Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Suppression of Dopamine Neurons Mediates Reward

Abstract

Massive activation of dopamine neurons is critical for natural reward and drug abuse. In contrast, the significance of their spontaneous activity remains elusive. In Drosophila melanogaster, depolarization of the protocerebral anterior medial (PAM) cluster dopamine neurons en masse signals reward to the mushroom body (MB) and drives appetitive memory. Focusing on the functional heterogeneity of PAM cluster neurons, we identified that a single class of PAM neurons, PAM-γ3, mediates sugar reward by suppressing their own activity. PAM-γ3 is selectively required for appetitive olfactory learning, while activation of these neurons in turn induces aversive memory. Ongoing activity of PAM-γ3 gets suppressed upon sugar ingestion. Strikingly, transient inactivation of basal PAM-γ3 activity can substitute for reward and induces appetitive memory. Furthermore, we identified the satiety-signaling neuropeptide Allatostatin A (AstA) as a key mediator that conveys inhibitory input onto PAM-γ3. Our results suggest the significance of basal dopamine release in reward signaling and reveal a circuit mechanism for negative regulation.

Author Summary

Dopamine neurons in the midbrain of mammals fire action potentials in response to rewarding stimuli, while punitive stimuli or omission of reward suppress their activity. Different signs in the activity of dopamine neurons thus can encode appetitive and aversive values; however, how these bidirectional activities directly relate to behavior has yet to be elucidated. In fruit flies Drosophila, en masse activation of dopaminergic neurons in the protocerebral anterior medial (PAM) cluster has been shown to signal reward. Here, we demonstrate that a specific sub-class of these dopaminergic neurons, called PAM-γ3, mediates both aversive and appetitive reinforcement through activation and suppression of their activity, respectively. Notably, transient inactivation of the basal activity of PAM-γ3 neurons substitutes for reward and induces appetitive memory formation. Interestingly, we found that Allatostatin A, a neuropeptide that signals satiety, conveys inhibitory input onto PAM-γ3 neurons. Our results highlight the bidirectional activity of defined dopaminergic neurons, which underlies encoding of behaviorally relevant appetitive and aversive values.

Introduction

Dopamine plays a pivotal role in a wide range of motivation and learning [14]. In the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster, phasic neurotransmission from specific dopamine neuron subsets mediate the reinforcing properties of salient unconditioned stimuli in associative learning [514]. These dopamine neurons endow positive and negative predictive values to associated environmental cues, thereby modulating the fly’s subsequent response to the conditioned cues [5,6,1113]. In the fly brain, the protocerebral anterior medial (PAM) cluster of dopamine neurons conveys reward signals to the mushroom body (MB) [7,8]. While PAM cluster neurons exist from the larva, their cellular organization in the adult is much more complex and functionally heterogeneous [7,15,16]. Distinct dopamine neurons in the adult PAM cluster, for example, mediate appetitive and aversive reinforcement [57] and respond to sugar reward differently [7,12,17].

Besides phasic neurotransmission, recent studies revealed that valence-coding dopamine neurons have basal activity with fluctuating Ca2+ transient at the presynaptic terminals in the MB [1721]. Ongoing dopamine release has been shown to control state-dependent consolidation of associative memory [1821]. Considering the functional heterogeneity of the adult PAM cluster neurons [7,12,17], excitation and inhibition of dopamine neurons may signal appetitive and aversive values to drive bidirectional associative memories.

By characterizing PAM-γ3, a single class of dopamine neurons projecting to the γ3 region of the MB, we here show that sugar ingestion drives appetitive memory by suppressing the baseline activity of PAM-γ3. Furthermore, we searched for feeding-related signal molecules that inhibit PAM-γ3 and identified the neurons expressing the neuropeptide Allatostatin A (AstA). These results point to the importance of basal dopamine release and its negative regulation in reward processing.

Results

PAM cluster dopamine neurons are heterogeneous both morphologically and functionally and project to the distinct domains of the MB [7,12,17,22,23]. PAM-γ3 extends their dendritic arbor in the brain area surrounding the MB medial lobes (crepine) and projects specifically to the γ3 compartment of the MB (Fig 1A and 1B). While the majority of PAM neurons convey reward, previous studies implied that PAM-γ3 mediates aversive reinforcement [6,12]. However, additional GAL4 expression in other PAM neurons and nondopaminergic neurons of the driver line precluded identifying the responsible cells. We thus employed recently established split-GAL4 drivers MB441B-GAL4 and MB195B-GAL4 to specifically label 9 and 5 PAM-γ3 neurons, respectively [22] (Fig 1A).

thumbnail
Fig 1. PAM-γ3 neurons specifically innervate the γ3 domain of the MB.

(A) MB441B-GAL4 specifically labels PAM-γ3 in the brain. (B) Magnification of the inset in A. The γ lobe is visualized with n-Cadherin (gray). CRE: crepine. Scale bars, 20 μm.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.g001

To examine the role of PAM-γ3 neurons in learning, we activated them by directing the expression of dTrpA1, a temperature-sensitive cation channel [24], using MB441B-GAL4 and MB195B-GAL4. Simultaneous presentation of an odor with thermoactivation of the PAM-γ3 neurons induced robust conditioned avoidance of the paired odor (Fig 2A). Aversive memory established by thermoactivation of PAM-γ3 suggests their role in aversive reinforcement. As electric shock, compared to other aversive reinforcers, recruits the broadest set of dopamine neurons [6,10,14], we examined the requirement of the PAM-γ3 neurons in shock-induced aversive olfactory memory using targeted shibirets1 (shits1) expression, a temperature-sensitive dominant negative form of dynamin GFPase that inhibits vesicle endocytosis [25]. Surprisingly, thermal blockade of PAM-γ3 using MB441B and MB195B did not significantly impair electric shock–reinforced aversive memory (Fig 2B). The result was same with the Shits1 blockade by another driver, R58E02-GAL4, which strongly drives transgene expression in the majority of the PAM cluster neurons, including PAM-γ3 [7] (S1 Fig).

thumbnail
Fig 2. PAM-γ3 neurons are involved in acquisition of both aversive and appetitive memories.

