Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

PER-TIM Interactions with the Photoreceptor Cryptochrome Mediate Circadian Temperature Responses in Drosophila

  • Rachna Kaushik,

    ¤ Current address: Department of Biological Sciences, Columbia University, New York, New York, United States of America

    Affiliations Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts, United States of America , National Center for Behavioral Genomics, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts, United States of America , Department of Biology, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Pipat Nawathean,

    Affiliations Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts, United States of America , National Center for Behavioral Genomics, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts, United States of America , Department of Biology, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Ania Busza,

    Affiliations University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, Massachusetts, United States of America , Program in Neuroscience, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, Massachusetts, United States of America , MD/PhD Program, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Alejandro Murad,

    Affiliations University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, Massachusetts, United States of America , Program in Neuroscience, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Patrick Emery,

    Affiliations University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, Massachusetts, United States of America , Program in Neuroscience, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, Massachusetts, United States of America

  • Michael Rosbash

    To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: rosbash@brandeis.edu

    Affiliations Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts, United States of America , National Center for Behavioral Genomics, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts, United States of America , Department of Biology, Brandeis University, Waltham, Massachusetts, United States of America

Retraction

Our original paper strongly implicated the photoreceptor cryptochrome (CRY) in the response of the Drosophila circadian system to temperature. The claim was based on three findings:

  1. CRY formed a complex with the two key clock proteins Period (PER) and Timeless (TIM) not only in response to a light pulse but also in response to a 37°C heat pulse.
  2. A mutant strain missing wild-type CRY did not phase shift in response to a heat pulse, implicating CRY in heat-mediated phase shifts in addition to its well-known importance for light-mediated phase shifts. This mutant phenotype was rescued by expressing wild-type CRY within clock neurons.
  3. The absence of CRY inhibited the perL effect on another temperature-centric feature of rhythms, temperature compensation of circadian period. The period of perL becomes longer at elevated temperatures, whereas it is essentially unaffected in wild-type flies. We reported that a double mutant strain, expressing PERL and also missing wild-type CRY, recovered normal temperature compensation. In other words, CRY was necessary for PERL to affect temperature compensation, consistent with a PER-CRY interaction.

We were contacted some months ago by Mike Young and colleagues at The Rockefeller University, who could not repeat the second finding. We subsequently repeated this experiment ourselves and also observed what Young and colleagues told us, i.e., we could not replicate the original observation that implicated CRY in heat-mediated phase shifts. As we no longer had the double mutant strain necessary to repeat the third finding (restoration of proper temperature compensation in a double mutant strain containing the mutant perL chromosome and missing wild-type CRY), we constructed a very similar strain and assayed these perL; cry0 flies for temperature compensation. It was aberrant, essentially indistinguishable from single mutant perL flies. We have therefore failed to reproduce two of the three findings in the original paper.

Although we have absolutely no explanation for the discrepancies with the original results and despite the lack of an attempt to reproduce the first finding (the heat-mediated formation of a CRY-PER-TIM interaction), the failure to reproduce two of the three findings in the original paper compels us to retract the paper. We deeply regret any inconvenience that this has caused the scientific community.

25 Feb 2016: Kaushik R, Nawathean P, Busza A, Murad A, Emery P, et al. (2016) Retraction: PER-TIM Interactions with the Photoreceptor Cryptochrome Mediate Circadian Temperature Responses in Drosophila. PLOS Biology 14(2): e1002403. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002403 View retraction

Abstract

Drosophila cryptochrome (CRY) is a key circadian photoreceptor that interacts with the period and timeless proteins (PER and TIM) in a light-dependent manner. We show here that a heat pulse also mediates this interaction, and heat-induced phase shifts are severely reduced in the cryptochrome loss-of-function mutant cryb. The period mutant perL manifests a comparable CRY dependence and dramatically enhanced temperature sensitivity of biochemical interactions and behavioral phase shifting. Remarkably, CRY is also critical for most of the abnormal temperature compensation of perL flies, because a perL; cryb strain manifests nearly normal temperature compensation. Finally, light and temperature act together to affect rhythms in wild-type flies. The results indicate a role for CRY in circadian temperature as well as light regulation and suggest that these two features of the external 24-h cycle normally act together to dictate circadian phase.

Author Summary

Circadian rhythms profoundly affect the physiology and behavior of most organisms. These rhythms are generated by a self-sustained molecular clock, which is largely conserved between fruit flies and mammals and synchronizes to the day/night cycle. This synchronization is achieved in most organisms by a daily resetting caused by light and/or temperature fluctuations. The molecular mechanisms underlying light synchronization are reasonably well understood, but an understanding of how temperature affects the circadian clock is lacking. This study demonstrates a striking and unanticipated relationship between light and temperature resetting mechanisms in Drosophila. An interaction between the circadian photoreceptor CRYPTOCHROME (CRY) and a complex composed of the key circadian regulators PERIOD (PER) and TIMELESS (TIM) are critical for circadian temperature responses as well a circadian light responses. Moreover, the data not only indicate that light and temperature reset the clock through similar mechanisms but also that these two inputs can act synergistically. An interaction between light and temperature may fine-tune the dawn and dusk response of the clock and even contribute to seasonal adaptation of clock function, an emerging area of research in circadian biology.

Introduction

Most organisms have circadian rhythms of gene expression and behavior that are controlled by endogenous clocks. A few studies have verified that these systems increase fitness and help organisms adapt to the physical and ecological environment in which they live [1]. At the molecular level, the central pacemaker of animals is proposed to consist of auto-regulatory feedback loops that regulate the expression of key clock genes [2]. An admittedly simplified view of the Drosophila central clock posits a core system of four interacting regulatory proteins. A circadian cycle begins when a CLOCK (CLK) and CYCLE (CYC) heterodimer activates the expression of two other proteins, PERIOD (PER) and TIMELESS (TIM). PER and TIM levels slowly accumulate over time, and these two proteins also heterodimerize. At some point, PER-TIM complexes enter the nucleus and inactivate CLOCK-CYCLE activity, slowing their own production and signaling the end of a cycle. Importantly, kinases and phosphatases modify PER, TIM, and CLK and play critical roles in circadian rhythms [37].

