Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Educating the Brain to Avoid Dementia: Can Mental Exercise Prevent Alzheimer Disease?

  • Margaret Gatz

Educating the Brain to Avoid Dementia: Can Mental Exercise Prevent Alzheimer Disease?

  • Margaret Gatz
PLOS
x

Physicians now commonly advise older adults to engage in mentally stimulating activity as a way of reducing their risk of dementia. Indeed, the recommendation is often followed by the acknowledgment that evidence of benefit is still lacking, but “it can't hurt.” What could possibly be the problem with older adults spending their time doing crossword puzzles and anagrams, completing figural logic puzzles, or testing their reaction time on a computer? In certain respects, there is no problem. Patients will probably improve at the targeted skills, and may feel good—particularly if the activity is both challenging and successfully completed.

But can it hurt? Possibly. There are two ways that encouraging mental activity programs might do more harm than good. First, they may offer false hope. Second, individuals who do develop dementia might be blamed for their condition. When heavy smokers get lung cancer, they are sometimes seen as having contributed to their own fates. People with Alzheimer disease might similarly be viewed as having brought it on themselves through failure to exercise their brains.

What Does the Evidence Show?

Three types of evidence are cited to support the idea that mental exercise can improve one's chances of escaping Alzheimer disease.

Epidemiological studies

Having more years of education has been shown to be related to a lower prevalence of Alzheimer disease in cross-sectional, population-based studies [1] and to a lower incidence of Alzheimer disease in cohorts followed longitudinally [2]. Typically, the risk of Alzheimer disease is two to four times higher in those who have fewer years of education, as compared to those who have more years of education. Other epidemiological studies, albeit with less consistency, have suggested that those who engage in more leisure activities, especially activities that are mentally stimulating, have a lower prevalence and incidence of Alzheimer disease [3,4]. Additionally, longitudinal studies have found that older adults without dementia who participate in more intellectually challenging daily activities show less decline over time on various tests of cognitive performance [5].

thumbnail
How effective is mental exercise in holding back dementia?

(Illustration: Sapna Khandwala, Public Library of Science)

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0020007.g001

In epidemiological studies, people cannot be randomly assigned to different levels of education, or to different kinds and levels of participation in leisure activities. Consequently, researchers must try to identify confounders and take them into account analytically. However, uncertainties remain. Both education and leisure activities are imperfect measures of mental exercise. For instance, leisure activities represent a combination of influences. Not only is there mental activation, but there may also be broader health effects, including stress reduction and improved vascular health—both of which may contribute to reducing dementia risk [6]. It could also be that a third factor, such as intelligence, leads to greater levels of education (and more engagement in cognitively stimulating activities), and independently, to lower risk of dementia. Research in Scotland, for example, showed that IQ test scores at age 11 were predictive of future dementia risk [7].

Another problem with these epidemiological studies is that reverse causation could be involved—in other words, that incipient dementia could be causing reduced engagement in leisure activities, although some prospective studies have been particularly attentive to controlling for this possibility [8]. Clinical trials are needed to test the hypotheses that emerge from the best epidemiological research. Moreover, because the onset of Alzheimer disease can be hard to pinpoint, and early changes may occur years before the disease is diagnosed, conclusions must be based on large samples, followed over a long period of time.

Randomized clinical trials

Many studies support the possibility of enhancing memory and other cognitive performance, or of slowing cognitive decline in older adults without dementia [9]. The most effective programs teach mnemonic strategies, provide practice, and give supportive feedback. Mnemonic strategies include the organization of items into meaningful groups, the use of imagery, and the method of loci (visualizing items to be remembered in a sequence of specific, well-learned locations). Comprehensive programs can also include: encouraging memory aids (such as appointment books), teaching relaxation techniques, and providing instruction about memory changes in normal aging. However, improvements are not found in all studies. When improvements are found, they are often modest, may not be maintained over time, and do not generalize beyond the skill being trained. Often, the subjective gains rival the objective ones; for example, participants do tend to report fewer complaints about their memory.