(A) Thermoactivation with MB441B-GAL4 and MB195B-GAL4 paired with an odor induces aversive memory of the paired odor. LI, learning index; n = 7–10. (B) Blockade of the PAM-γ3 neurons in MB441B-GAL4/UAS-shits1 and MB195B-GAL4/UAS-shits1 flies leaves electric shock–reinforced immediate aversive memory intact. n = 8. (C) In contrast, the same blockade impairs sugar-rewarded immediate appetitive memory. n = 12–20. These data are reproduced with the same genotypes, but outcrossed with a wild-type strain for four generations (S2 Fig). (D) Transient blockade of PAM-γ3 during training impairs acquisition of appetitive memory. n = 23–35. (E) The performance of MB441B-GAL4/UAS-shits1 and MB195B-GAL4/UAS-shits1 flies is normal at a permissive temperature (24°C, 30 min memory). n = 8. (F) Blockade of PAM-γ3 with MB441B-GAL4/UAS-shits1 or MB195B-GAL4/UAS-shits1 does not impair innate sucrose preference. PI, performance index; n = 6–8. c, conditioning; t, test. Bars and error bars indicate means and standard error of the mean (SEM), respectively, throughout this study. The numerical data used in all figures are included in S1 Data. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01; ns, not significant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.g002

As multiple classes of adult PAM neurons contribute to reward signaling differently [7,12,17], we examined the requirement of PAM-γ3 for sugar-induced appetitive memory. Contrary to aversive memory induced by depolarizing the PAM-γ3 neurons, thermal blockade of PAM-γ3 with MB441B-GAL4/UAS-shits1 and MB195B-GAL4/UAS-shits1 flies significantly impaired appetitive memory (Fig 2C). Moreover, blocking PAM-γ3 only during the acquisition of appetitive memory revealed a similar impairment, suggesting the role of PAM-γ3 in reward processing (Fig 2D). Their memory performance at a permissive temperature was not significantly different from those of the genetic controls (Fig 2E). The blockade of PAM-γ3 did not impair innate sugar preference at the concentration used for the learning experiment or lower (Figs 2F and S3A). These results suggest the selective requirement of PAM-γ3 for mediating sugar reward.

As PAM-γ3 activation drives aversive memory [7], inhibition of the basal activity might be important for processing sugar reward. To examine this hypothesis, we imaged the Ca2+ response of PAM-γ3 by expressing GCaMP5 [26], a genetically encoded fluorescent calcium sensor, under the control of MB441B-GAL4. The baseline activity was fluctuating without stimulation (Fig 3A and 3B). Strikingly, sugar ingestion immediately silenced the baseline activity (Fig 3A, 3B and 3C). The Ca2+ level of PAM-γ3 neurons remained suppressed even after the ingestion (Fig 3B), which recovered to the baseline level approximately 20 s after the stimulus offset (Fig 3B). These data are compatible with the idea that PAM-γ3 neurons mediate appetitive reinforcement by acutely suppressing their baseline activity.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Sugar reward suppresses the baseline activity of PAM-γ3.

(A) Representative raw fluorescence in the PAM-γ3 neurons of the MB441B-GAL4/UAS-GCaMP5 fly with the mushroom bodies labelled with dsRed (orange). Two-photon images for one second before (baseline) and during the first second of sucrose stimulation (during sugar) are averaged and presented in a pseudocolor code. Region of interest is defined by terminals of PAM-γ3 in the medial lobes (outlined). Scale bar, 20 μm. (B) Time course of baseline activity (gray) and sugar response (red). Black bar: stimulus application. (C) Average calcium responses of PAM-γ3 neurons. Sucrose ingestion significantly reduces the activity level of PAM-γ3 (Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank test, n = 5). * p < 0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.g003

We hypothesized that transient inactivation of the PAM-γ3 dopamine neurons may be sufficient to signal appetitive reinforcement. Similar to the reinforcement substitution experiment with dTrpA1-mediated depolarization (Fig 1), we paired the Shits1 blockade of PAM-γ3 with one of the two odors; temperature was shifted only during the presentation of the conditioned odor (Fig 4A). This protocol is different from the former Shi ts1 experiments (Fig 2C and 2D), in which PAM-γ3 was blocked during both CS+ and CS-. The paired blockade of PAM-γ3 in MB441B-GAL4/UAS-shits1 flies indeed induced significant appetitive odor memory (Fig 4B). To confirm this appetitive memory, we established an optogenetic silencing approach by using engineered halorhodopsin (eNpHR) [27], a light-gated chloride ion pump. Transient blockade of the PAM-γ3 output by applying yellow light (591 nm) during the odor presentation resulted in an induction of appetitive memory (Fig 4C and 4D). We thus conclude that the suppression of PAM-γ3 baseline activity is sufficient to signal appetitive reinforcement.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Transient inactivation of PAM-γ3 mediates reward.