Endogenous periods are usually different from the precise 24-h rotation of Earth. Nonetheless, circadian clocks keep precise 24-h time under normal conditions and are reset every day by environmental signals like light and temperature, which are the dominant entraining cues in nature. In Drosophila, circadian light perception is well-understood, and a major fraction of it is mediated by the circadian photoreceptor molecule cryptochrome (CRY) [2,8]. Cryptochromes are related to photolyases, a family of blue-light–sensitive DNA repair enzymes, and also play important roles in photoreception and circadian rhythms of other animals as well as plants [9,10].

Drosophila CRY is prominently expressed in pacemaker neurons [1113]. Moreover, a mutant cry strain (cryb) manifests severe molecular and behavioral problems. These include a lack of PER and TIM molecular cycling in peripheral tissues under light-dark cycles and an inability to undergo phase resetting in response to short light pulses [14]. cryb flies are also rhythmic in constant light, i.e., the characteristic arrhythmicity of Drosophila and many other animals in constant light is absent [15]. Finally, there is strong evidence that CRY contributes to standard entrainment by light-dark cycles [16].

At the biochemical level, photon capture by CRY leads to an interaction with TIM or with the PER-TIM complex [1720]. CRY also interacts with and blocks the function of the PER-TIM complex in a light-dependent manner in an S2 cell-based assay [17]. The current view is that the CRY:TIM interaction leads to TIM degradation, which results in phase-resetting in response to a light pulse [2125].

In addition to light, other factors such as social interactions, activity, and especially temperature can modulate free-running rhythms. Indeed, temperature is generally regarded as secondary only to light as an entrainment cue [26]. Circadian clocks can be highly sensitive to temperature changes; e.g., clocks can be entrained by a regular temperature cycle that oscillates by only 1–2 degrees in some insects, lizards, and vertebrates [2729]. It has also been shown that temperature cycles induce synchronized behavioral rhythms and oscillations of the clock proteins PER and TIM in constant light, a situation that normally leads to molecular and behavioral arrhythmicity [30]. Also relevant to the relationship between temperature and circadian rhythms is temperature compensation: the free-running period in many different circadian systems, including Drosophila, is generally insensitive to alterations in (constant) incubation temperature, i.e., Q10 (the relative rate enhancement corresponding to a 10 °C rise in temperature) ≈ 1.0 [31]. It is believed that temperature compensation is integral to circadian clock function and critical to maintaining dependable time keeping despite fluctuations in ambient temperature.

We found a surprising relationship between the response of the Drosophila clock to heat pulses and light pulses as well as between heat pulses and temperature compensation; the connector is the photoreceptor CRY. The heat-induced phase delays that take place in a wild-type strain are paralleled by a physical interaction between CRY and PER-TIM. In perL mutant flies, heat-phase shifts are more robust and occur at lower temperatures, which are mirrored by parallel CRY:PERL-TIM interactions. perL phase shifts are also severely reduced by the addition of cryb to the genetic background. Remarkably, these perL; cryb double-mutant flies have largely restored temperature compensation. The results indicate that a more potent interaction between CRY and PERL-TIM causes most of the temperature compensation defects of perL as well as the more robust heat-mediated phase shifts of these mutant flies.

Results

Heat Pulse–Mediated Phase Delays of Wild-Type Flies Require 37 °C

To investigate the effect of heat on Drosophila locomotor activity rhythms, we first compared a heat phase response curve (PRC) to a standard light PRC. In both cases, the pulses lasted for 30 min, either with saturating light or with a shift from 25 °C to 37 °C. We used a modified PRC protocol, called the anchored PRC (APRC; [3234]: the pulses are applied to wild-type flies during the night half of a light-dark cycle (zeitgeber time [ZT]12–24) and then during the first 12 h of the subsequent “day” in constant darkness (circadian time [CT]0–12). Locomotor activity phases were then measured after several subsequent days in constant darkness.

A typical PRC was obtained for light, with maximum phase delays of about 3.5 h in the early night and maximum phase advances of about 2.5 h in the late night. For heat, early night delays were more modest, about 2.5 h, whereas late night advances were very small or absent (Figures 1A and S1). The data are essentially indistinguishable from the only published examples of 37 °C heat pulse–mediated phase shifts in Drosophila [35,36]. Moreover, there was little or no behavioral phase shift after 30 °C or 34 °C heat pulses (Figure 1A) [36].

thumbnail
Figure 1. Heat Pulse–Mediated Phase Delays of Wild-Type Flies Require 37 °C

Phase response curves for CS flies after heat (circle) versus light pulse (square). Flies were entrained for 3 d in 12 h:12 h LD cycles and pulsed for 30 min of light and 30 °C, 34 °C, and 37 °C heat pulse (HP) during the last night of the LD entrainment cycle, after which the flies were released in constant darkness for 5 d. Phase changes were calculated by comparing behavioral offsets of light or HP treated flies 3 d after the pulse to the behavior of the control group of the same genotype that did not receive a pulse. The calculations were made by MATLAB software using previously described methods [46]. Phase delays and phase advances are plotted (± SEM) as negative and positive values respectively. In all cases, the experiments were repeated at least twice with similar results. Data were pooled from the following number of flies (each pair of values referring to wild-type light-pulsed and wild-type 37 °C heat-pulsed): control: 32, 32; pulse at ZT12: 32, 22; pulse at ZT15: 32, 26; pulse at ZT18: 23, 26; pulse at ZT21: 24, 19; and pulse at ZT24: 29, 27.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050146.g001

CRY:PER-TIM Heat-Dependent Interactions Parallel the Behavioral Responses

The weak heat-mediated delay and absence of a substantial advance makes it uncertain whether there is a relationship between the heat and light PRCs. We therefore assayed the biochemical effects of a heat pulse and compared them to those of a light pulse. The strategy was based on the interaction of CRY with TIM and/or PER, which is a light-dependent event (e.g., [20]). There is also substantial evidence that these events are crucial to clock resetting after short light pulses [19,37,38]. To assay CRY interactions in flies, we used a previously described strain that expresses N-terminal MYC-tagged CRY [20] . We subjected flies to either light or heat pulses and then assayed CRY complexes via immunoprecipitation with anti-MYC anti-sera.

