of male and female candidates solely on the basis of which person could run the fastest? The capability to do this would seem relevant, and yet would obviously be biased towards a male candidate, as well as seeming to be only a (very small) part of what would make a good sprint-running coach.

The problem in science is that productivity is a particularly pervasive measure (viz. our finding that more productive scientists also receive more citations per paper). Hence our call to recognise that quality-based measures of research performance should not covertly actually reflect quantity (as the h-index does).

We do agree with your point that a worthwhile objective would be to encourage women not to fall behind men in terms of their research output, and recommend improvement and earlier application of mentoring (see page 2). But we see this as a long-term strategy and not one which will improve representation of women in science in the short-term.

Interestingly, the one situation you describe where you advocate selecting a female candidate over a male candidate of equal ability would to me seem like the greater case of discrimination, since selection would be based solely on the criterion of gender.

RE: RE: the aim of the study

MarkvP replied to mresymonds on 06 Mar 2007 at 21:17 GMT

Dear mresymonds,

I am sorry I did not reply sooner. but here it goes.

I think it is all about the question we ask, and how we answer that question: What scientists do we want to get tenure and grants? Is it the best, then we need to apply criteria to get the best scientists. If current criteria are insufficient to get the best scientists, then we need to address that insufficiency. But if we observe an under representation of any specific group or minority, that by itself need not automatically imply that the best researchers are not selected. And as such, there is, in my opinion, no need to adjust the parameter research productivity merely to include more people of a certain group.

That is also why I don’t understand your use of the word ‘impartial’ for research productivity.
(See above where you stated “Clearly the main issue for debate here is whether research productivity is a fair and impartial means of assessing research performance”)

Of course, I do acknowledge the importance of comparing the quality of research output between differently represented groups (as you did in your paper).


>>>Interestingly, the one situation you describe where you advocate selecting a female candidate over a male candidate of equal ability would to me seem like the greater case of discrimination, since selection would be based solely on the criterion of gender.

I can see how my statement can be misread. Please read my statement as: I understand the perspective of affirmative action. I do not advocate it, nor would I defend it. Affirmative action is to me just another type of discrimination.

RE: RE: RE: the aim of the study

mresymonds replied to MarkvP on 23 Mar 2007 at 02:11 GMT

Thanks again for your comments.

You are right to identify that the question we ask and how we answer is it is the crux of the matter. If productivity is the main benchmark of research excellence, as is the case at present, then of course funding/promotions are going to be judged on this criterion, regardless of gender of the applicant. I won't reiterate the reasons why this may be unfair to women. We undoubtedly want to reward the 'best' scientists, but my question is - what is the best measure of 'best'? We don't think it strange in science to consider productivity as the key indicator of performance. Yet in the arts it would be considered outrageous to award, say, government funding to a painter based on how many pictures he or she had produced in the last five years. We'd be much more concerned with the 'quality' of their work (I appreciate that this is a much more nebulous concept in the arts). Obviously there needs to be a base line of productivity - it would be foolish to fund someone who never produces anything - but the overwhelming importance of quantity of output in science (think how we are encouraged to divide our work into least publishable units) is I think problematic, particularly if some elements of the workforce face obstacles in achieving high productivity.

I agree that improving the standing of any under represented group should not be automatically done just because they are under represented. But it is important to understand WHY a group is under represented in order to assess whether something should be done.

My dictionary (Chambers' 20th Century) defines impartial as 'not favouring one more than another'. In the context of discussing the use of research productivity as a metric of research excellence, then it is appropriate since males are favoured by this measure.

RE: RE: the aim of the study

S-E-L replied to mresymonds on 03 May 2007 at 10:24 GMT

mresymonds,

Your analogy with a sprint coach is disingenuous. If the speed at which the coach could run was deemed to be an important factor, then it would be important to use their actual speed in deciding between applicants; not their speed divided by the length of their legs (because males are, on average, taller and therefore females are "disadvantaged" if we simply use speed as a measure of how fast someone can run). If, as you suggest, other factors need to be taken into consideration (e.g. how well the applicant can motivate someone), then they can be taken into account by other measures.

Similarly, if you want to measure research output, then you measure quantity and quality. It is not surprising that there is a correlation between these variables...if you're good, then it will be evident in a variety of ways (i.e. you do lots well), and it is these people that universities want to (and should) hire. The problem with the Research Status metric is that adjusts for the quantity of publications; this is not something that needs to be adjusted for, but an important metric in its own right. The H-index, for example, takes both into account.

To use another analogy, suppose you were a basket-ball coach selecting an Olympic team, and were using "ability" (defined as the number of points scored per game) as an important factor in deciding who will get selected. Since certain subgroups of the population are shorter than others (which is not their fault, and it doesn't mean that they are not as good...because that conclusion is simply not an option), would it be a good idea to develop a metric which divides the number of points scored by height (because there may be a correlation between height and points scored). You would end up with a number of short players being selected that score few points, but were "good for their height". And how do you think this team of residual stars will do when faced with competition from other countries that simply selected those players that scored the most points? And if these players from other countries happened to be taller than what would be predicted base on population norms, then so what?

Top universities want to select the best applicants (high actual scores), not those who have high residual scores!

RE: RE: RE: the aim of the study

mresymonds replied to S-E-L on 30 May 2007 at 02:25 GMT

S-E-L

Apologies for the delay in replying. Though my sprint coach analogy was a bit strained, I disagree with the assertion that it was disingenuous, actually because of the very same reasons that you state; i.e. what are deemed to be the important criteria for selection. My point was to call into question the heavy focus on one parameter (running speed in the analogy, quantity of publications for real) that may be neither important* nor impartial to assessing ability. Beyond the obvious point of not wanting to fund people who don't get anything achieved, why should quantity be valued so highly?

This question becomes even more pertinent if confronted by the possibility (we would argue likelihood) that a proportion of the scientific community face obstacles that prevent them from achieving high productivity (obstacles that are not related to their own ability).

It would be interesting to know exactly why productivity is correlated with quality of publications. How could one test whether it reflects overall research ability (the best scientists produce high quality science in large amounts), as you suggest, or whether people who publish lots tend to get cited more because their name is more prevalent (the ‘fast food effect’ suggested by Kelly and Jennions – ref 9 in our paper)?

If you assume the former, then by extension you are saying that those who are not as productive are in fact less able scientists. The point of our Research Status metric is to control for a variable that we do not want to measure, on the basis that it does not reflect ability and discriminates against women scientists (both points, I concede, with which some/many will disagree). Controlling in this manner (using residuals) for unintended variables is standard practice in allometry. I was going to extend the basketball analogy here, but I suspect it will start to cloud the issue, so I'll leave it at that.

* I should qualify the word important here. From the point of view of a university department deciding on a job appointment (for example) then it is difficult to criticise them for taking strongly into account the productivity of the candidate. Currently, that is the key indicator of their future funding success. Whether, though, at the funding body/research assessment level and more generally, productivity is an important measure of scientific ability, is debatable.