(A) Schematic diagram of reinforcement substitution experiment by transient inactivation with Shits1. (B) Paired suppression of PAM-γ3 activity using MB441B-GAL4 induces significant appetitive memory. n = 11–19. (C) Schematic diagram of reinforcement substitution experiment by light-induced inactivation of PAM-γ3 with eNpHR3.0. (D) Paired suppression of PAM-γ3 activity with eNpHR3.0 using MB441B-GAL4 induces significant appetitive memory. n = 8–11. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.g004

How does sugar ingestion suppress PAM-γ3 activity? Many neuropeptides are known to reflect feeding states and inhibit target cells through their receptors coupled with inhibitory G proteins [28]. We thus examined the expression patterns of a series of neuropeptide-related GAL4 drivers for their potential connection with the PAM-γ3 dendrites in silico [12]. Image registration of confocal stacks of the expression of neuropeptide GAL4 lines and MB441B-GAL4 into a standard brain revealed a spatial overlap between the processes of AstA and the PAM-γ3 neurons (Fig 5A and 5B). This putative connection was experimentally confirmed using reconstituted GFP signal between PAM-γ3 and AstA neurons by the GFP reconstitution across synaptic partners (GRASP) technique [29] (Fig 5C).

thumbnail
Fig 5. Allatostatin A neurons project to PAM-γ3.

(A) Expression patterns of AstA- (magenta) and MB441B-GAL4 (green) neurons aligned in a standard brain. Three brains for each line were examined, which essentially exhibited the same overlap pattern. (B) Magnification of (A) around the MB γ lobe. These processes overlap in the crepine (CRE; arrowheads). (C) Complementary GFP moieties are expressed in the AstA and PAM-γ3 neurons. The GRASP signals from six animals are averaged and pseudocolor-coded. Gray shades indicate the MB. Scale bar, 20 μm.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.g005

AstA was shown to signal satiation in Drosophila [30] and inhibits target neurons [31]. We therefore asked the involvement of the AstA neurons in mediating sugar reward. Thermoactivation of dTrpA1 with AstA-GAL4, which exclusively labels a subset of AstA immunopositive neurons [30], resulted in the formation of appetitive odor memory (Fig 6A). The blockade of AstA neurons during learning significantly lowered appetitive memory of both sucrose (Fig 6B) and nonnutritive sugar arabinose (Fig 6E). The sugar preference of AstA-GAL4/UAS-shits1 flies and their memory performance at a permissive temperature were unimpaired (Figs 6C and 6D and S3B). Thus, AstA-expressing neurons are necessary and sufficient for mediating the reinforcement property of sugar reward, likely sweetness.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Allatostatin A neurons mediate reward.

(A) Thermoactivation of AstA-expressing neurons using UAS-dTrpA1 induces immediate appetitive memory. n = 16. (B–C) Blockade of the AstA neurons with UAS-shits1 impairs immediate appetitive memory (B, n = 8) while leaving sucrose preference intact (C, n = 6–8). (D) Normal learning performance of AstA-GAL4/UAS-Shits1 flies at a permissive temperature. n = 6–8. (E) Blockade of the AstA neurons with UAS-shits1 during learning impairs immediate appetitive memory of nonnutritive sugar arabinose. n = 9–16. ns, not significant, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.g006

To confirm that the AstA protein is the underlying modulatory signal, we generated multiple null alleles of AstA using the CRISPR/Cas9 system. Appetitive memory of these mutant flies was significantly impaired (Fig 7A) while leaving their innate sugar preference unaffected (Fig 7B). Moreover, we also generated an RNA interference (RNAi) fly line against AstA based on the small hairpin RNA (shRNA) technique [32]. Down-regulation of AstA using AstA-GAL4 resulted in an impaired appetitive learning (Fig 7C) while leaving sugar preference intact (Fig 7D). Given that AstA is an inhibitory neuropeptide [31], these results suggest that the AstA release conveys sugar reward by inhibiting the PAM-γ3 dopamine neurons.

thumbnail
Fig 7. AstA protein is required for sugar learning but dispensable for innate sucrose preference.

(A) AstA mutant flies (AstASK1, AstASK4, and AstASK1/SK4) are defective in sugar memory performances. n = 10–32. (B) Sucrose preference of AstA mutant flies are indistinguishable from that of the wild-type strain. n = 8–16. (C-D) AstA knockdown using AstA-GAL4 impairs sugar memory (C, n = 12–16), while leaving sucrose preference intact (D, n = 8). ns, not significant, * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.g007

In order to visualize the distribution of an AstA receptor, we inserted the GAL4 transgene into the C-terminus of the Allatostatin A receptor 1 (DAR-1) coding region [33] by means of the CRISPR-Cas9 system. Confocal examination of DAR-1-GAL4 expression revealed positive labelling in the dopamine neurons projecting to the MB, including PAM-γ3 (Figs 8A and S4).

thumbnail
Fig 8. DAR-1 signaling is required for sugar-induced suppression of PAM-γ3 activity.

(A) DAR-1-GAL4 expression in PAM-γ3. The MB γ lobe is labelled in blue. Scale bar, 20 μm. (B) Averaged time course of the baseline activity of PAM-γ3 with (yellow, n = 6 animals) or without (black, n = 5) DAR-1 knockdown. Gray bar: 500 mM sucrose application for 10 s. (C) Averaged calcium responses of PAM-γ3 neurons. DAR-1 knockdown significantly attenuated sugar-induced suppression of PAM-γ3 activity (Mann–Whitney test, n = 5–6). * p < 0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.g008

If AstA/DAR-1 signaling also works in an inhibitory manner in the PAM-γ3, down-regulation of DAR-1 may weaken the PAM-γ3 suppression. To test this, we generated RNAi fly strains against DAR-1 based on the shRNA technique. Strikingly, knocking down DAR-1 in the PAM-γ3 significantly attenuated the suppression of the baseline activity (Fig 8B and 8C). Altogether, we propose that AstA provides an inhibitory signal to PAM-γ3 upon the ingestion of rewarding substances.