Remarkably, an interaction between CRY and PER-TIM was observed at ZT15 after a 37 °C heat pulse as well as after a light pulse. There was no detectable interaction if the ZT15 heat pulse was at 30 °C (Figure 2A), nor was there a robust 37 °C heat-mediated interaction at ZT21, despite a canonical light-mediated interaction at this time (Figure 2B). These results mirror the behavioral observations, namely, a 37 °C phase shift and no 30 °C phase shift at ZT15, with no 37 °C phase shift at ZT21 (Figure 1) [35]. The data indicate that a CRY:PER-TIM interaction correlates with heat-mediated phase shifts and suggest that it might underlie the behavioral phase shifts.

thumbnail
Figure 2. CRY Interacts with PER/TIM in a 37 °C Heat Pulse–Dependent Manner at ZT15 but Not at ZT21

Heat- and light-dependent interactions among CRY, TIM, and PER were measured three times with similar results. (A) TMC flies (Myc-CRY) were subjected to standard 12:12 LD conditions referred as control, pulsed with 37 °C (2), pulsed with 30 °C (3), light-pulsed (4), or not (1) for 30 min at ZT15, collected, and frozen. Head extracts (HE) were immunoprecipitated with antibody to MYC (IP), all as previously described [20]. CRY, PER, and TIM levels were measured by Western blotting. (B) Exactly as above but pulses were at ZT21.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050146.g002

CRY Is Required for Heat-Mediated Phase Shifts

These results predict that heat PRCs should be affected in the severe loss-of-function mutant cryb. Indeed, these flies show little to no response to a heat pulse, i.e., an almost flat PRC (Figure 3A). The results are very similar to those observed for a light PRC in cryb [14].

thumbnail
Figure 3. 37 °C Heat Pulse–Mediated Phase Responses of cryb and Rescued Strains

(A) Phase response curves for wild-type (circle) flies and cryb (square) mutant flies. The experiment was performed as described in Figure 1. Phase delays and phase advances are plotted (± SEM) as negative and positive values, respectively. Data were pooled from the following number of flies (each pair of values referring to wild-type heat pulsed and cryb heat pulsed): control: 32, 32; pulse at ZT12: 22, 39; pulse at ZT15: 26, 40; pulse at ZT18: 26, 39; pulse at ZT21: 19, 36; and pulse at ZT24: 27, 15.

(B and C). Bottom left and right panels show the phase changes observed at ZT15 (B) and ZT21(C), respectively. On the x-axis, the Zeitgeber 37 °C heat (HP) or light pulse (LP) is indicated. Phase delays and advances are described in Figure 1 and plotted on the y-axis (± SEM) as negative and positive values, respectively. The genotype of the flies is indicated on the x-axes: the first row shows the transgenes present (plus sign corresponds to a chromosome without a transgene), whereas the second row indicates the genetic background (wild-type [WT] or cryb).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050146.g003

To verify that this result is not due to a strain differences unrelated to the cry locus, we rescued the cryb mutation by expressing CRY in clock-pacemaker cells using pdf-GAL4 [13,39,40]. pdf-GAL4–mediated CRY expression partially rescued the cryb heat delay at ZT15 (Figure 3B) as well as the cryb light delay as previously described [13]. We also compared the response of these strains to heat and light pulses at ZT21 (Figure 3C). As predicted from the wild-type heat PRC pattern (Figure 3A), the addition of pdf-GAL4–mediated CRY expression to the cryb background had no effect on the essentially nonexistent heat-phase shift at ZT21, whereas it rescued the cryb ZT21 light-phase shift (Figure 3C) [13]. In contrast, tim-GAL4–mediated CRY-B expression was unable to rescue either light- or heat-mediated cryb phase shifts (Figure S2), consistent with the strong hypomorphic cryb mutation. Taken together with the heat-mediated physical interaction between CRY and PER-TIM (Figure 2), the results indicate that CRY is important for circadian clock heat responses as well as light responses.

perL Flies Are Hypersensitive to Heat

The perL genotype shows aberrant temperature compensation, with dramatically increased periods at elevated constant temperatures [41,42]. We speculated that this phenomenon might be related to heat-pulse responses and even light pulse–mediated phase shifts. To examine this possibility, we first assayed a standard light PRC of perL flies. It is very similar to that for wild-type flies, except that the perL curve is delayed by several hours (Figure 4A) [34]. There are essentially indistinguishable phase delays 18 h after the last DL (dark-light) transition for perL flies and 15 h after the last DL transition for wild-type flies. Moreover, there are similar phase advances, about 26 h after the last DL transition in perL and 21 h after the last DL transition in wild-type (compare Figure 4A with Figure 1).

thumbnail
Figure 4. perL Is Hypersensitive to Heat Pulses

Phase response curves are shown for perL flies, after a heat pulse at 37 °C (square), after a 30 °C heat pulse (diamond) or after a light pulse (circle). The experiment was performed as described in Figure 1A and repeated twice with similar results. Phase delays and phase advances are plotted (+/- SEM) as negative and positive values, respectively. Data were pooled from the following number of flies (each set of values referring to perL light pulsed, heat pulsed at 30 °C or 37 °C): control: 32, 32, 32; pulse at ZT18: 26, 32, 29; pulse at ZT21: 19, 32, 31; pulse at ZT24: 27, 31, 27; pulse at CT02: 30, 31, 22; pulse at CT04: 32, 29, 21. (B) perL; TMC flies (see above) were entrained to 12:12 light:dark conditions and heat pulsed at 37 °C (2) heat pulsed at 30 °C (3), light pulsed (LP) (4) or not (1) for 30 min all at CT02, collected, and frozen. CRY, PER and TIM levels were measured by Western blotting after anti-MYC immunoprecipitation (IP) from head extracts (HE). Heat- and light-dependent interactions among CRY, TIM, and PER were assayed three times with essentially indistinguishable results.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050146.g004

Consistent with the notion that perL flies are more heat sensitive than wild-type flies, there is essentially no difference between the perL heat and light PRCs in the delay zone (Figure 4A), in contrast to the magnitude of the wild-type heat-mediated delay, which is clearly less than that of the wild-type light-mediated delay (Figure 1) [35]. Even more impressive is the heat-mediated advance for perL flies, which is indistinguishable from the light-mediated maximal advance (Figure 4A); there is little or no heat-mediated advance in wild-type flies (Figure 1). Finally, perL flies are sensitive to a 30 °C heat pulse, whereas wild-type flies are insensitive even to a 34 °C pulse (Figures 1 and 4B) [36].