These limitations are evident in one of the largest randomized controlled trials of cognitive training with older adults, a large, multisite study named ACTIVE (Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly) [10]. Participants were assigned to receive training in one of three cognitive skills: memory, reasoning, or speed of processing. Tests of cognitive abilities given immediately after training showed large improvements on the particular cognitive skill on which the individual had been trained, but no transfer to the other two cognitive domains. Additionally, for the control group that received no training, simply taking the test battery at pre-test led to improvement on the post-test. The effects of training were maintained over a two-year follow-up. However, the cognitive training program had no significant effect on measures of everyday functioning. Finally, for participants in ACTIVE or in other memory training programs, it remains unknown whether eventual rates of Alzheimer disease will be reduced.

Neurobiology studies

The third type of evidence suggesting that mental exercise may help to prevent Alzheimer disease comes from neurobiology studies that show greater brain complexity in those with higher levels of mental activity. Many such studies, done with animals, show greater neural complexity after having been exposed to an enriched environment that provides lots of stimulation, for example by including wheels, tunnels, toys, and gnawing sticks [11]. One human study with magnetic resonance spectroscopy showed changes in the hippocampus in elderly memory training participants compared to controls [12]. Another report found changes on positron emission tomography scanning following two weeks of a comprehensive memory program that included memory training, special diet, physical exercise, and stress reduction [13].

Mental Exercise and Cognitive Reserve

The concept of cognitive reserve is often used to explain why education and mental stimulation are beneficial. The term cognitive reserve is sometimes taken to refer directly to brain size or to synaptic density in the cortex. At other times, cognitive reserve is defined as the ability to compensate for acquired brain pathology. This definition encompasses coping skills as well as recruitment of other brain areas, with cognitive reserve thus accounting for individual differences in severity of cognitive dysfunction when there are pathological neural changes. People with a higher level of education have greater cognitive reserve. In some studies, education or occupation are even used as proxy measures of cognitive reserve, while others are beginning to measure neural substrates that correspond to reserve [14].

Taken together, the evidence is very suggestive that having greater cognitive reserve is related to a reduced risk of Alzheimer disease. But the evidence that mental exercise per se can increase cognitive reserve and stave off dementia is weaker. Epidemiological studies suggest that individual differences in cognitive reserve may actually be lifelong. In addition, people with greater cognitive reserve may choose mentally stimulating leisure activities and jobs, leading to a chicken-and-egg dilemma for the interpretation of the relationship between mentally stimulating activities in adulthood and dementia risk. Cognitive training has demonstrable effects on performance, on views of self, and on brain function—but the results are very specific to the skills that are trained, and it is as yet entirely unknown whether there is any effect on when or whether an individual develops Alzheimer disease. Further, the types of skills taught by practicing mental puzzles may be less helpful in everyday life than more prosaic “tricks,” such as concentrating, or taking notes, or putting objects in the same place each time so that they won't be lost.

Conclusion

So far, we have little evidence that mental practice will help prevent the development of dementia. We have better evidence that good brain health is multiply determined, that brain development early in life matters, and that genetic influences are of great importance in accounting for individual differences in cognitive reserve and in explaining who develops Alzheimer disease and who does not. At least half of the explanation for individual differences in susceptibility to Alzheimer disease is genetic, although the genes involved have not yet been completely discovered [15]. The balance of the explanation lies in environmental influences and behavioral health practices, alone or in interaction with genetic factors.

For older adults, health practices that could influence the brain include sound nutrition, sufficient sleep, stress management, treatment of mood or anxiety disorders, good vascular health, physical exercise, and avoidance of head trauma. But there is no convincing evidence that memory practice and other cognitively stimulating activities are sufficient to prevent Alzheimer disease; it is not just a case of “use it or lose it.”

Acknowledgments

The author is grateful to Hans-Olov Adami for helpful discussions, and for grant support from the National Institutes of Health (R01 AG08724, P30 AG17265, P50 AG05142) and from the Alzheimer's Association (ZEN-02-3895).