To examine the behavioral effect of AstA/DAR-1 signaling in the PAM-γ3, we down-regulated DAR-1 expression in the PAM-γ3 neurons and examined their appetitive memory. Consistent with our proposal, the knockdown significantly impaired sugar learning (Fig 9A and 9B) while leaving the innate sugar preference intact (Fig 9C and 9D).

thumbnail
Fig 9. Requirement of DAR-1 in the PAM-γ3 in sugar learning.

(A) Down-regulation of DAR-1 expression in PAM-γ3 by MB441B impairs immediate sugar memory. n = 12–15. (B) Knocking down DAR-1 in MB441B-GAL4 driven by second line of UAS-DAR-1-RNAi impairs sugar memory. n = 24–26. (C) Knocking-down DAR-1 in the PAM-γ3 driven by MB441B-GAL4 did not alter innate sugar preference. n = 9. (D) Knocking down DAR-1 in the MB441B-GAL4 driven by second line of UAS-DAR-1-RNAi did not alter innate sucrose preference. n = 8. ns, not significant. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.g009

AstA/DAR-1 signaling was shown to suppress neuronal activity in receiving cells through Gαi/o signaling [31,34]. To examine the intracellular mechanism of AstA/DAR-1 signaling in PAM-γ3, we inhibited the Gαo subunit by expressing the pertussis toxin with MB441-GAL4 [35]. As these flies had defective sugar memory (Fig 10A) but unimpaired sugar preference (Fig 10B), we suggest that DAR-1 inhibits PAM-γ3 activity by recruiting Gαo.

thumbnail
Fig 10. Requirement of Gαo signaling in PAM-γ3 for appetitive learning.

(A) Pertussis toxin expression in the PAM-γ3 impairs immediate sugar memory. n = 8. (B) Innate sugar preference is not significantly impaired. n = 8. ns, not significant, * p < 0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.g010

Discussion

Sugar ingestion triggers multiple reward signals in the fly brain [7,12]. We here provided lines of evidence that part of the reward is signaled by inactivating dopamine neurons (Figs 14). The role of PAM-γ3 highlights the striking functional heterogeneity of PAM cluster dopamine neurons. The decrease and increase of dopamine can convey reward to the adjacent compartments of the same MB lobe—γ3 and γ4—(Figs 3 and 4) [9,17]. The reward signal by the transient decrease of dopamine is in stark contrast to the widely acknowledged role of dopamine [36,37]. Midbrain dopamine neurons in mammals were shown to be suppressed upon the presentation of aversive stimuli [38] or the omission of an expected reward, implying valence coding by the bidirectional activity [39]. As depolarization of PAM-γ3 can signal aversive reinforcement (Fig 2), these neurons convey the opposite modulatory signals to the specific MB domain by the sign of their activity. Intriguingly, the presentation and cessation of electric shock act as punishment and reward, respectively [40]. Such bidirectional activity of PAM-γ3 may represent the presentation and omission of reward (Figs 14).

While thermoactivation of PAM-γ3 induced robust aversive memory, blocking their synaptic transmission did not affect shock learning, leaving a question regarding their role in endogenous aversive memory process. PAM-γ3 may only be involved in processing aversive reinforcement different from electric shock—like heat [10] or bitter taste [11,41]—or respond only to the omission of a reward as pointed above [40,42]. However, two studies show that dopamine neurons mediating aversive reinforcement of high temperature and bitter N,N-Diethyl-3-methylbenzamide (DEET) are part of those for electric shock. Identification of such aversive stimuli that are signaled by PAM-γ3 activation is certainly interesting, as it is perceived as the opposite of sugar reward and thus provides the whole picture of the valence spectrum. Another scenario where sufficiency and necessity do not match is the compensation of the reinforcing effect by other dopamine cell types (e.g. MB-M3 [6]). The lack of PAM-γ3 requirements for electric shock memory may be explained by a similar mechanism.

How can the suppression of PAM-γ3 modulate the downstream cell and drive appetitive memory? Optogenetic activation of the MB output neurons from the γ3 compartment induces approach behavior [43]. This suggests that the suppression of the PAM-γ3 neurons upon reward leads to local potentiation of Kenyon cell output. This model is supported by recent studies showing the depression of MB output synapses during associative learning [17,4446]. A likely molecular mechanism is the de-repression of inhibitory D2-like dopamine receptors, DD2R [47]. As D2R signaling is a widely conserved mechanism [48], it may be one of the most ancestral modes of neuromodulation.

Furthermore, recent anatomical and physiological studies demonstrated that different MB-projecting dopamine neurons are connected to each other and act in coordination to respond to sugar or shock [17, 43]. Therefore, memories induced by activation or inhibition of PAM-γ3 may well involve the activity of other dopamine cell types.

Our finding that appetitive reinforcement is encoded by both activation and suppression of dopamine neurons raises the question as to the complexity of reward processing circuits (Fig 11). It is, however, reasonable to implement a component like PAM-γ3 as a target of the satiety-signaling inhibitory neuropeptide AstA. Intriguingly, the visualization of AstA receptor distribution by DAR-1-GAL4 revealed expression in two types of MB-projecting dopamine neurons: PAM-γ3 and MB-MV1 (also named as PPL1- γ2α’1). Given the roles of MB-MV1 in aversive reinforcement and locomotion arrest [6,10,17,19], AstA/DAR-1 signaling may also inhibit a punishment pathway upon feeding. We thus speculate that this complex dopamine reward circuit may be configured to make use of bidirectional appetitive signals in the brain (Fig 11).

thumbnail
Fig 11. Reward signals by excitation and inhibition of dopamine neurons.