The heat-mediated phase advance of perL flies suggested that there might be an interaction between CRY and PERL-TIM at these times, e.g., at CT2 (ZT26 = CT2). Indeed, we confirmed such an interaction after a 30 °C as well as a 37 °C heat pulse (Figure 4B). With minor differences, the interaction was similar to that elicited by a light pulse at this same time, and no interaction was observed without a heat or a light pulse (Figure 4B). There is no detectable heat-mediated interaction between CRY and wild-type PER-TIM in the advance zone or at 30 °C (Figures 1 and 2), i.e., the interactions between CRY and PERL-TIM correlate well with the behavioral observations (Figure 4A) and further indicate that they are important for the observed heat-mediated phase shifts.

perL Heat-Mediated Phase Shifts, CRY, and Temperature Compensation

To verify that the CRY:PERL-TIM interaction is functionally relevant, we generated perL; cryb double mutant flies. They have a long free running period of ~28 h, characteristic of perL, and are rhythmic in light-light (LL), characteristic of cryb (Figure 5A). These flies also show much smaller phase shifts in response to 37 °C heat pulses in the delay zone at ZT18 as well as in the advance zone at ZT26 (ZT26 = CT2; Figure 5B). The exaggerated perL heat-mediated phase shifts are therefore CRY dependent.

thumbnail
Figure 5. perL; cryb Flies Show Reduced Heat Phase Shifts and Better Temperature Compensation

(A) Behavior in DD and LL cycles was monitored for per L and per L; cryb flies. The data were collected at a constant temperature of 25 °C. The average activity plots for 16 flies are shown. Adult male flies of 1–3 d old were entrained for 3 d (12 h: 12 h LD) and released in DD (as indicated by shading throughout the actogram in the left columns for each genotype) for 6 d followed by LL (as indicated by lack of shading throughout the actogram in the left columns for each genotype) for 4 d. Within each actogram, a given row shows two consecutive days of activity; the second such day is replotted in the left half of the next row down (thus, consecutive days of locomotion can be viewed both horizontally and vertically); heights of bars within a given actogram row reflect varying amounts of locomotion per half hour data collection bin. In the column next to the actograms, autocorrelation plots for these behavioral records are shown. The autocorrelation plot indicates rhythmicity and gives a measure of rhythm intensity (RI). In LL, the per L flies become arrhythmic after 1 d; per L; cryb remains rhythmic. The data were analyzed as described in Materials and Methods [46]. For details on autocorrelation, also see Material and Methods.

(B) Phase response to 37 °C heat pulse in per L and per L; cryb flies. Left and right panels show the phase changes observed at ZT18 and CT02, respectively. On the x-axis, the Zeitgeber 37 °C heat pulse is shown. Phase delays and advances are calculated as described in Figure 1A and are plotted on the y-axis (± SEM) as negative and positive values, respectively. For each genotype, an average phase shift from 15–32 flies is shown.

(C) Period in hours was calculated for cryb (circle and blue), per L (squares and red), or per L; cryb (diamonds and yellow) at 15 °C, 18 °C, 25 °C, and 29 °C in constant darkness. The average period is determined from three independent experiments. For each experiment, the average period was calculated using MESA for individual flies and then combined to obtain an average period length. For details on MESA, see Materials and Methods section. The average period (T) and SEM of the period (in hours) are as follows: 15 °C cryb (23, 0.5), 15 °C per L (24.8, 0.5), 15 °C per L; cryb (27.2, 0.5), 18 °C cryb (23.3, 0.58), 18 °C per L (26.9, 1.2), 18 °C per L; cryb (27.5, 0.6), 25 °C cryb (23.3, 0.3), 25 °C per L (29.02, 0.2), 25 °C per L; cryb (27.8, 0.7). 29 °C cryb (23.3, 0.5), 29 °C per L (31.7, 0.5), 29 °C per L; cryb (28, 0.5). Period lengths for the three genotypes at a given temperatures were found to be significantly different using analysis of variance (ANOVA) (p < 0.0001).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050146.g005

Finally, to establish a link between the exaggerated heat-mediated phase shifts and the temperature compensation defect of perL flies, we assayed the free-running period of perL; cryb double mutant flies at constant temperatures (Figure 5C). The results indicate that this genotype shows much less period change with temperature, in striking contrast to perL flies. This indicates that a temperature-sensitive CRY:PERL-TIM interaction underlies most of the perL loss of temperature compensation. It also connects the free-running period phenotype assayed at constant temperatures with the response to a heat pulse. Indeed, there is also a CRY:PERL-TIM interaction after incubation of perL flies at a constant temperature of 29 °C (Figure S3). Moreover, the fact that the perL strain has an altered period compared to the perL; cryb double mutant strain at 15 °C (Figure 5C) suggests that even at low temperatures, the PERL-TIM complex interacts with CRY. We suggest that advances predominate (an aggregate shorter period) at 15 °C, whereas delays predominate (an aggregate longer period) at temperatures ≥ 25 °C.

A Model

These data suggest that the perL missense mutation facilitates a PER-TIM conformational change (Figure 6A; 1 → 2). Heat facilitates the same change in wild-type PER-TIM, although higher temperatures are required and a smaller fraction of PER-TIM is affected. If CRY interacts predominantly with TIM, then the per mutation and heat must also help promote a TIM conformational change (Figure 6A; 3). We imagine that this altered PER-TIM conformation could also facilitate an interaction with active CRY, which is a conformational state similar to that promoted by illumination, i.e. by CRY photon capture (Figure 6A; CRY*). The key phase-shifting complex can then be promoted by increasing the concentration of either component, activated PER-TIM by temperature/mutation or activated CRY by light (Figure 6A; 2–3 or CRY*, respectively). Importantly, a temperature-sensitive PERL-TIM complex is consistent with a slightly longer average period (~1 h) of the perL; cryb strain at 29 °C relative to 15 °C (Figure 5C).

thumbnail
Figure 6. CRY Links Temperature and Light Responses

(A) Model picturing the role of CRY in heat-mediated phase shifts. See text for details. (B) Wild-type flies are more rhythmic in constant light at lower temperature. Behavioral LL cycles of wild-type flies were monitored. The data were collected at a constant temperature of 25 °C (lower panels) or 15 °C (top panels). Average activity plots are shown. For details on autocorrelation and the actogram, see Figure 5 legend. Adult male 1–3-d-old wild-type flies were entrained for 3 d (12 h: 12 h LD) followed by 6 d of LL as indicated on top of each column, either for 10 lux (25 °C, n = 26 flies and %R = 23; 15 °C, n = 26 and %R = 65) or 100 lux (25 °C, n = 27 and %R = 18; 15 °C, n = 29 and %R = 50). Only the LL data from days 2–6 are shown. n, number of flies analyzed; %R, percentage of rhythmic flies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050146.g006