References

  1. 1. Ott A, Breteler MM, van Harskamp F, Claus JJ, van der Cammen TJ, et al. (1995) Prevalence of Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia: Association with education. The Rotterdam study. BMJ 310: 970–973.A. OttMM BretelerF. van HarskampJJ ClausTJ van der CammenPrevalence of Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia: Association with education. The Rotterdam study.BMJ1995310970973
  2. 2. Ott A, van Rossum CT, van Harskamp F, van de Mheen H, Hofman A, et al. (1999) Education and the incidence of dementia in a large population-based study: The Rotterdam Study. Neurology 52: 663–666.A. OttCT van RossumF. van HarskampH. van de MheenA. HofmanEducation and the incidence of dementia in a large population-based study: The Rotterdam Study.Neurology199952663666
  3. 3. Scarmeas N, Levy G, Tang MX, Manly J, Stern Y (2001) Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 57: 2236–2242.N. ScarmeasG. LevyMX TangJ. ManlyY. SternInfluence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer's disease.Neurology20015722362242
  4. 4. Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Bienias JL, Aggarwal NT, Mendes de Leon CF, et al. (2002) Cognitive activity and incident AD in a population-based sample of older persons. Neurology 59: 1910–1914.RS WilsonDA BennettJL BieniasNT AggarwalCF Mendes de LeonCognitive activity and incident AD in a population-based sample of older persons.Neurology20025919101914
  5. 5. Hultsch DF, Hertzog C, Small BJ, Dixon RA (1999) Use it or lose it: Engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychol Aging 14: 245–263.DF HultschC. HertzogBJ SmallRA DixonUse it or lose it: Engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging?Psychol Aging199914245263
  6. 6. Fratiglioni L, Paillard-Borg S, Winblad B (2004) An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurol 2004: 343–353.L. FratiglioniS. Paillard-BorgB. WinbladAn active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia.Lancet Neurol20042004343353
  7. 7. Whalley LJ, Starr JM, Athawes R, Hunter D, Pattie A, et al. (2000) Childhood mental ability and dementia. Neurology 55: 1455–1459.LJ WhalleyJM StarrR. AthawesD. HunterA. PattieChildhood mental ability and dementia.Neurology20005514551459
  8. 8. Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, Hall CB, Derby CA, et al. (2003) Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N Engl J Med 348: 2508–2516.J. VergheseRB LiptonMJ KatzCB HallCA DerbyLeisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly.N Engl J Med200334825082516
  9. 9. Mohs RC, Ashman TA, Jantzen K, Albert M, Brandt J, et al. (1998) A study of the efficacy of a comprehensive memory enhancement program in healthy elderly persons. Psychiatry Res 77: 183–195.RC MohsTA AshmanK. JantzenM. AlbertJ. BrandtA study of the efficacy of a comprehensive memory enhancement program in healthy elderly persons.Psychiatry Res199877183195
  10. 10. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, et al. (2002) Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 288: 2271–2281.K. BallDB BerchKF HelmersJB JobeMD LeveckEffects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: A randomized controlled trial.JAMA200228822712281
  11. 11. Diamond M (1988) Enriching heredity: The impact of the environment on the anatomy of the brain. London: Collier MacMillan. 191 p.M. DiamondEnriching heredity: The impact of the environment on the anatomy of the brain1988LondonCollier MacMillan
  12. 12. Valenzuela MJ, Jones M, Wen W, Rae C, Graham S, et al. (2003) Memory training alters hippocampal neurochemistry in health elderly. Neuroreport 14: 1333–1337.MJ ValenzuelaM. JonesW. WenC. RaeS. GrahamMemory training alters hippocampal neurochemistry in health elderly.Neuroreport20031413331337
  13. 13. Small G (2004) The memory prescription. New York: Hyperion. 368 p.G. SmallThe memory prescription2004New YorkHyperion
  14. 14. Stern Y (2002) What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 8: 448–460.Y. SternWhat is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept.J Int Neuropsychol Soc20028448460
  15. 15. Ashford JW, Mortimer JA (2002) Non-familial Alzheimer's disease is mainly due to genetic factors. J Alzheimers Dis 4: 169–177.JW AshfordJA MortimerNon-familial Alzheimer's disease is mainly due to genetic factors.J Alzheimers Dis20024169177