Schematic diagram of the reward circuits in the MB. Sugar ingestion activates multiple modulatory pathways, such as octopamine and AstA, that bidirectionally regulate distinct dopamine neurons. These dopamine neurons convey reward signals to the MB by their activation or inhibition and induce appetitive memory.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.g011

Materials and Methods

Flies

Canton-S was used as a wild-type strain. Generation and basic characterization of split-GAL4 drivers, w;MB441B-GAL4 and w;MB195B-GAL4, is described in [22]. w;;R58E02-GAL4 and w;;AstA-GAL4 are described in [7,30]. UAS-PTX16 is described in [35]. AstA mutants were generated as described previously [49]. Two null alleles, AstASK1 and AstASK4, in which the initiation methionine codon is deleted, were used throughout the present study. DAR-1-GAL4 was generated by CRISPR/Cas9-mediated targeted integration of a GAL4 transgene immediately in front of the stop codon of the DAR-1 gene. The GAL4 transgene contains a T2A peptide at the amino-terminus [50], such that GAL4 protein is synthesized from the same transcript as DAR-1 through the ribosomal skipping mechanism. The insertion was validated with genomic PCR and sequencing. Flies were raised at 24°C except for DAR-1 and AstA knockdown experiments. Thermoactivation experiments (Fig 1A) used the F1 progeny of the crosses between females of w;UAS-dTrpA1 [24] or w and males of the GAL4 drivers. Synaptic blockade experiments (Figs 2, 4, 6, S1, S2 and S3) used the F1 progeny of the crosses between females of w;;UAS-shits1 [51] or w and males of the GAL4 drivers. For the AstA (Fig 7) and DAR-1 knockdown experiments (Figs 8 and 9), we generated a UAS-AstA-RNAi and two UAS-DAR-1-RNAi transgenes in the Vailum20 vector [52]. The 21-bp, gene-specific sequence was: ACGCAGCGACTACGACTACGA (AstA), TCGGTCATTATTCAGATTATA (DAR-1), and CACGATAGGGATCTCTGTCAA (DAR-1), respectively. Females of those flies were crossed with males of the GAL4 drivers or w. The F1 progeny was raised at 24°C until 6 d after hatch, moved to 30°C, and kept for 1–14 d before experiments. For immunohistochemistry, a female reporter strain y w UAS-mCD8::GFP;UAS-mCD8::GFP;UAS-mCD8::GFP or w;58E02-LexA LexAop-mCD4::GFP11;UAS-mCD4::GFP1-10 was crossed to male GAL4 drivers, w;MB441B-GAL4, w;;AstA-GAL4, w;;DAR-1-GAL4. Flies used for whole-mount immunohistochemistry were aged to 5–10 d after eclosion. For Ca2+ imaging experiments, males of w; mb247-dsRed, UAS-GCaMP5; UAS-GCaMP5 [26] or w; UAS-V20-DAR-1-RNAi; UAS-GCaMP5 were crossed to w;MB441B-GAL4 females and raised at 24°C (Figs 3 and 8). For detailed fly genotypes used for behavioral experiments, see S1 Table.

Immunohistochemistry

Immunolabelling for the analysis of GAL4 lines was performed as previously described [57,12]. Either 97% TDE [53] or Vectashield (VECTASHIELD®, Vector) was used as mounding medium. The employed primary antibodies were the rabbit anti-GFP (1:1000; Invitrogen; A11122) or rat anti-N-cadherin (DN-EX #8; 1:100; Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank). The employed secondary antibodies were the cross-adsorbed secondary antibodies to IgG (H+L): AlexaFluor-488 goat anti-rabbit (1:1000; Invitrogen; A11034) or Cy3 goat anti-rat (1:200; Jackson Labs). Optical sections of whole-mount brains were sampled with a confocal microscope (Olympus FV1200). Confocal stacks were analyzed with the open-source software Image-J (National Institute of Health) and Fiji [54].

Image Registration

Landmark matching–based affine and nonrigid registration of whole brains was performed as previously described [12]. Confocal images of entire brains of GAL4/UAS-mCD8::GFP flies were scanned with n-cadherin (n-Cad) counterstaining and registered into the standardized brain by referring the n-Cad channel. The transformations computed with the n-Cad channel were then applied to the mCD8::GFP channel. The registered images were assigned into the standardized brain and represented as different colors using ImageJ.

Behavioral Assays

The conditioning and testing protocol was as described previously [7,12]. Briefly, for sugar learning and the US substitution experiment by dTrpA1-mediated thermoactivation, a group of approximately 50 flies in a training tube alternately received octan-3-ol (OCT; Merck) and 4-methylcyclohexanol (MCH; Sigma-Aldrich) for 1 min in a constant air stream with or without dried sucrose paper or 30°C heat. For the US substitution experiment by Shits1-mediated thermoinactivation (Fig 4), a group of approximately 50 flies in a training tube alternately received OCT and MCH for 2 min in a constant air stream with or without 33°C heat. For the US substitution experiment by eNpHR3-mediated light-inactivation (Fig 4), a group of approximately 50 flies were put into a custom-made LED-embedded aluminum tube and alternately received OCT and MCH for 1 min twice in a constant air stream with or without a continuous light exposure (591 nm). The light intensity was approximately100 mW/mm2 at a distance of 10 mm from the LED, measured with the Laser Power Meter Console (Thorlabs, PM100A). Flies were fed with all-trans-retinal contained food (2.5 mM) at least for 3 d before the experiments. OCT and MCH were diluted 10% in paraffin oil (Sigma-Aldrich) and placed in a cup with a diameter of 3 mm or 5 mm, respectively. After a given retention time, the conditioned response of the trained flies was measured with a choice between CS+ and CS- for 2 min in a T maze. The memories were tested immediately after training unless otherwise stated. The restrictive temperature for the experiments with UAS-shits1 was 33°C and the permissive temperature was 24°C, measured with the VC-960 digital multimeter (Voltcraft). For memory retention, trained flies were kept in a vial with moistened filter paper. After a given retention time, the trained flies were allowed to choose between MCH and OCT for 2 min in a T maze. A learning index was then calculated by taking the mean preference of the two reciprocally trained groups. Half of the trained groups received reinforcement together with the first presented odor and the other half with the second odor to cancel the effect of the order of reinforcement.