Light and Temperature Can Act Together on Wild-Type Fly Rhythms

These observations suggest that even in wild-type flies, temperature and light can synergize to affect CRY:PER-TIM complex formation at physiologically normal temperatures. To test this hypothesis, we subjected Canton-S (CS) flies to constant illumination at 10 and 100 lux. Constant light even at low intensities render most flies arrhythmic at a standard incubation temperature of 25 °C (Figure 6B) [41], and constant light arrhythmicity requires CRY [15]. The results and model (Figure 6A) suggest that low temperatures might reduce complex formation and arrhythmicity, and constant light arrhythmicity has not been assayed at 15 °C. Indeed, we observed substantially larger numbers of arrhythmic flies at 25 °C than at 15 °C, at 100 as well as at 10 lux of constant light (Figure 6B). We interpret the result to indicate more CRY:PER-TIM complex formation at 25 °C than at 15 °C, indicating that light and temperature can act together in wild-type flies at physiologically relevant temperatures. The convergence induces phase shifts as well as causes arrhythmicity in constant light. We speculate that it also serves to fine tune the dawn and dusk response of the clock when light and temperature increase and decrease together.

Discussion

We show here that the photoreceptor CRY and its interaction with the PER-TIM complex is critical for heat shock–mediated phase shifts as well as for the loss of temperature compensation in the perL mutant strain. Heat-induced phase delays take place in a wild-type strain, and they are severely reduced in the cry loss-of-function mutant cryb. Moreover, there is a physical interaction between CRY and PER-TIM at circadian times that correspond to phase delays. More robust heat-mediated phase delays and even phase advances occur in perL mutant flies. The perL behavioral results are mirrored by CRY:PERL-TIM interactions, which occur in the advance zone and also in response to 30 °C temperature pulses. perL phase shifts like wild-type phase shifts are severely reduced by the addition of cryb to the perL background. These perL; cryb double mutant flies also have largely restored temperature compensation, indicating that an interaction between CRY and PER-TIM is responsible for the loss of temperature compensation in the perL strain as well as for heat-mediated phase shifts of wild-type as well as perL flies. The similarity between heat-mediated and light-mediated phase shifts suggests that light and temperature can synergize to cause phase shifts, and an experiment in wild-type flies supports this notion.

The temperature-induced complex formation between CRY and PER-TIM parallels the substantial evidence that a similar interaction is critical for light-mediated phase shifts. Biochemical as well as genetic data indicate that complex formation between light-activated CRY and TIM, or between light-activated CRY and PER-TIM, leads to TIM degradation, which is believed to advance or delay the clock (e.g., [25]). Although some data indicate a physical interaction between CRY and PER, most observations indicate that physical contact is predominantly between CRY and TIM; for example, PER usually requires the presence of TIM to interact with CRY, but a TIM:CRY interaction can take place without PER (e.g., [20]). Because much of TIM is in complex with PER, especially in the early night [22], a CRY-TIM interaction is effectively a CRY:PER-TIM interaction. All of this begins with CRY photon capture, which activates CRY by causing a conformational change and a subsequent interaction with PER-TIM. Indeed, experimental studies on Drosophila CRY as well on other related proteins provide a coherent view of a CRY-centric light-initiation event [10].

Although a connection between light pulse– and heat pulse–initiated interactions appeared enigmatic, previous studies in wild-type flies suggested that heat phase shifts are like light pulses and are due to posttranscriptional events that influence PER and/or TIM [36]. The failure to elicit a phase shift with a 34 °C pulse (Figure 1) indicates that a heat shock may be required [43]. This is accompanied by numerous changes in cell physiology and gene expression, which could perturb the dynamics of an oscillatory system [44]. However, perL flies show robust phase shifts and CRY:PER-TIM complex formation after a 30 °C heat pulse, making it unlikely that a heat-shock response is generally required for heat pulse–mediated phase shifts in Drosophila. Extrapolation to wild-type flies makes two assumptions: (i) perL flies do not have an unprecedented heat-shock response triggered at much lower temperatures and (ii) the failure to observe 30 °C behavioral phase shifts and biochemical interactions in wild-type reflects quantitative rather than a qualitative differences between 30 °C and 37 °C and between wild-type and perL genotypes. Indeed, the convergence of light and temperature on wild-type fly behavior at physiological temperatures (Figure 6B) suggests that these CRY:PER-TIM interactions are normally difficult to detect at lower temperatures, because they are quantitatively minor.

The perL behavioral and biochemical results indicate that the missense mutation causes a large increase in the fraction of PERL-TIM interacting with CRY at normal temperatures (Figure 4B). This suggests that the PERL-TIM structure is temperature sensitive (Figure 6A), an interpretation consistent with the period of the perL; cryb double mutant strain being somewhat temperature sensitive (Figure 5C; see below). Moreover, this strain has a substantially longer period than the perL single mutant strain at 15 °C (Figure 5C), suggesting that PERL-TIM manifests an enhanced interaction with CRY at all physiologically relevant temperatures. These experiments cannot definitively rule out CRY as the temperature-sensitive component; in this case, the perL mutation would only cause an increased interaction between PER-TIM and CRY. In either case, the close correspondence between the 37 °C perL heat and light PRCs (Figure 4A) indicates that the CRY photocycle is inessential for CRY:PER-TIM interactions and behavioral phase shifts in Drosophila. We speculate that heat activation of PER-TIM causes the same CRY conformational change as does light—albeit indirectly (Figure 6A).

The heat-induced interactions between PERL-TIM and CRY as well as the perL; cryb phenotype make a strong link between the circadian response to temperature pulses and incubations at constant temperatures, analogous to nonparametric and parametric light entrainment, respectively. This is because a persistent CRY:PERL-TIM interaction affects the perL period like the enhanced phase-shift response of perL to a heat pulse. This recalls the hypersensitivity of perL to incubation at constant low light intensities, which lengthen the perL period more severely and at lower intensities than is required to lengthen wild-type periods [41]. Our results explain this observation and suggest that the more CRY-interactive PERL-TIM requires less CRY light activation than does wild-type PER-TIM. Moreover, the similarities between light and heat inspired the experiment suggesting that light and temperature function together, even on wild-type flies (Figure 6B). This synergy might fine-tune the dawn and dusk response of the clock and even contribute to seasonal adaptation of clock function [45].

The circadian problem of temperature compensation has gained little traction since the discovery more than 15 y ago that the per missense mutants manifest aberrant temperature compensation [41]. Our results here suggest that the timSL allele suppresses the temperature compensation defect of perL by failing to interact with CRY [42]. The observations suggest that the same PERL-TIM structure that facilitates a CRY interaction in response to a phase-shifting perturbation (heat- or light-mediated CRY activation) keeps time in a temperature-sensitive manner under constant conditions. Characterization of this altered PERL-TIM structure is an important goal for the near future.

Materials and Methods

Drosophila genetics.

Wild-type CS, perL, and cryb flies were used for average activity and phase response analyses (see below) and as controls for the locomotor activity analyses. The perL mutation was combined with cryb to generate perL; cryb flies. The pdf-GAL4 and UAS-cry transgenic flies have been described previously [13]. The y w; tim-GAL4 UAS-myccry/CyO line (TMC) was previously described [20]. The TMC transgenes were introduced into perL to obtain perL; tim-GAL4 UAS-myccry (abbreviated as perL; TMC). The UAS-cry and pdf-GAL4 transgenes were introduced in cryb backgrounds to produce y w; pdf-GAL4/UAS-cry; cryb flies.

Phase shift protocol and behavioral analysis.

In all experiments unless stated otherwise, CS males were collected at 1–3 d old and reared in LD 12:12 at 25 °C for 3 d. In the APRC protocol, flies were given a 10-min saturating white light pulse (2000 lux) during the third dark phase of the cycle, at the indicated times during the night and the following subjective day. A separate control group of flies was not given a pulse. Flies were then put into constant darkness for another 5 d. For the heat pulse PRCs, flies were placed in activity monitors in LD 12:12 at 25 °C for 3 d. During the third dark phase of the cycle, one monitor of untreated flies was retained as a control. For the heat treatment, behavior tubes containing flies were removed from the monitors, held upright, and an elastic band placed around them to hold them tightly together. The entire package was then placed in a 50-ml conical tube, so that the tubes would stay upright and be in a water-tight environment but small enough for efficient heat transfer from the water bath to the tubes. The top of the activity tubes were always an inch below the top of the 50-ml conical tube, so the water level would be above the tubes. Incubation was in the water bath for 30 min at 37 °C. The 50-ml tube was then removed, and the behavior tubes placed back in the monitors. Each tube had been marked on the top with a number and then placed back in the same monitor channel. A second control set of flies was handled identically except that they were just kept upright (with the elastic band) in 50-ml tubes in the incubator but not placed in a water bath. In all cases, the experiments were repeated at least twice with essentially identical results. For each genotype an average phase shift from 15–32 flies is shown. Locomotor activities of individual flies were monitored using Trikinetics Drosophila activity monitors (TriKinetics Inc, Waltham, Massachusetts, United States). The analysis was done with a signal processing toolbox implemented in MATLAB (Mathworks; http://www.mathworks.com) as described [46].

Autocorrelation it is a measure of how well a signal matches a time-shifted version of itself as a function of the amount of time shift. In our analysis, autocorrelation and spectral analysis were used to assess rhythmicity and to estimate period. The phase information was obtained with circular statistics [46]. The column in Figure 5A labeled autocorrelation shows correlograms for the data. Correlation coefficients are plotted on the ordinate with a range of values from −1 to 1. The gray region centered around 0 describes a 95% confidence interval. The lag of the autocorrelation function is plotted on the abscissa. An asterisk is placed above the third peak of the autocorrelation function. The value at that point defines the rhythmicity index (RI), an estimate of the strength of rhythmicity. When the asterisk is not present, the autocorrelation function indicates a lack of rhythmicity. Values for the RI appear in the lower left corner of these plots along with a related number called the rhythmicity statistic (RS). The RS value is the ratio of the RI to the absolute value of the confidence line. This metric indicates that the rhythmicity described by the correlogram is statistically significant when the value is ≥ 1 [46] .

The MESA analysis is a spectral analysis of the data that provides an estimate of period. Spectral density is given in arbitrary units on the ordinate, and the range of assessed periods is shown on the abscissa. Asterisks are placed over the highest peak shown in a range between 18–30 h. Although this value is generally taken as the estimate of circadian period, there may be other periodicities present within the horizontal range (the width) of the peak or elsewhere on the plot, and these additional rhythmic components are also present in the data. Absence of an asterisk indicates either the absence of a peak or that a peak within the plot occurs outside the circadian range. Note that the autocorrelation plot is used to determine rhythmicity, and mesa is used to provide an estimate of the period only when warranted by correlogram [46].

Immunoprecipitation.

About 250 adult flies were entrained to a 12-h-light: 12-h-dark cycle for 3 d. At ZT15, ZT21, or CT02, they were pulsed with bright white light for 15 min and 30 °C or 37 °C for 30 min before being collected and frozen. Head extracts were prepared and homogenized in Extraction Buffer (20 mM Hepes, pH 7.5, 100 mM KCl, 1mM Dithiothreitol, 5% glycerol, 0.05% Nonidet P40, 1× Complete Protease Inhibitor [Roche; http://www.roche.com]). Protein G sepharose fast flow beads (Amersham; http://www.amersham.com) were coated with anti-MYC antibody (2 μl; Santa Cruz Biotechnologies; clone 9E10; http://www.scbt.com] plus 20 μl beads/sample) for 1 h. The beads were then washed twice and incubated with the head extracts for 4 h at 4 °C. Pulled-down beads were washed four times with 750 μl extraction buffer before being resuspended in 40 μl 1× SDS loading buffer for Western blot analysis. Head homogenization, incubation, and immunoprecipitation for the light-pulsed samples were done under normal laboratory lighting, whereas the nonpulsed and heat-pulsed samples were processed under red light (700 nm) and incubated in the dark.

Protein extracts and Western blots.

Fly heads extracts were prepared and Western blots were performed as described [22]. Equal loading and quality of protein transfer were first verified by Ponceau Red staining and then by the intensity of cross-reacting bands on the Western blots, or by reprobing the membrane with a monoclonal α-tubulin antibody (clone DM1A, Sigma, 1:1000 dilution; http://www.sigmaaldrich.com). The anti-CRY rabbit antibody was used at 1:500 dilution [47]. The anti-PER antibody is previously described and used at 1:1500 dilution, whereas the anti-TIM antibody was made in rat and used at 1:3000 dilution [22].