Statistics

Statistical analyses were performed with Prism5 (GraphPad). Most of the data did not violate the assumption of normal distribution and homogeneity of variance. Therefore, the data were analyzed with parametric statistics: one-way analysis of variance followed by the planned pairwise multiple comparisons (Bonferroni two-tailed test). Figs 2B, 2F, 6C, 6D, 7B, 9A, S1 and S2 were analyzed with nonparametric statistics: Kruskal–Wallis one-way analysis of variance followed by the planned pairwise multiple comparisons (Dunn’s test). The significance level of statistical tests was set to 0.05. For detailed results for statistical tests, see S1 Table. The numerical data used in all figures are included in S1 Data.

In Vivo Calcium Imaging

The expression of GCaMP5 [26] calcium reporter was targeted to PAM-γ3 neurons by crossing MB441B-GAL4 to mb247-dsRed, UAS-GCaMP5, or UAS-V20-DAR-1-RNAi; UAS-GCaMP5 flies. Flies that were 2 to 3 d old from the offspring were starved at 25°C for 24 h on a Kimwipe soaked with water. For DAR-1 knockdown experiments, flies were aged to 8–12 d after eclosion. Flies were then prepared for in vivo imaging by confocal microscopy as previously described [55]. Fluorescence was recorded in a transverse section of the brain. Recordings were made with a frame rate of 2 Hz in two animals, 10 Hz in two animals, and for the rest at 5 Hz, which did not alter the results. Each fly was presented with a droplet of 500 mM sucrose. The fly had access to the gustatory stimulus for 10 s. Image analysis was performed essentially as described previously [55]. Briefly, an object in each recording was stabilized by phase correlation–based image alignment using dsRed signal, then GCaMP5 signal was used as a fluorescent F value. In each animal, a region of interest in the left hemisphere was used. The baseline value of fluorescence Fmean was calculated as the average of ΔF/Fmean over 15 s before the start of the stimulation.

Supporting Information

S1 Fig. Neurotransmission from the PAM neurons is dispensable for shock learning.

Blockade of the PAM neurons in R58E02-GAL4/UAS-shits1 flies did not impair shock learning. n = 16. Results are means ± SEM. ns, not significant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.s001

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Shibire blockade with back-crossed MB441B impairs appetitive memory.

n = 8–10. Results are means ± SEM. * p < 0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.s002

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Low concentration sugar preference of MB441B-GAL4/UAS-shits1 and AstA-GAL4/UAS-shits1 flies.

(A) Blockade of the PAM-γ3 neurons in MB441B-GAL4/UAS-shits1 flies (A, n = 8) or AstA neurons in AstA-GAL4/UAS-shits1 flies (B, n = 8) did not impair 500 mM sucrose preference. Results are means ± SEM. ns, not significant.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.s003

(TIF)

S4 Fig. DAR-1 expression in the brain revealed by knockin DAR-1-GAL4.

Scale bar, 20 μm.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.s004

(TIF)

S1 Table. List of crosses and statistics for behavior experiments

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.s005

(DOCX)

S1 Data. Excel spreadsheet containing, in separate sheets, the underlying numerical data and statistical analysis for Fig panels 2A, 2B, 2C, 2D, 2E, 2F, 3C, 4B, 4D, 6A, 6B, 6C, 6D, 6E, 7A, 7B, 7C, 7D, 8C, 9A, 9B, 9C, 9D, 10A and 10B, S1, S2, S3A and S3B.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002586.s006

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We thank A. B. Friedrich and R. Sima for technical assistance and experiments that inspired this study; D. Yamamoto for generously sharing lab equipment; Y. Aso, G. Rubin, P. Garrity, D. Anderson, B. Pfeiffer, H. Kohsaka, G. Roman, and the Bloomington Stock Center for fly stocks; and T. Tabata, Y. Hiromi, and the members of the Tanimoto laboratory for discussion and/or critical reading of the manuscript.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: NY HT.
  2. Formal analysis: NY.
  3. Funding acquisition: NY HT MH.
  4. Investigation: NY MH AA SK.
  5. Methodology: NY.
  6. Project administration: NY HT.
  7. Resources: SK.
  8. Supervision: NY HT.
  9. Writing – original draft: NY HT.