Supporting Information

Figure S1. 37 °C Heat Pulses Result in Robust Phase Shifts of Wild-Type and perL Flies but Not of cryb or perL; cryb Flies

(A) Circular analysis figures of locomotor behavior in wild-type CS and cryb flies after a 37 °C heat pulse (HP). (B) The same circular analyses for perL and perL; cryb after a 37 °C HP. On these plots, time moves forward in a counter-clockwise direction. The behavioral phase estimates for each rhythmic specimen are plotted just outside the unit circle and a mean vector summarizes the phase of the group. The direction of the vector indicates the behavioral phase, whereas its length reflects the dispersion (variability) of the individual estimates (see [46] for more details). The Rayleigh's test was used to determine whether each vector is significantly different (p < 0.05) from the null vector (random distribution). Then, the Watson-Williams-Stevens test was used to obtain an F-statistic that determined whether the two vectors obtained from the nonpulsed control and the experimental group of flies are significantly different, (p < 0.01) Statistically significant differences were found for CS flies at ZT12 and ZT15 and for perL at ZT18 and CT2. cryb and perL; cryb flies did not significantly shift their phase at any time points. For the estimates of the phase differences see Figure 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050146.sg001

(5.0 MB TIF)

Figure S2. Functional CRY Is Required for HP-Mediated Phase Delays

Top and bottom panels show the phase changes observed at ZT15 (A) and ZT21(B), respectively. On the x-axis, the zeitgeber 37 °C HP or light pulse (LP) is indicated. Phase delays and advances are described in Figure 1A and plotted on the y-axis (± SEM) as negative and positive values, respectively. The genotype of the flies is indicated on the x-axis: the first row shows the transgenes present (a plus sign corresponds to a chromosome without a transgene), whereas the second row indicates the genetic background (wild-type [WT] or cryb).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050146.sg002

(1.2 MB TIF)

Figure S3. CRY Forms a Complex with PERL/TIM at a Constant Temperature of 29 °C but Not with Wild-Type PER/TIM

(A) TMC flies (MYCCRY); per+ (PER+), and TMC flies (Myc-CRY); perL (PER-L) were subjected to standard 12:12 light:dark conditions at 29 °C. Samples were collected at either ZT15 (lane 1) or ZT21(lane 2) for PER+ flies and at ZT18 (lane 3) and CT02 (lane 4) for PER-L and frozen. Head extracts (HE) were immunoprecipitated with antibody to MYC (IP), all as previously described [20]. CRY, PER, and TIM levels were measured by Western blotting. These heat-dependent interactions among CRY, TIM, and PER were measured twice with similar results.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050146.sg003

(2.6 MB TIF)

Acknowledgments

We thank Jose Agosto for help with the MATLAB analysis; Dan Stoleru, Emi Nagoshi, Jerome Menet, Jose Agosto, Rebecca Schoer, and Kavita Babu for inspiration and helpful discussions; and Ravi Allada for critical readings of the manuscript. We also thank Heather Felton, Krissy Palm, and Shawn Jennings for administrative assistance.

Author Contributions

RK, PN, and MR conceived and designed the experiments. RK and PN performed the experiments. AB contributed to experiments in Figure 6. RK, PN and MR analyzed the data. RK, PN, AM, PE, and MR contributed reagents /materials /analysis tools. RK and MR wrote the paper.

References

  1. 1. Dunlap JC, Loros JJ, Liu Y, Crosthwaite SK (1999) Eukaryotic circadian systems: Cycles in common. Genes Cells 4: 1–10.
  2. 2. Allada R, Emery P, Takahashi JS, Rosbash M (2001) Stopping time: The genetics of fly and mouse circadian clocks. Annu Rev Neurosci 24: 1091–1119.
  3. 3. Nawathean P, Rosbash M (2004) The doubletime and CKII kinases collaborate to potentiate Drosophila PER transcriptional repressor activity. Mol Cell 13: 213–223.
  4. 4. Dunlap JC (2004) Kinases and circadian clocks: per goes it alone. Dev Cell 6: 160–161.
  5. 5. Sathyanarayanan S, Zheng X, Xiao R, Sehgal A (2004) Posttranslational regulation of Drosophila PERIOD protein by protein phosphatase 2A. Cell 116: 603–615.
  6. 6. Yu W, Zheng H, Houl JH, Dauwalder B, Hardin PE (2006) PER-dependent rhythms in CLK phosphorylation and E-box binding regulate circadian transcription. Genes Dev 20: 723–733.
  7. 7. Kim EY, Edery I (2006) Balance between DBT/CKIepsilon kinase and protein phosphatase activities regulate phosphorylation and stability of Drosophila CLOCK protein. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103: 6178–6183.
  8. 8. Stanewsky R (2002) Clock mechanisms in Drosophila. Cell Tissue Res 309: 11–26.
  9. 9. Cashmore AR (2003) Cryptochromes: Enabling plants and animals to determine circadian time. Cell 114: 537–543.
  10. 10. Partch CL, Sancar A (2005) Photochemistry and photobiology of cryptochrome blue-light photopigments: The search for a photocycle. Photochem Photobiol 81: 1291–1304.
  11. 11. Klarsfeld A, Malpel S, Michard-Vanhee C, Picot M, Chelot E, et al. (2004) Novel features of cryptochrome-mediated photoreception in the brain circadian clock of Drosophila. J Neurosci 24: 1468–1477.
  12. 12. Egan ES, Franklin TM, Hilderbrand-Chae MJ, McNeil GP, Roberts MA, et al. (1999) An extraretinally expressed insect cryptochrome with similarity to the blue light photoreceptors of mammals and plants. J Neurosci 19: 3665–3673.
  13. 13. Emery P, Stanewsky R, Helfrich-Förster C, Emery-Le M, Hall JC, et al. (2000) Drosophila CRY is a deep brain circadian photoreceptor. Neuron 26: 493–504.
  14. 14. Stanewsky R, Kaneko M, Emery P, Beretta M, Wager-Smith K, et al. (1998) The cryb mutation identifies cryptochrome as a circadian photoreceptor in Drosophila. Cell 95: 681–692.
  15. 15. Emery P, Stanewsky R, Hall JC, Rosbash M (2000) A unique circadian-rhythm photoreceptor. Nature 404: 456–457.
  16. 16. Helfrich-Forster C, Winter C, Hofbauer A, Hall JC, Stanewsky R (2001) The circadian clock of fruit flies is blind after elimination of all known photoreceptors. Neuron 30: 249–261.
  17. 17. Ceriani MF, Darlington TK, Staknis D, Mas P, Petti AA, et al. (1999) Light-dependent sequestration of TIMELESS by CRYPTOCHROME. Science 285: 553–556.
  18. 18. Rosato E, Codd V, Mazzotta G, Piccin A, Zordan M, et al. (2001) Light-dependent interaction between Drosophila CRY and the clock protein PER mediated by the carboxy terminus of CRY. Curr Biol 11: 909–917.
  19. 19. Lin FJ, Song W, Meyer-Bernstein E, Naidoo N, Sehgal A (2001) Photic signaling by cryptochrome in the Drosophila circadian system. Mol Cell Biol 21: 7287–7294.
  20. 20. Busza A, Emery-Le M, Rosbash M, Emery P (2004) Roles of the two Drosophila CRYPTOCHROME structural domains in circadian photoreception. Science 304: 1503–1506.
  21. 21. Hunter-Ensor M, Ousley A, Sehgal A (1996) Regulation of the Drosophila protein timeless suggests a mechanism for resetting the circadian clock by light. Cell 84: 677–685.
  22. 22. Zeng H, Qian Z, Myers MP, Rosbash M (1996) A light-entrainment mechanism for the Drosophila circadian clock. Nature 380: 129–135.
  23. 23. Lee C, Parikh V, Itsukaichi T, Bae K, Edery I (1996) Resetting the Drosophila clock by photic regulation of PER and a PER-TIM complex. Science 271: 1740–1744.
  24. 24. Myers MP, Wager-Smith K, Rothenfluh-Hilfiker A, Young MW (1996) Light-induced degradation of TIMELESS and entrainment of the Drosophila circadian clock. Science 271: 1736–1740.
  25. 25. Koh K, Zheng X, Sehgal A (2006) JETLAG resets the Drosophila circadian clock by promoting light-induced degradation of TIMELESS. Science 312: 1809–1812.
  26. 26. Pittendrigh CS (1960) Circadian rhythms and the circadian organization of living systems. Cold Spring Harbor Symp Quant Biol 25: 159–182.
  27. 27. Lahiri K, Vallone D, Gondi SB, Santoriello C, Dickmeis T, et al. (2005) Temperature regulates transcription in the zebrafish circadian clock. PLoS Biol 3(11): e351..
  28. 28. Underwood H, Calaban M (1987) Pineal melatonin rhythms in the lizard Anolis carolinensis: I. Response to light and temperature cycles. J Biol Rhythms 2: 179–193.
  29. 29. Francis AJ, Coleman GJ (1988) The effect of ambient temperature cycles upon circadian running and drinking activity in male and female laboratory rats. Physiol Behav 43: 471–477.
  30. 30. Yoshii T, Heshiki Y, Ibuki-Ishibashi T, Matsumoto A, Tanimura T, et al. (2005) Temperature cycles drive Drosophila circadian oscillation in constant light that otherwise induces behavioural arrhythmicity. Eur J Neurosci 22: 1176–1184.
  31. 31. Zimmerman WF, Pittendrigh CS, Pavlidis T (1968) Temperature compensation of the circadian oscillation in Drosophila pseudoobscura and its entrainment by temperature cycles. J Insect Physiol 14: 669–684.
  32. 32. Aschoff J (1965) Response curves in circadian periodicity. In: Aschoff J, editor. Circadian clocks. Amsterdam: North Holland. pp. 94–111.
  33. 33. Levine JD, Casey CI, Kalderon DD, Jackson FR (1994) Altered circadian pacemaker functions and cyclic AMP rhythms in the Drosophila learning mutant dunce. Neuron 13: 967–974.
  34. 34. Rutila JE, Maltseva O, Rosbash M (1998) The timSL mutant affects a restricted portion of the Drosophila melanogaster circadian cycle. J Biol Rhythm 13: 380–392.
  35. 35. Edery I, Rutila JE, Rosbash M (1994) Phase shifting of the circadian clock by induction of the Drosophila period protein. Science 263: 237–240.
  36. 36. Sidote D, Majercak J, Parikh V, Edery I (1998) Differential effects of light and heat on the Drosophila circadian clock proteins PER and TIM. Mol Cell Biol 18: 2004–2013.
  37. 37. Suri V, Qian Z, Hall JC, Rosbash M (1998) Evidence that the TIM light response is relevant to light-induced phase shifts in Drosophila melanogaster. Neuron 21: 225–234.
  38. 38. Yang Z, Emerson M, Su HS, Sehgal A (1998) Response of the timeless protein to light correlates with behavioral entrainment and suggests a nonvisual pathway for circadian photoreception. Neuron 21: 215–223.
  39. 39. Renn SC, Park JH, Rosbash M, Hall JC, Taghert PH (1999) A pdf neuropeptide gene mutation and ablation of PDF neurons each cause severe abnormalities of behavioral circadian rhythms in Drosophila. Cell 99: 791–802.
  40. 40. Kaneko M, Hall JC (2000) Neuroanatomy of cells expressing clock genes in Drosophila: Transgenic manipulation of the period and timeless genes to mark the perikarya of circadian pacemaker neurons and their projections. J Compar Neuro 422: 66–94.
  41. 41. Konopka RJ, Pittendrigh C, Orr D (1989) Reciprocal behavior associated with altered homeostasis and photosensitivity of Drosophila clock mutants. J Neurogenet 6: 1–10.
  42. 42. Rutila JE, Zeng H, Le M, Curtin KD, Hall JC, et al. (1996) The timSL mutant of the Drosophila rhythm gene timeless manifests allele-specific interactions with period gene mutants. Neuron 17: 921–929.
  43. 43. Lindquist S (1986) The heat-shock response. Annu Rev Biochem 55: 1151–1191.
  44. 44. Rensing L, Bos A, Kroeger J, Cornelius G (1987) Possible link between circadian rhythm and heat shock response in Neurospora crassa. Chronobiol Int 4: 543–549.
  45. 45. Stoleru D, Nawathean P, Fernandez Mde L, Menet JS, Ceriani MF, et al. (2007) The Drosophila circadian network is a seasonal timer. Cell 129: 207–219.
  46. 46. Levine JD, Funes P, Dowse HB, Hall JC (2002) Signal analysis of behavioral and molecular cycles. BMC Neurosci 3: 1.
  47. 47. Rush BL, Murad A, Emery P, Giebultowicz JM (2006) Ectopic CRYPTOCHROME renders TIM light sensitive in the Drosophila ovary. J Biol Rhythms 21: 272–278.