References

  1. 1. Wise RA. Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(6):483–94. pmid:15152198
  2. 2. Bromberg-Martin ES, Matsumoto M, Hikosaka O. Dopamine in motivational control: rewarding, aversive, and alerting. Neuron. 2010;68(5):815–34. pmid:21144997
  3. 3. Waddell S. Reinforcement signalling in Drosophila; dopamine does it all after all. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2013;23(3):324–9. pmid:23391527
  4. 4. Krashes MJ, DasGupta S, Vreede A, White B, Armstrong JD, Waddell S. A neural circuit mechanism integrating motivational state with memory expression in Drosophila. Cell. 2009;139(2):416–27. pmid:19837040
  5. 5. Aso Y, Siwanowicz I, Bracker L, Ito K, Kitamoto T, Tanimoto H. Specific dopaminergic neurons for the formation of labile aversive memory. Current biology. 2010;20(16):1445–51. pmid:20637624
  6. 6. Aso Y, Herb A, Ogueta M, Siwanowicz I, Templier T, Friedrich AB, et al. Three dopamine pathways induce aversive odor memories with different stability. PLoS Genet. 2012;8(7):e1002768. pmid:22807684
  7. 7. Liu C, Placais PY, Yamagata N, Pfeiffer BD, Aso Y, Friedrich AB, et al. A subset of dopamine neurons signals reward for odour memory in Drosophila. Nature. 2012;488(7412):512–6. pmid:22810589
  8. 8. Burke CJ, Huetteroth W, Owald D, Perisse E, Krashes MJ, Das G, et al. Layered reward signalling through octopamine and dopamine in Drosophila. Nature. 2012;492(7429):433–7. pmid:23103875
  9. 9. Lin S, Owald D, Chandra V, Talbot C, Huetteroth W, Waddell S. Neural correlates of water reward in thirsty Drosophila. Nature neuroscience. 2014;17(11):1536–42. pmid:25262493
  10. 10. Galili DS, Dylla KV, Ludke A, Friedrich AB, Yamagata N, Wong JY, et al. Converging circuits mediate temperature and shock aversive olfactory conditioning in Drosophila. Current biology. 2014;24(15):1712–22. pmid:25042591
  11. 11. Masek P, Worden K, Aso Y, Rubin GM, Keene AC. A dopamine-modulated neural circuit regulating aversive taste memory in Drosophila. Current biology. 2015;25(11):1535–41. pmid:25981787
  12. 12. Yamagata N, Ichinose T, Aso Y, Plaçais P-Y, Friedrich AB, Sima RJ, et al. Distinct dopamine neurons mediate reward signals for short- and long-term memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2015;112(2):578–83.
  13. 13. Vogt K, Schnaitmann C, Dylla KV, Knapek S, Aso Y, Rubin GM, et al. Shared mushroom body circuits underlie visual and olfactory memories in Drosophila. Elife. 2014;3:e02395. pmid:25139953
  14. 14. Das G, Klappenbach M, Vrontou E, Perisse E, Clark CM, Burke CJ, et al. Drosophila learn opposing components of a compound food stimulus. Current biology. 2014;24(15):1723–30. pmid:25042590
  15. 15. Rohwedder A, Wenz Nana L, Stehle B, Huser A, Yamagata N, Zlatic M, et al. Four Individually Identified Paired Dopamine Neurons Signal Reward in Larval Drosophila. Current Biology. 2016; 26(5):661–9. pmid:26877086
  16. 16. Mao Z, Davis RL. Eight different types of dopaminergic neurons innervate the Drosophila mushroom body neuropil: anatomical and physiological heterogeneity. Frontiers in neural circuits. 2009;3:5. pmid:19597562
  17. 17. Cohn R, Morantte I, Ruta V. Coordinated and Compartmentalized Neuromodulation Shapes Sensory Processing in Drosophila. Cell. 2015; 163(7):1742–55. pmid:26687359
  18. 18. Berry JA, Cervantes-Sandoval I, Nicholas EP, Davis RL. Dopamine is required for learning and forgetting in Drosophila. Neuron. 2012;74(3):530–42. pmid:22578504
  19. 19. Berry JA, Cervantes-Sandoval I, Chakraborty M, Davis Ronald L. Sleep Facilitates Memory by Blocking Dopamine Neuron-Mediated Forgetting. Cell. 2015;161(7):1656–67. pmid:26073942
  20. 20. Placais PY, Trannoy S, Isabel G, Aso Y, Siwanowicz I, Belliart-Guerin G, et al. Slow oscillations in two pairs of dopaminergic neurons gate long-term memory formation in Drosophila. Nature neuroscience. 2012;15(4):592–9. pmid:22366756
  21. 21. Musso PY, Tchenio P, Preat T. Delayed dopamine signaling of energy level builds appetitive long-term memory in Drosophila. Cell Rep. 2015;10(7):1023–31. pmid:25704807
  22. 22. Aso Y, Hattori D, Yu Y, Johnston RM, Iyer NA, Ngo TT, et al. The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learning. Elife. 2014;3:e04577. pmid:25535793
  23. 23. Huetteroth W, Perisse E, Lin S, Klappenbach M, Burke C, Waddell S. Sweet taste and nutrient value subdivide rewarding dopaminergic neurons in Drosophila. Current biology. 2015;25(6):751–8. pmid:25728694
  24. 24. Hamada FN, Rosenzweig M, Kang K, Pulver SR, Ghezzi A, Jegla TJ, et al. An internal thermal sensor controlling temperature preference in Drosophila. Nature. 2008;454(7201):217–20. pmid:18548007
  25. 25. Kitamoto T. Conditional modification of behavior in Drosophila by targeted expression of a temperature-sensitive shibire allele in defined neurons. J Neurobiol. 2001;47(2):81–92. pmid:11291099
  26. 26. Akerboom J, Chen TW, Wardill TJ, Tian L, Marvin JS, Mutlu S, et al. Optimization of a GCaMP calcium indicator for neural activity imaging. The Journal of neuroscience. 2012;32(40):13819–40. pmid:23035093
  27. 27. Gradinaru V, Zhang F, Ramakrishnan C, Mattis J, Prakash R, Diester I, et al. Molecular and Cellular Approaches for Diversifying and Extending Optogenetics. Cell. 2010; 141(1):154–65. pmid:20303157
  28. 28. van den Pol Anthony N. Neuropeptide Transmission in Brain Circuits. Neuron. 2012;76(1):98–115. pmid:23040809
  29. 29. Feinberg EH, Vanhoven MK, Bendesky A, Wang G, Fetter RD, Shen K, et al. GFP Reconstitution Across Synaptic Partners (GRASP) defines cell contacts and synapses in living nervous systems. Neuron. 2008;57(3):353–63. pmid:18255029
  30. 30. Hergarden AC, Tayler TD, Anderson DJ. Allatostatin-A neurons inhibit feeding behavior in adult Drosophila. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012;109(10):3967–72. pmid:22345563
  31. 31. Lechner HA, Lein ES, Callaway EM. A genetic method for selective and quickly reversible silencing of Mammalian neurons. The Journal of neuroscience. 2002;22(13):5287–90. pmid:12097479
  32. 32. Ni JQ, Zhou R, Czech B, Liu LP, Holderbaum L, Yang-Zhou D, et al. A genome-scale shRNA resource for transgenic RNAi in Drosophila. Nature methods. 2011;8(5):405–7. pmid:21460824
  33. 33. Lenz C, Sondergaard L, Grimmelikhuijzen CJ. Molecular cloning and genomic organization of a novel receptor from Drosophila melanogaster structurally related to mammalian galanin receptors. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2000;269(1):91–6. pmid:10694483
  34. 34. Birgül N, Weise C, Kreienkamp HJ, Richter D. Reverse physiology in drosophila: identification of a novel allatostatin-like neuropeptide and its cognate receptor structurally related to the mammalian somatostatin/galanin/opioid receptor family. The EMBO Journal. 1999;18(21):5892–900. pmid:10545101
  35. 35. Ferris J, Ge H, Liu L, Roman G. G(o) signaling is required for Drosophila associative learning. Nature neuroscience. 2006;9(8):1036–40. pmid:16845387
  36. 36. Tsai H-C, Zhang F, Adamantidis A, Stuber GD, Bonci A, de Lecea L, et al. Phasic Firing in Dopaminergic Neurons Is Sufficient for Behavioral Conditioning. Science (New York, NY). 2009;324(5930):1080–4.
  37. 37. Mirenowicz J, Schultz W. Preferential activation of midbrain dopamine neurons by appetitive rather than aversive stimuli. Nature. 1996;379(6564):449–51. pmid:8559249
  38. 38. Danjo T, Yoshimi K, Funabiki K, Yawata S, Nakanishi S. Aversive behavior induced by optogenetic inactivation of ventral tegmental area dopamine neurons is mediated by dopamine D2 receptors in the nucleus accumbens. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2014;111(17):6455–60. pmid:24737889
  39. 39. Tobler PN, Dickinson A, Schultz W. Coding of predicted reward omission by dopamine neurons in a conditioned inhibition paradigm. The Journal of neuroscience. 2003;23(32):10402–10. pmid:14614099
  40. 40. Tanimoto H, Heisenberg M, Gerber B. Experimental psychology: event timing turns punishment to reward. Nature. 2004;430(7003):983. pmid:15329711
  41. 41. Kirkhart C, Scott K. Gustatory learning and processing in the Drosophila mushroom bodies. The Journal of neuroscience. 2015;35(15):5950–8. pmid:25878268
  42. 42. Aso Y, Rubin GM. Dopaminergic neurons write and update memories with cell-type-specific rules. Elife. 2016;5.
  43. 43. Aso Y, Sitaraman D, Ichinose T, Kaun KR, Vogt K, Belliart-Guerin G, et al. Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in Drosophila. Elife. 2014;4:e04580.
  44. 44. Sejourne J, Placais PY, Aso Y, Siwanowicz I, Trannoy S, Thoma V, et al. Mushroom body efferent neurons responsible for aversive olfactory memory retrieval in Drosophila. Nature neuroscience. 2011;14(7):903–10. pmid:21685917
  45. 45. Owald D, Felsenberg J, Talbot CB, Das G, Perisse E, Huetteroth W, et al. Activity of defined mushroom body output neurons underlies learned olfactory behavior in Drosophila. Neuron. 2015;86(2):417–27. pmid:25864636
  46. 46. Hige T, Turner G. Learning: the good, the bad, and the fly. Neuron. 2015;86(2):343–5. pmid:25905804
  47. 47. Hearn MG, Ren Y, McBride EW, Reveillaud I, Beinborn M, Kopin AS. A Drosophila dopamine 2-like receptor: Molecular characterization and identification of multiple alternatively spliced variants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2002;99(22):14554–9. pmid:12391323
  48. 48. Chung J-m, Spencer A. Dopamine acts through a D2-like receptor on a jellyfish motor neuron. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 1991;169(5):599–606.
  49. 49. Hentze JL, Carlsson MA, Kondo S, Nässel DR, Rewitz KF. The Neuropeptide Allatostatin A Regulates Metabolism and Feeding Decisions in Drosophila. Scientific Reports. 2015;5:11680. pmid:26123697
  50. 50. Diao F, White BH. A novel approach for directing transgene expression in Drosophila: T2A-Gal4 in-frame fusion. Genetics. 2012;190(3):1139–44. pmid:22209908
  51. 51. Pfeiffer BD, Truman JW, Rubin GM. Using translational enhancers to increase transgene expression in Drosophila. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109(17):6626–31.
  52. 52. Ni J-Q, Zhou R, Czech B, Liu L-P, Holderbaum L, Yang-Zhou D, et al. A genome-scale shRNA resource for transgenic RNAi in Drosophila. Nat Meth. 2011;8(5):405–7.
  53. 53. Staudt T, Lang MC, Medda R, Engelhardt J, Hell SW. 2,2'-thiodiethanol: a new water soluble mounting medium for high resolution optical microscopy. Microsc Res Tech. 2007;70(1):1–9. pmid:17131355
  54. 54. Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Frise E, Kaynig V, Longair M, Pietzsch T, et al. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat Meth. 2012;9(7):676–82.
  55. 55. Hiroi M, Ohkura M, Nakai J, Masuda N, Hashimoto K, Inoue K, et al. Principal component analysis of odor coding at the level of third-order olfactory neurons in Drosophila. Genes Cells. 2013;18(12):1070–81. pmid:24118654